Free Essay

Psychology

In:

Submitted By wenciejane
Words 20526
Pages 83
Pearson, P. D., & Cervetti, G. N. (2013). The psychology and pedagogy of reading processes. In W. Reynolds, & G. Miller, (Eds.),
Educational Psychology, V.VII, of Handbook of Psychology (2nd Ed) (pp. 507-554). New York: John Wiley & Sons.

Chapter 12
The Psychology and Pedagogy of Reading Processes
P. David Pearson and Gina Cervetti
As we approach the monumental task of living up to the standard imposed by our predecessor, the late Michael Pressley, in writing the reading chapter for this, the seventh volume in the series of Handbooks of Psychology, we are both privileged and humbled by the opportunity of continuing the legacy of providing a comprehensive account of new theoretical and empirical contributions to reading research. Respectful of the cross-age approach that Pressley took in the last volume (account for progress of beginning readers, adolescent and adult readers— and along the way highlight some pedagogical processes that are salient at all levels, such as word recognition, vocabulary, and comprehension), we took a different approach.
We decided to focus on reading as a fundamentally cognitive process that can be influenced by contextual forces at many levels, most notably for education, schools, and policy environments. Thus we deal with the fundamental psychological aspects of reading—word level processes (including subword processes such as phonological awareness and decoding, word reading, and vocabulary, with all of its entailments), and text-level processes as they are grounded in structures, genres, and disciplinary knowledge pursuits. After the account of these cognitive processes, we turn to a setting-level analysis, in which we examine word- and text level processes within schooling (including instruction in English language arts and the subject matters of history and science) and policy contexts. As we unpack each element in our review, our goal is to answer the question: How has what we have learned in the last decade advanced the knowledge base available to us? As we move to setting level analysis, we meet head on practices that have emerged less to understand and more to improve the acquisition of those processes among students in our schools (though not always with positive effects!). We end intentionally with what might be considered an anomaly in the Handbook of Educational Psychology—a section on the policy context in which debates about the science of reading, especially reading pedagogy, occur. Reading has, for better or worse, always been contested ground.

508

And even the very act of reading—whether for gist, enjoyment, or critique—is never free of ideology.
Our method for locating research relevant to our charge was to rely first on highly regarded syntheses and analyses of the research base, most notably in our case (a) the 2006 handbook, The Science of Reading (Snowling & Hulme, 2005),
(b) Volume IV of the Handbook of Reading Research (Kamil, Pearson, Moje, &
Afflerbach, 2011), and (c) seminal reviews, including meta-analyses, appearing in national initiatives (e.g., Preventing Reading Difficulties and the National Reading
Panel) and in other outlets. From there we worked our way back to individual research articles that were important in their own right and/or typical of a large class of studies. And, in areas in which we were ourselves work, we relied on our professional knowledge of the most important reviews and research studies.
A review such as this, in which we try to capture in a handful of pages what has taken others a full tome to unpack, is necessarily selective. We could not hope to convey either the breadth or depth of scholarship of the field, not even the past decade. So we apologize in advance to all of our colleagues whose work we did not cite and all users whose favorite topics are omitted. All we can hope for is that we have chosen, in our selection process, important and relevant (if not the most important and the most relevant) topics to guide readers who want to know what matters most in the psychological foundations of basic processes and instructional practices in reading.
One final introductory comment: We come close, in the chapter title, to plagiarizing another of our heroes, Edmund Burke Huey in the title of his landmark 1908 book, The Psychology and Pedagogy of Reading. The similarity is intentional. Huey was a remarkable scholar who reflected both the issues and understandings of the day and anticipated phenomena and insights that would not appear in the research until five decades after his career had ended. That we could achieve either of those goals for these times—reflecting the present and anticipating the future—would please us enormously. But even if we cannot achieve either of those goals, at least we have our “titled” brush with history.

Examining Basic Reading Processes
For our purposes, basic processes include those processes that enable to us perceive, pronounce, and understand words and those that enable us to build models of meaning for—and use information and insights from—sentences, paragraphs, and entire passages of text.

509

Word Level Processes
Word level processes are defined as those entailed in word recognition, either as component or prerequisite skills. Specifically, we discuss word recognition and its acquisition, phonological awareness, and vocabulary.

Expert Word Recognition
Over the past 40 years, we have learned a great deal about the complex nature of word recognition among skilled readers, in particular about the manner in which recognition is conditioned by a range of lexical and semantic structures— some promoting bottom-up and others top-down processing—that interact with one another in the word recognition process (see Lupker, 2005). These advances notwithstanding, a central (perhaps the central) debate in word recognition is whether the pathway from the orthographic representation in print to lexical representation in memory is mediated by a phonological representation prior to recognition. The data (see Van Orden & Kloos, 2005, for a systematic review) are, at best, ambiguous. Some evidence points to phonological mediation; for example, the categorization of a homophone like brake as part of a car, an obvious clue for break, occurs with some frequency. Other evidence points to a direct access from print to lexical representation; for example, semantic categorization errors occur often for low frequency homophones, like peek, but rarely for high frequency homophones like break, implying that with greater exposure, access even for ambiguous words is direct and automatic. These sorts of ambiguous findings have led many scholars to posit various versions of a dual route model (Coltheart, 2005;
Coltheart, Rastle, Perry, Langdon, & Ziegler, 2001; Davis, 2010). Dual route models posit that readers go directly from print to lexical representation when words are highly familiar and unambiguous, but interpose a phonological representation on the way from print to lexical representation when words are unfamiliar and opaque. The movement in modern theories of expert word recognition is toward highly contextualized models of word recognition, models in which feedback between graphemic, phonemic, and lexical levels of analysis, implying dual if not more routes to meaning, are the order of the day (Van Orden
& Kloos).

Acquiring Word Reading Skills
There are numerous accounts of the ways in which students develop as word readers, most of them organized into “stages” or “phases” in which certain approaches to reading words are statistically dominant over others (see Ehri,
2005b, for a thorough comparison of the various stage theories). We will avoid

510

entering into the debate on the precise boundaries between stages, opting instead for “general” dispositions toward word reading that seem to hold across stages.
In order to read the words that appear before them in text, readers have several choices (Ehri, 2005a). They can read a word by decoding—converting the constituent letters into sounds, blending the sounds together, and pronouncing the word. They can read a word by memory—calling up a trace of its form and pronunciation from memory, otherwise known as sight word reading. They can read a word by analogy—inferring its pronunciation because of its similarity to a known word (e.g., brother is like mother except at the beginning). Finally, they can predict its pronunciation by relying on contextual features such as clues in the text (this must be “bark” because the text mentions a dog) or pictures, at least in the earliest stories.
Students go through a predictable set of phases in their word reading repertoire—from pre-alphabetic reading (recognizing monkey by the tail) to partially alphabetic (using salient clues such as initial letters or word families) to full alphabetic (sequentially decoding letters into sounds) to consolidated alphabetic (dynamically orchestrating all four word reading strategies). The progression is from single to multiple approaches, where students are increasingly empowered to use all four of these word-reading processes. Furthermore, they use the processes in synergistic and complementary ways. For example, once they reach the full alphabetic stage, they can decode words readily. Once decoded, they can transfer the visual and auditory traces of a word to memory so that, after a few successful exposures, the word enters their sight word repertoire. Note that this repertoire is not limited to irregularly spelled words that must be recognized as units, but it consists of all of the words, including decodable words, that are immediately apprehended as units, without the need for arduous analysis. This
“self-teaching” mechanism (see Share, 1995) is crucial in early reading development because the more words that readers can move into their sight word repertoire, the more cognitive energy they can allocate to the really challenging tasks of reading, such as inferring the meanings of obscure words, text comprehension. A similar phenomenon happens with reading by analogy, usually in the latter part of grade one to early grade two. At this fairly advanced point in the development of their phonological recoding repertoire, readers are chunking letters into groups, such as prefixes (inter-, pre-, post-), suffixes (-est, -tion), and word families (-at, -eet, - ough). Once they have those chunks under control, they can recognize the chunks as units and transform even longer and more complex words into immediately recognizable sight words. Again, evidence of the selfteaching mechanism at work.

511

Phonological Awareness
A consistent finding in early reading development research, both correlational and experimental, of the past 25 years is that attention to the patterns of sounds that operate at the subword level matter (Adams, 1990; National Early
Literacy Panel, 2008; National Institute of Child Health and Human Development,
2000; Snow, Burns, & Griffith, 1998). Whether we define it as awareness of the words that make up a compound (sword + fish = swordfish), the syllables within a word (tay + buhl = table), the onset-rime structure of monosyllables (buh + ad = bad), or the phonemic components of a word (buh + ah + duh = bad), phonological awareness both predicts and improves later reading achievement.
Various measures of phonological awareness are strong predictors of later reading achievement, at least through grade one and into grade two. With respect to early indicators of later success, the NELP (2008), in an extensive metaanalysis, identified 11 variables that have proven to be moderate to strong predictors of later literacy proficiency. Six of these variables, the panel concluded, served as the “best” (i.e., strongest and most consistent) predictors. Of these six, two—alphabet knowledge (which the panel defined to include letter-sound as well as letter-name correspondences) and phonological awareness—proved to be the best of the best. This combination appears to be quite durable, having been reported as part of federal initiatives for more than 40 years, beginning with the
First-Grade Studies (Bond & Dykstra, 1967), and even earlier in the work of
Durrell and Murphy (1953) and extending into the 1990s and early 2000s (Adams,
1990; NICHD, 2000; Snow et al., 1998). Interestingly, an equally important finding in the predictive research is that subword factors like alphabet knowledge and phonemic awareness predict achievement for the early stages of learning to read (Grade 1 into 2), but it is early measures of language and vocabulary that predict achievement beyond the early stages (Snow et al, 1998). A more important, and equally consistent, finding about subword level factors is that, when they are taught systematically in the early stages of learning to read, they lead to an advantage over other sorts of instruction in overall reading achievement, particularly on word reading tasks (Adams, 1990; Ehri et al., 2001); NELP, 2008;
NICHD, 2000; Snow et al., 1998)—but that is a matter for a later section of this review. Several meta-analyses looking both at the impact of phonological awareness (PA) and its relationship to other early reading indicators have been published in the past decade, and they lead to somewhat different conclusions than those conducted in the previous decade. For example, Swanson, Trainin,
Necoechea, and Hammill (2003) examined the relationships among PA, rapid naming (of colors, pictures, letters, numbers or words), and word reading. Looking across 35 studies, they found that phonemic awareness was no better at predicting later word reading than other variables, such as pseudoword reading, IQ,

512

vocabulary, orthography, spelling, or memory. One result of these later syntheses has been to cast doubt on the preeminence of phonological awareness as a predictor of reading. These studies position phonological processing as only one contributor to word reading skill, and several recent analysis have pointed, in particular, to the contribution of orthographic processing (Badian, 2001;
Blaiklock, 2004; Hagiliassis, Pratt, & Johnston, 2006). Cunningham, Nathan, and
Raher (2010) point out that, while phonological processing accounts for significant variance in word recognition ability, there is variance left unaccounted for, which may be attributable to orthographic processing. That additional explained variance may help account for why some children who have adequate phonological awareness fall behind in their word recognition skills. We are left with the conclusion that has characterized most reviews of phonological processing and reading—that it is a necessary but not a sufficient condition for reading success.

Vocabulary
We had difficulty deciding whether we should consider vocabulary acquisition as a “high-level” word level process or a “low-level” text level process. Vocabulary is, at least at a surface level, all about words and their meanings, but words, from the perspective of meaning, are only incidentally about words; they are better thought of as fundamentally conceptual entities. Thus we could just as easily located this review as the first part of the text level processes section. This progress we report notwithstanding, we still have not unambiguously settled the question of why vocabulary and comprehension are so closely related, a question unpacked by Anderson and Freebody in 1981. Does learning new word meanings cause comprehension (what Anderson and Freebody label the instrumentalist hypothesis)? Or is vocabulary knowledge an alias for some other factor that is the real cause of comprehension—either a store of important conceptual information about the world and the various disciplines (the knowledge hypothesis) or general verbal ability (the aptitude hypothesis).
One thing is certain: The decades preceding the new millennium brought considerable research that both underscored the significance of vocabulary knowledge for success in reading—and in school more generally—and that established fundamental understandings about how vocabulary words are learned.
It is well-established, for example, that vocabulary knowledge is multidimensional and incrementally acquired through repeated exposure. That is, to know a word is to know more than its definition, and knowledge of a word’s definition alone is not sufficient to enhance reading comprehension (Stahl & Fairbanks, 1986). When children are exposed to words in different contexts through repeated encounters, each encounter provides new information about the word, such as contexts-of-use

513

and aspects of the word’s meaning (S. Stahl & K. Stahl, 2004). Exposures over time and in varied contexts, then, allow for refinement and differentiation in word knowledge. In addition, repeated encounters seem to ensure that words are known well enough to be accessed quickly during reading. McKeown, Beck, Omanson, and Pople (1985) famously found that 12 encounters with a word reliably improved understanding, but four encounters did not.
It is also well understood that active interaction with words enhances vocabulary acquisition. Nagy (1988) synthesized research suggesting that meaningful processing of words is an important factor in learning new words, a finding that was later affirmed by the National Reading Panel (National Institute of Child Health & Human Development, 2000). Not surprisingly, then, studies have documented that, while many words are acquired incidentally through extensive and wide reading (e.g., Nagy, Anderson, & Herman, 1987; Nagy,
Herman, & Anderson, 1985), instruction of word meanings produces stronger word learning than encounters with words through reading alone (e.g., Beck,
Perfetti, & McKeown, 1982; Paribakht & Wesche, 1997; Stahl & Fairbanks,
1986).
By the turn of the century, research had also established the significance of vocabulary knowledge for comprehension (Beck & McKeown, 1990; Cunningham
& Stanovich, 1997) and had documented significant discrepancies in vocabulary knowledge between high- and low-socio-economic status (SES) students (Hart &
Risley, 1995).
In spite of all that we now know about vocabulary acquisition, the vexing issue of the volume of words to be taught continues to be the biggest dilemma in the instruction of vocabulary. Nearly three decades ago, Nagy and Anderson
(1984) estimated that, excluding proper names, there were more than 88,500 word families represented in printed school English—far too many to teach through direct instruction. They suggested that other methods should be used to enable and encourage students to learn new words on their own. In essence, they were advocating a “self-teaching” mechanism for vocabulary.
The problem of volume is exacerbated by concerns about the discrepancy between vocabulary knowledge of low- and high-SES students, and the significance of vocabulary knowledge for literacy development. Interventions have been effective at advancing students’ vocabulary knowledge, but they have so far failed to close the gap. Higher SES students tend to start school with larger vocabularies, and interventions tend to benefit students who start with more word knowledge. Marulis and Neuman (2010) conducted a meta-analysis of vocabulary intervention studies for pre-K and kindergarten children. While the overall impact of vocabulary instruction was strong, middle- and upper income-children benefited most from the instruction.

514

In recent years, several lines of work have arisen in part to address the problem of volume. Foremost among these has been the attempt to identify a core vocabulary as a way of focusing attention on a smaller number of important words. There have been a number of recent attempts to identify a corpus of
“academic vocabulary” words—words worth teaching because they appear frequently in school texts (Baumann & Graves, 2010). A number of taxonomies
(Beck, McKeown, & Kucan, 2002; Fisher & Frey, 2008; Harmon, Wood, and
Hedrick, 2008; Hiebert & Lubliner, 2008), and instructional approaches have been built on the idea of a general academic that includes high-utility, cross-disciplinary words, none more popular than Beck, McKeown, and Kucan’s (2002) tiered scheme. Beck et al. developed a widely used vocabulary selection scheme that advises teachers to select a narrow band of useful general academic words, or tier two words, for instruction from the texts students encounter. Tier two words are words that are uncommon in life outside of school, but common in school texts.
Beck et al. estimate that there are only about 7,000 tier two word families, so that teaching just a few hundred each year could contribute to students verbal functioning and reading comprehension in school.
As yet, there is little research to support the efficacy of using academic vocabulary selection schemes to guide vocabulary instruction; the work, which we report later in a section on vocabulary pedagogy, of Snow and her colleagues on word generation (Snow, Lawrence, & White, 2009) serves as a notable exception.
Some researchers have questioned the idea that there is a single core vocabulary needed for academic study. For example, Hyland and Tse (2009) asked how well the words found on the widely used Academic Word List (Coxhead, 2000), which includes 570 word families, account for the words in texts that university students encounter across disciplines. They found that the Academic Word List (AWL) in combination with the 2,000 words on the general service list covered about 85% of the words in the corpus they studied, but that this distribution was uneven. Areas that require a more specialized vocabulary, such as science, were not well-covered by the AWL. They conclude that disciplinary words are shaped for highly specialized uses, undermining attempts to construct a core academic vocabulary.
In summary, a number of word level processes are known to underlie the meaningful reading of connected text, including the ability to manipulate sounds in speech, the ability to leverage a range of strategies for efficiently identifying words, and the ability to associate those words with information about their meaning and uses. Although the community of reading researchers has made significant progress over the past several decades in identifying these processes and understanding how they are learned, questions remain regarding individual variations among learners that impede some from becoming fluent word readers and how to contend with the multitude of words that students must read and understand in order to access school texts.

515

Text Level Processes
We turn now to the core of reading processes—text understanding. We deal with several key constructs: construction integration models of comprehension, the role of context, knowledge and comprehension, and disciplinary perspectives on reading comprehension.

Construction-Integration Models
If word recognition and word meaning are the point of word level processes, then comprehension is the point of text level processes—and comprehension is infinitely more complex, partially because it entails all of the word level processes. Successful reading comprehension depends on the proper execution and combination of a large number of cognitive processes. Despite differences in details, the theoretical cognitive models of reading comprehension are rather consistent in many respects (e.g., Goldman, Graesser & van den Broek,
1999; Ruddell & Unrau, 2004), so we adopt the language and constructs of
Kintsch’s (1998) Construction-Integration Model to illustrate the general principles of this class of models.1 Central to comprehension of a text is the construction of a coherent mental representation of the text (van den Broek, 2010).
A text can be represented at different levels: a surface form, a text-base, and a situation model (Kintsch, 1998). The surface form representation captures the actual words and phrases of the text. It tends to be short-lived and not strongly related to comprehension per se, as it contains little semantic information. The text-base representation includes the individual propositions/words in the text, together with the referential and other semantic relations that obtain between those propositions. The coherence of the text base depends on the quality of the original text, the reader’s accuracy at encoding that text (Cote, Goldman, & Saul, 1998), and the generation of local “bridging” inferences (e.g., those that resolve anaphoric reference and create cohesive ties, such as causal or time links, among propositions). Finally, the situation model representation captures the information provided by the text, independent of its particular expression, and integrated with the reader’s background knowledge. The situation model representation is the most relevant for educational purposes because it constitutes a generalizable and applicable knowledge base. Successful comprehension and the construction of a coherent representation require the development of a highly elaborated situation model (Trabasso, Secco, & van den Broek, 1984). Precisely how much and what prior knowledge becomes integrated in the situation model depends on the text and
1. This description of construction-integration models is based on an account co-constructed by Pearson and Paul van den Broek in a research proposal to the Institute of Education Sciences in 2009 (Wilson &
Pearson, 2009).

516

the reader’s prior knowledge but also on the task or purpose of comprehension
(van den Broek, Fletcher, & Risden, 1993; van den Broek, Lorch, Linderholm &
Gustafson, 2001). The properties of readers’ mental representations can be determined through various outcome tasks, for example, tasks that assess memory for what was presented, others that identify inferences that are warranted by the text in conjunction with general world knowledge, and still others require the application of the information in the text to new situations (Goldman, 1997;
Graesser, Gernsbacher, & Goldman, 1997; Kintsch, 2004; van den Broek, 1994).
Included in this family of theories are models that characterize text processing and knowledge representations in terms of semantic networks (Anderson, 1983;
Trabasso et al., 1984), schemas, frames, and scripts (Anderson & Pearson, 1984;
Rumelhart & Ortony, 1977), mental models (Johnson-Laird, 1983; McNamara,
Miller, & Bransford, 1991), and dual-coding in verbal and nonverbal systems
(Paivio, 1990; Sadoski & Paivio, 2001).
The construction of a mental representation occurs primarily online as the text is read—rather than after reading has been completed. And it is inherently iterative and dynamic, with the situation model changing as new information from the text and new knowledge sources from memory are instantiated moment by moment (Linderholm, Virtue, van den Broek & Tzeng, 2004). For this reason, much research has been dedicated to identifying the processes, strategies, skills, and background knowledge that readers must have to arrive at a coherent situation model of the text. Using a variety of methods, including speeded responses, reading rate, verbal think-aloud protocols, computer simulations, and, recently, eye tracking and neuro-imaging techniques— considerable insights have been gained in the online process of comprehension. One such insight is that “each new piece of linguistic information is understood in terms of the information it evokes from memory” (Gerrig & McKoon, 1998, p. 69). A crucial aspect of the reading process as it runs its course during reading is that the reader has to achieve a balance between the severe limitations of his/her working memory, or attentional capacity, on the one hand and his/her need to achieve coherence (Kintsch, 2004; van den Broek, Young, Tzeng, & Linderholm, 1998/2004). As a result of limited working memory capacity, only a small subset of the textual information and of background knowledge can be processed by the reader at any particular instant during reading. The selection of information for retention in working memory is a critical determinant of the eventual representation of the text as a whole. Such selection is partly the result of automatic processes (once certain lexical items make it into memory, strong evocations are sure to follow), and partly that of strategic (i.e., reader-controlled and deliberate) processes (e.g., searching for a plausible fit among items in memory) (Thurlow & van den Broek, 1997)
Kintsch’s Construction-Integration model (Kintsch, 1988, 1998) captures the interaction between text and knowledge in a two-phase process model. The construction phase is text-based and bottom-up; in that phase, textual information

517

activates background knowledge in an associative and relatively uncontrolled, almost automatic, manner (see also the memory-based model; Gerrig & O’Brien,
2005). The initial activation is followed by a second phase in which activated knowledge and the concepts/ideas in the text are integrated into a coherent mental representation; the product of this integration phase is the situation model. During integration, background knowledge supports connections between and to ideas from the texts, and provides the foundation for inferences. This balancing act by the reader implies that the complex cognitive processes require coordination and regulation: Readers may strategically search and reactivate information from the preceding text (from memory or by reinspecting the actual text) and/or strategically search and activate background knowledge (van den Broek, 1990).
Effective readers know when their efforts at comprehension require such strategic interventions and what constitutes appropriate, corrective steps (Baker & Brown,
1984; Cote et al., 1998).

Coordinating Cognitive Processes
These examples illustrate the extent to which reading comprehension requires the coordination of various cognitive processes and skills. Individuals can differ considerably in these processes and skills. As noted, they depend on efficient attention allocation strategies that select information that is likely to serve as an appropriate context for the integration of new information in a text. They depend on the availability of working memory capacity to hold the selected information until it has been processed adequately. They depend on rapid, automatic access to long-term memory so that connections are recognized between currently processed information and relevant information encountered much earlier in a text, or to make connections between information presented by the author and relevant background knowledge possessed by the reader (van den
Broek, Bohn-Gettler, Kendeou, Carlson, & White, 2011).

Moderating Contextual Effects
To some extent, the skills and processes required for successful reading in these illustrations apply to all contexts in which reading takes place. However, their implementation is strongly influenced by the context—for example, the text genre, the subject area of the text (history versus physics or literature), and the reader’s goals (Kintsch, 1998). Moreover, readers must apply context-appropriate strategies. For example, different types of text invite different purposes, possess different structures and features, and revolve around different types of information: Narratives revolve around characters in specific situations and with specific goals, whereas exposition revolves around the development of topics that may be related in many different ways. Moreover, they differ in the kind of

518

background knowledge that may be helpful in comprehending the text. In this sense, discipline-specific background knowledge includes both content knowledge
(i.e., background knowledge about the topics in the text) and strategic knowledge
(which standards of coherence are appropriate to this discipline and its default reading goals, what processes are appropriate given the particular text genre/structure, how this influences effective allocation of attention and processes such as memory search, what text-processing signals are present in this discipline).
An important implication is that a reader’s background knowledge, understanding of a specific text genre, knowledge of situation-dependent strategies, and other considerations all influence the extent to which the reader will be able to construct a coherent representation. Thus, the reading comprehension skills of a reader vary and then converge to allow her to construct an understanding while reading a particular text for a particular purpose (van den Broek et al., 2011).
In addition to the cognitive processes enacted during reading comprehension, a reader needs to possess basic language and reading skills such as letter- and word-identification, syntactic knowledge. A certain level of mastery of these skills is necessary for comprehension to occur; such mastery allows for automatic, or at least facile, translation of the symbols in the text into the propositions that will constitute the basis for constructing models at all three levels—the surface form and the text base and situation model. In addition, if the basic language and reading skills consume considerable working memory capacity, then the capacity available to the comprehension processes themselves will be severely limited (Perfetti, 1999), rendering the construction of the situation model in particular more difficult. However, these skills are themselves not enough to produce comprehension of the text as a whole. Thus, word and sentence level skills are necessary but insufficient for adequate comprehension. Recent investigations of the developmental trajectories of language comprehension skills and basic language skills, respectively, confirm this view (Gough & Tunmer,
1986; Kendeou, Savage, & van den Broek, 2009; Whitehurst & Lonigan, 1998). In longitudinal studies, the two sets of skills—those pertaining to basic language and language comprehension skills, respectively have been found to develop relatively independently from preschool into the early primary grades and, then, combine to predict reading comprehension in the later grades (Kendeou, van den
Broek, White, & Lynch, 2007; Kendeou, van den Broek, White & Lynch, 2009;
Oakhill, Cain, & Bryant, 2003; van den Broek, White, Kendeou & Carlson, 2009).
As a matter of practice, then, monitoring and teaching both sets of skills and strategies in their own right seems necessary; indeed, the research on pedagogy reviewed in other parts of this chapter suggests exactly that.

519

Knowledge—A Multilayered Construct
Knowledge, as represented in long-term memory, is key to the comprehension process. Its role in integration phase of building a situation model is transparent, as the ideas from the emerging text-base trigger or instantiate precisely those schemata from long-term memory required to build that coherent representation of text we call the situation model. Many of the schemata that are triggered in this process will be ideas about the topic, domain, or discipline in which the text resides. But many other kinds of knowledge are also implicated.
For example, knowledge about language at virtually every level of analysis— phonological, morphological, lexical, semantic, syntactic, and pragmatic—can and will be engaged in building both a situation model and, equally as important, in establishing the cohesion among sentences (e.g., resolving anaphora or logical relations among sentences) that distinguishes a text-base from the mere surface form of a text.
But also important will be knowledge about text—what it is and how it works. Text knowledge includes everything from (a) the conventions of a particular orthography and how they map onto the phonological code required for accessing the lexicon to (b) knowledge of the genres that typify a subject matter like geography—what they are how they work to (c) small but significant matters such as text features—headings, visual displays, lists, captions, indexes, and the like. Strategic Knowledge
One type of knowledge plays a very special role in reading comprehension—strategy knowledge. As we suggested earlier, readers use strategies throughout the comprehension process as they engage in intentional searches of the text-base and their knowledge structures at points in the process of building a situation model. Most commonly, strategies are invoked precisely when the automatic processes of constraint satisfaction (making sure that the current version of the situation model satisfies the informational constraints coming from the text-base and the knowledge base) are not working well (van den Broek et al,
2011). Many readers develop these strategies for “free” in the sense that they pick them up along the way by just reading a lot. Other readers require more intentional efforts on the part of schools and teachers in order to use strategies effectively;
Kintsch (2004) discusses the high likelihood that many if not most novice readers will require explicit instruction and modeling in using these strategies. But strategy use is not solely the province of novice or poor readers. To the contrary, expert readers are highly competent strategy users; it is just that their strategy use is so fluent, so “skilled” in the sense of having reached an automatic level of operation, that we do not see it in action very easily or often. But put those expert

520

readers in a situation where they are forced to use them (a really difficult or unfamiliar text) or ask to use them (as in a think-aloud protocol), and a wellelaborated, well-articulated strategy infrastructure is readily revealed (Alexander,
2003, 2005). This does not mean that these strategies are necessarily a normal part of the everyday reading process for them (i.e., when readers are experiencing nothing but the automatic “clicks” of comprehension), but it does mean that they are always there to assist in case a comprehension “clunk” (Klinger & Vaughn,
1999) has just been or is about to be experienced.

Disciplinary Perspectives on Reading
In recent years, the dominant view of reading as a set of general skills that can be applied to a variety of texts, purposes, and disciplines has been challenged by research and theory suggesting instead that reading is dependent on the nature of texts and disciplinary practices in which it is situated. This shift in perspectives on reading is attributable in part to the genre movement discussed below.
However, it has also been precipitated by concerns that a decade-long focus on
“basic” reading skills, including generalizable comprehension skills and strategies, failed to produce a generation of students who were prepared in adolescence or adulthood for the demands of discipline-based reading.
Several recent reports on reading and adolescent literacy have called for attention to text- and discipline-specific reading practices (e.g., Alliance for
Excellent Education, 2010; Heller & Greenleaf, 2007; Rand Reading Study Group,
2002). In fact, it can be argued that the recently developed Common Core
Standards privilege just such an approach with the inclusion of separate standards strand for literature, history, and science (and technical subjects). These reports point to the need not only to continue to support students’ development of literacy skills beyond the early elementary years, but also to support students in learning to read and write in ways that will specifically foster involvement in disciplinary learning (Alliance for Excellent Education, 2010; Common Core Standards, 2010;
Heller & Greenleaf, 2007; T. Shanahan & C. Shanahan, 2008).

Texts
The most obvious difference in reading as students move into different disciplinary context concerns the nature of the texts (van den Broek, 2010). Texts that students encounter in history are quite different from those than they encounter in chemistry (Carnegie Corporation of New York’s Council on
Advancing Adolescent Literacy). Lee and Spratley (2010) note that scientific reports and textbooks include vocabulary and syntactic forms that can be difficult for inexperienced readers (see also Snow, 2010). In addition, these texts often include features, such as abstracts, headings, and diagrams, which can support

521

understanding if students are taught to use them. The recent reports on adolescent literacy generally express concern that the emphasis on generic reading comprehension strategies may lead students to conclude that all content-area texts can be approached the same way such that reading in math is identical to reading in history (Heller & Greenleaf, 2007). The authors of the Carnegie report, Time to
Act (2010), conclude that students should be taught skills and strategies for reading texts in each content-area. The Rand Reading Study Group (2002) goes further, suggesting that discipline specific reading comprehension tasks must be learned in the context of learning the content of the discipline and participating in disciplinary inquiry.

Skills and Processes
Although systematic variations in text content and organization are the most visible difference in reading across content areas, T. Shanahan and C.
Shanahan (2008) point out that the move into disciplinary reading involves more than the application of generalized reading skills to new texts; it involves the use of more sophisticated and specialized skills and practices. Interest in the lexical, syntactic, and organizational characteristics of content area texts and the challenges that these present to students is not new (Osborne, 2010; Snow, 2010).
What has come to the fore in recent decades is interest in discipline-specific inquiry practices and methods of communication, and how these are reflected in uses of language, the organization of texts, and the relationships between texts and ways of developing knowledge (Heller & Greenleaf, 2007; Moje, 2008). The turn toward a disciplinary view of literacy also reflects a recognition that literacy is an essential part of any disciplinary practice rather than merely a means of improving students’ reading of content-area textbooks (Moje). Heller and Greenleaf point out that: To become competent in a number of academic content areas requires more than just applying the same old skills and comprehensions strategies to new kinds of texts. It also requires skills and knowledge and reasoning processes that are specific to particular disciplines. (p. 10)

Empirical and theoretical work in disciplinary literacy has started to identify how literacy practices differ across disciplines and how these differences are related to the nature of the disciplinary practices. For example, T. Shanahan and C. Shanahan (2008) examined the reading processes of disciplinary experts as they read and thought about texts in their areas. The researchers found that the experts in each discipline approached texts differently and leveraged a different set of reading strategies. For example, whereas historians attended to possible sources of bias, mathematicians engaged in close examination and rereading of the text qua text, to ensure they understood the contribution of each word to the meaning,

522

and scientists tended to examine the credibility of the work that lay behind the text
(who produced, where, and for what purpose).
T. Shanahan and C. Shanahan (2008) suggested that differences in the reading practices of disciplinary experts are related to the values, norms, and methods of scholarship within each discipline. That is, historians read for the author’s perspective, because historical scholarship is characterized by retrospective analysis of source documents and thus risks selective analysis and biased interpretation. Because chemists build knowledge through experimentation, they read to understand the procedures used to obtain particular results.
Leinhardt and Young (1996) asked three expert historians to read and interpret two historical documents, one of which was close to and one far from their area of expertise. The researchers found that historians engaged in classification (identifying the type of document), corroboration (checking the accuracy of a document by looking for consistency across the text and with other texts), sourcing (identifying things like authorship, publication date, and location to uncover the nature and influence of the bias that is assumed to be part of every historical document), and contextualization (“asking what else was happening when and where the document was written by locating it historically in terms of prior, coincident, and consequential events”).
Wineburg (1991) examined the reading practices of historians and high school students as they read a set of historical documents about the American
Revolution. He found that historians, unlike high school students, move beyond a literal reading of history texts to approach them as both rhetorical artifacts and as human artifacts. When approaching texts as rhetorical artifacts, historians consider authors’ purposes, intentions and goals, and the ways that the authors use language for persuasive purposes. In approaching texts as human artifacts, historians examine how texts reveal information about authors’ views and beliefs. The historians also engaged in conversations with the texts that extend beyond the author-reader dialogue to include different reader stances and audiences.
Wineburg attributed differences in the ways historians and students read the texts to different epistemological beliefs about historical inquiry. Students approached texts as the bearers of information and approaches reading as a process of information gathering, whereas historians viewed the texts as human creations and social exchanges. For students, the connection between the author and the text was scarcely a consideration in their reading.
In this section, we have discussed the many factors—from knowledge and reparative strategies to text and context—that influence a reader’s ability to make meaning as they read. Taken together with the underling word level processes, a picture of the complexity of reading, and by implication the complexity of learning to read, begins to emerge. In the next section, we transition to the learning to read perspective by examining the research on the role of instruction in helping

523

students gain mastery of the many processes and understandings that comprise expert reading.

Instructional Contexts for Reading
Development
We turn now from research about processes to research about the intentional acquisition of those processes, namely curriculum and pedagogy. The sections within this section parallel those in the previous section on basic processes. Word Level Instruction
An important benchmark in shaping instructional practices for word level processes—and for early reading instruction more generally—was Marilyn
Adams’ 1990 book, Beginning to Read: Teaching and Learning about Print. It provided a complete synthesis of our pre-1990 knowledge base about basic reading processes and the processes of reading acquisition. Sponsored by the then
Office of Educational Research and Improvement, it was the third in a long line of national syntheses about how best to teach the basic components of early reading, preceded historically by Chall’s 1967, Learning to Read: The Great Debate, and
Anderson, Hiebert, Wilkinson, and Scott’s 1984, Becoming a Nation of Readers. It is a benchmark because it appeared in the field at the height of the surge of constructivist reforms of the era, most notably whole language and literature based reading (see Pearson, 2004, for a detailed summary of this era) and preceded the return to an early emphasis on the code that began about a half decade later. While causal inferences are unwarranted, it is certainly likely that Adams’ book provided a ready and credible knowledge base for those wishing to move back to an earlier and more consistent early code emphasis.
In 1998, another national synthesis, this time commissioned by the National
Academy of Science, resulted in Snow, Burns, and Griffith’s Preventing Reading
Difficulties. It is distinguishable from Adams’ synthesis in taking on a much broader research and policy agenda (e.g., mainstream instruction, instruction for struggling students, preschool, early reading, teacher education, and professional development) but to a somewhat narrower end (i.e., preventing reading difficulties through early interventions of various sorts). The past decade ushered in two additional syntheses, the 2000 National Reading Panel Report (NICHD, 2000) and the National Early Literacy Panel of 2008.

524

Code-Focused Instruction, and More
Across the last four syntheses, beginning with Adams’ book, a remarkably consistent message has emerged, based on the then available research. The conclusions and recommendations differ only in particular details, which are driven most likely by additional research insights undergirding each report. All four reports, for example, converge on an early emphasis on the code, opting for systematic phonics instruction (of no particular variety—analytic and synthetic are not privileged over one another) early in K-1. That phonics instruction should be accompanied by instruction in phonemic awareness (hearing the separate sounds in spoken words), with a nod going approaches that link phonemic awareness to specific letter sound correspondences and do not dally for too long on the process).
And all of these word-level skills, according to all of these reports, should be situated within a language rich, balanced literacy program that promotes wordlevel and text-level expertise and general language competence and world knowledge. We mention these latter recommendations because these reports, especially the National Reading Panel (NRP), often get labeled as code-based or at least skill-based policy documents when, in fact, their recommendations tend to be much more balanced than their public reputation.

The National Early Literacy Panel
The review of interventions in the National Early Literacy Panel (2008) deserves elaborated consideration because it is quite comprehensive and, in comparison to earlier syntheses, comes with the added benefit of research conducted in the era of No Child Left Behind (NCLB). In a nutshell, the NELP identified five interventions that achieved moderate to large effects on student literacy outcomes. Like earlier reports, NELP found strong effects for codefocused instruction, both phonemic awareness training and early phonics instruction on a range of early literacy outcomes. Like the Preventing Reading
Difficulties (PRD) report of 1998, it also found enduring effects for languageenhanced programs, primarily on oral language development. But unlike earlier syntheses, it found additional effects for book-sharing programs (on print knowledge and oral language), home and parent programs (on oral language and general cognitive abilities), and comprehensive preschool and kindergarten programs (on spelling and reading readiness skills). In other words, NELP expanded beyond the traditional word and within word foci to include a range of contextual variations in pedagogy, with the result that both word level (e.g., alphabet knowledge, phonemic awareness, and letter sound correspondences) and meaning level (oral language and cognitive abilities) were enhanced.
Earlier national syntheses identified most of these categories of interventions as useful in developing students’ literacy background and capacity

525

for benefiting from instruction (Adams, 1990; Snow et al., 1998). However, no previous effort had collected all of the available evidence on all of these programs and examined it through the lens of meta-analyses. It is encouraging to early literacy experts to know that this range of interventions make a consistent difference in profiles of student achievement on valued early literacy outcomes.
Pearson and Hiebert, in reviewing NELP in 2010, noted that another unique finding from NELP was these five general programmatic categories tended to influence different sorts of outcomes, suggesting a kind of “specificity” of effects, a phenomenon that often influences instructional research in general (see Moran,
Ferdig, Pearson, Wardrop, & Blohmeyer, 2008, for a vivid example of this specificity phenomenon).

Vocabulary Instruction
A wealth of instructional studies during the 1980s and 1990s demonstrated that the meanings of words can be taught through a wide assortment of approaches. In 2000, the National Reading Panel (NRP) emphasized the importance of vocabulary instruction, but did not find sufficient evidence to recommend some methods of instruction over others. The NRP’s analysis did synthesize the findings of previous research to identify characteristics of successful vocabulary instruction, including opportunities for students to encounter target words multiple times in meaningful contexts and to use the words actively. A decade earlier, Stahl and Fairbanks (1986) had famously found that instruction of word meanings in context is more effective than no-context instruction of word meanings. The NRP also confirmed the effectiveness of direct instruction of at least some words as a supplement to exposure through wide reading. Many vocabulary instruction experts recommend a multicomponent approach to developing vocabulary knowledge. For example, Graves (2000) has advocated a four-part program that includes:
1. Teaching individual, high-utility words.
2. Wide reading.
3. Teaching word-learning strategies, including morphology.
4. Fostering word consciousness, an interest in words.
A significant recent development in vocabulary research has concerned instruction of generative word learning strategies that allow students to more readily acquire knowledge of new words. In part as a response to the aforementioned volume problem in vocabulary instruction has been interest in identifying effective practices for supporting students’ incidental learning of new vocabulary from reading and listening. While in the past there has been a tendency to think about vocabulary knowledge as “consisting of isolated, memorized information about the meanings of specific words,” this conception has been come

526

to be seen as inadequate (p. 29). Vocabulary researchers are seeking ways to teach knowledge and dispositions that increase the likelihood that children will learn new words on their own. The most notable of these is the work on instruction about the morphological structure of words.

Morphology
A growing line of work on generative word knowledge has considered the role of morphological knowledge—knowledge of small, meaningful units of language, including roots and affixes—in acquiring knowledge of new words. The question underlying this work is whether students are able to infer the meanings of new words through the analysis of the words’ meaningful parts. Morphological knowledge has long been identified as part of the explanation for how students acquire new vocabulary knowledge (Carlisle, 2007; Nagy & Anderson, 1984). It has also been known for some time that morphological awareness is related to size of vocabulary and reading comprehension. Morphological awareness has recently been associated with several additional components of literacy development, including decoding and spelling, vocabulary, and reading comprehension
(Carlisle, 2010).
The research on the utility of teaching morphological analysis has been limited (Baumann, Bradley, Edwards, Font, & Hruby, 2000), but in recent years, there has been an interest in whether morphological instruction can support word learning (Carlisle, 2010), and a growing body of evidence that students can use knowledge of meaningful word parts to solve the meanings of novel words containing the same parts.
Several studies have explored the efficacy of instruction in morphological analysis. Baumann, Edwards, Boland, Olejnik, and Kame’enui (2003) compared the effects of morphemic and contextual analysis instruction (MC) with textbook vocabulary (TV) instruction on fifth-grade students’ vocabulary learning and reading comprehension. Students in the TV group were taught specific words from the textbook, while MC students received instruction in morphemic and contextual analysis strategies using example words from the textbook. Students who were directly taught the vocabulary words made stronger growth on a test of those words, but students who received the MC intervention made stronger growth in their abilities to decipher the meaning of new morphemically decipherable words in isolation and (on a delayed but not immediate posttest) in context. There were no differences in comprehension growth.
Bowers and Kirby (2010) examined the impact of an intervention focused on teaching morphological word structure to fourth and fifth graders. Students who received the morphological instruction were better able to identify base words

527

in new words and better able to define taught and new words as long as the new words were within taught morphological families.

Teaching Academic Vocabulary
One recent instructional program of direct teaching of target words that has demonstrated effectiveness in middle school is the Word Generation program
(Snow et al., 2009). Snow et al. describe the program as it was implemented with students in grades six through eight. The program, which focused on teaching high-utility academic words, involved students in encountering words repeatedly in semantically rich and varied contexts. It also offered opportunities for students to use the words actively in talk and writing. Students in the word generation classrooms made greater gains in their knowledge of the instructed academic vocabulary words than students in control classrooms, and there was some evidence that participation also positively impacted students’ standardized state
English language arts test scores.

Text-Level Instruction
Beginning in the 1970s, research on improving text level comprehension has been an active area of pedagogical scholarship, with a wide range of synthetic reviews and meta-analyses (e.g., Duke & Pearson, 2002; Duke, Pearson, Strachan,
& Billman, 2011; Murphy, Wilkinson, Soter, Hennessey, & Alexander, 2009;
Pearson & Fielding, 1991; Pressley, 2000; Tierney & Cunningham, 1984;
Wilkinson & Son, 2011). Looking across the substantial body of research and at the key syntheses, several consistent findings emerge, although none of them comes with what would be judged a strong evidence base (e.g., as defined by the
What Works Clearninghouse). Consistently emerging in these reviews are several interventions that could be labeled comprehension fostering (after Palincsar &
Brown, 1984, and recently reinvoked by Duke, et al., 2011) to capture a set of practices that are less about direct instruction of comprehension processes, skills, or strategies and more about facilitating comprehension though other activities in the school environment—practices such as numbers 2, 3, 6, 8, 9, and 10 in this list from Duke et al., 2011) in the list below. The others (4, 5, 7, and perhaps 1) come closer to what we have in mind when we talk about direct comprehension instruction, but even building disciplinary knowledge and promoting vocabulary growth have as much of a fostering as they do a teaching patina to them.
1. Build disciplinary and world knowledge.
2. Provide exposure to a volume and range of texts.
3. Provide motivating texts and contexts for reading.
4. Teach strategies for comprehending.
5. Teach text structures.

528

6. Engage students in discussion.
7. Build vocabulary and language knowledge.
8. Integrate reading and writing.
9. Observe and assess.
10. Differentiate instruction
For our purposes, we have divided this research into four categories: (1) discussion as a medium for promoting text comprehension; (2) reading strategy instruction; (3) instruction in text structures, including genres; and (4) instruction embedded in the pursuit of acquiring disciplinary knowledge. It should be noted that the evidence for comprehension instruction, be it fostering or teaching, is not limited to the intermediate and secondary levels of schooling. To the contrary,
Shanahan et al. (2010) were able to document five practices with various levels of empirical evidence to support their efficacy: (1) strategy instruction, (2) using text structure to organize learning, (3) discussion, (4) selecting texts to support comprehension, and (5) establishing an engaging and motivating classroom context for supporting comprehension. Only strategy instruction earned a strong evidence rating, while text structure and providing an engaging context earned a moderate evidence rating. Discussion, and text selection earned a weak evidence rating. A weak evidence rating is a bit misleading in this context because these practices, while not possessing anything like randomized field trial evidence to assess their efficacy, at least (and unlike a host of highly recommended and widely implemented practices with absolutely no evidence to evaluate their efficacy) have been evaluated in either correlational or nonrandomized experimental studies.

Talk About Text
Reading comprehension instruction has been heavily influenced by understandings about the role of social interaction in learning. Following in the tradition of Vygotsky (1934/1978), who suggested that higher order cognitive functions develop first in the social sphere, many approaches to comprehension instruction focus on discussion of text as a key aspect of learning to develop the cognitive habits of highly skilled readers. Discussion-oriented approaches reflect the idea that talk not only helps students to internalize expert ways of interacting with text, but also helps readers to clarify and consolidate their learning from text.
The positive effects of thoughtful and cognitively challenging discussion on reading achievement have been documented in a wide array of studies (e.g.,
Gambrell & Morrow, 1996; Kong & Pearson, 2003; Raphael & McMahon, 1994;
Taylor, Pearson, Peterson, & Rodriguez, 2003). In addition, numerous instructional routines for text-based discussion have been described in the reading research and practice literatures. These include Book Club (Raphael, FlorioRuane, & George, 2001; Raphael & McMahon, 1994); Questioning the Author
(Beck & McKeown, 2006); Instructional Conversations (Goldenberg, 1993;

529

Rueda, Goldenberg, & Gallimore, 1992; Tharp & Gallimore, 1991); and
Collaborative Reasoning (see Clark et al., 2003)
These instructional routines can all be described as a set of strategies, or moves, which can be used flexibly by teachers to encourage talk that invites students to share their reasoning and grapple with cognitively challenging ideas. In all of these frameworks it is expected that students will assume a degree of control over their own learning over time, and that they will work toward improving their peer conversations through intentional reflections.
In addition, these approaches generally share a focus on teacher as coach and guide. The teacher’s role is not to provide answers but instead to model the language of academic discussion for students through clarifying, mediating turn taking when necessary, and probing students to think even more deeply about relevant aspects of the text.
Soter et al. (2008) reported the results of a study designed to evaluate the relative efficacy of nine discussion routines. In addition to those mentioned above,
Soter et al. examined Grand Conversations (Eeds & Wells, 1989), Literature
Circles (Short & Pierce, 1990) Junior Great Books (Great Books Foundation,
1987), Philosophy for Children (Sharp, 1985), and Paedia Seminar (Billings &
Fitzgerald, 2002). Soter et al. identified features of classroom discourse that indicate high-level thinking and comprehension. These included the posing of authentic questions by teachers and students; students’ elaborated responses, questions, and reasoning language; and the presence of uptake by teacher and students. The researchers then used the features to analyze samples of student discourse resulting from each of the nine discussion routines. The researchers found that critical-analytic approaches, such as Collaborative Reasoning and
Philosophy for Children, and the expressive approaches, such as Book Club and
Grand Conversations, invited the most high-level thinking and reasoning by students. These approaches involved a high incidence of authentic questions, elaborated explanations, and uptake.
In a related meta-analysis, Murphy et al. (2009) found that the impacts on students’ comprehension were inconsistent. In particular they found that, while many of the discussion routines promoted students’ literacy and inferential comprehension, there was great variability in the degree to which different routines promoted high level comprehension of text (e.g., critical thinking, reasoning, and argumentation). Only a few of the routines (Collaborative
Reasoning, Philosophy for Children, and Junior Great Books) were effective at increasing both literacy comprehension and higher-level comprehension in multiple-group design studies.
Although Soter et al. (2008) looked only at discussions of literature, there is some evidence that involvement in discussions about text supports content-area learning by, for example, supporting conceptual understanding in science

530

(Palincsar & Magnusson, 2001), inviting all readers to employ reading strategies
(Chinn, Anderson, & Waggoner, 2001), and even increasing the efficacy of comprehension strategy instruction (Berne & Clark, 2008).

Comprehension Strategy Instruction
Reading comprehension in U.S. classrooms is largely taught through instruction and practice with comprehension strategies, such as predicting, clarifying, activating prior knowledge, summarizing, and questioning. The rise of comprehension strategy instruction in recent decades has been grounded in substantial research demonstrating that high-achieving readers use more strategies than low-achieving readers (Block & Pressley, 2001; Dole, Duffy, Roehler, &
Pearson, 1991; Rapp, van den Broek, McMaster, Kendeou & Espin, 2007)). Strategy instruction emerged from the understanding that good readers are thoughtful about their own understanding (or lack of understanding) and skilled in developing plans for fixing comprehension when it goes awry.
In addition, the ascendancy of strategy instruction in the reading curriculum has been bolstered by a vast body of correlational and intervention studies that have supported the value of comprehension strategies. These studies have consistently demonstrated that students who are explicitly taught to use comprehension strategies can apply them with the result of improved comprehension and can transfer the strategies to the comprehension of new texts
(Brown, Pressley, Van Meter, & Schuder, 1996; Paris, Lipson, & Wixson, 1994; see also Dole, et al, 1991; Duke et al., 2011; Duke & Pearson, 2001, 2002;
Pressley, 2000; Rosenshine & Meister, 1994).
The particular configuration of cognitive and metacognitive strategies for reading varies across studies and research syntheses, but the list from the National
Reading Panel (NICHD, 2000) report is a good representation of the core set of strategies. This list includes: comprehension monitoring, cooperative learning, graphic organizers, story structure, question answering, question generation, and summarization. There are also several major strategy “suites” in reading instruction, which combine multiple strategies into a coherent approach. These include Reciprocal Teaching (Palincsar & Brown, 1984; Rosenshine & Meister,
1994) and Transactional Strategies Instruction (Brown et al., 1996).
In response to the question of how to teach strategies, Duke and Pearson
(2002) offer a set of steps that typically occur in effective explicit strategy instruction across scores of instructional studies:
Naming and describing the strategy—why, when, and how it should be used.
Modeling the strategy in action—either by teacher or student, or both.
Using the strategy collaborative—in a sort of group think-aloud.
Guiding practice using the strategy with gradual release of responsibility.

531

Using the strategy independently, with no teacher guidance, either individually or in small student-led groups.
For more than two decades there has been broad consensus that strategies should be taught, along with agreement about which strategies matter most (Duke
& Pearson, 2002). In the past few years, and in light of the emergence of attention to nonfiction text genres, cracks have emerged in the consensus around comprehension strategy instruction. For example, most studies of strategy instruction have focused on comprehension of fictional texts and has been conducted in the context of English language arts instruction, but Vitale and
Romance (2007) suggest that research on comprehension strategy instruction lacks ecological validity for science reading because it fails to situate comprehension in forms of content-area learning that require cumulative meaningful understanding.
Likewise, Fisher and Frey (2008) express concern that the preoccupation with strategies is making strategies, rather than texts, the focus of reading instruction.
While not faulting strategy instruction itself, Pearson (2011) points out that the dynamic and responsive character of strategy use has sometimes been lost as it is encoded in commercial reading programs. That is, strategy use has sometimes become an end unto itself, rather than a set of tools for achieving and repairing comprehension. McKeown, Beck, and Blake (2009), in a direct comparison with of strategy instruction with content focused instruction (what might be construed as a “rich talk about ideas in the text” approach), found that content treatment resulted in better performance on narrative recall and expository learning probes.
The strategy instruction group was indistinguishable from a basal driven control group (and the control group actually exceeded the strategy group on a couple of measures given along the way). In addition, Wilkinson and Son (2011) point out, while we know that comprehension strategy instruction improves comprehension, two decades of research has failed to identify the optimal set of strategies or even the optimal number of strategies. The most obvious explanation for the efficacy of strategy instruction is that instruction increases the use of strategies, and strategies increase understanding of text (Pressley, Brown, El-Dinary, & Afflerbach, 1995).
However, it may simply be that strategic (focused and intentional) behavior in general, rather than any set of particular strategies, matters most. Following W.
Kintsch and E. Kintsch (2005), Wilkinson and Son note that a common feature of all comprehension strategies is that they support students in actively constructing meaning as they read and invite readers to connect texts with their prior knowledge. It may be that that these underlying activities, rather than any particular strategies, are key ingredients of comprehension.

Genre and Text Structure Instruction
In the 1990s interest in text genre began to take hold in North America, although it had been alive and well in Australia (see Cope & Kalantzis, 1993) since at least the early application of functional systemic linguistics (Halliday,

532

1961) to pedagogy and curriculum. Some reading educators in the United States had become concerned about the emphasis on “authentic” fictional literature had excluded attention to other text genres, particularly nonfiction text genres.
Motivated in part by suspicions that literature-based programs were failing to support vocabulary development, and that reading instruction was failing to prepare students for the texts and tests of later schooling (Rand, 2002), advocates of greater emphasis on informational text argued that the balance of texts in early reading should reflect the balance of texts that students will encounter as they continue in school and the texts that they will read in their lives outside of school—contexts dominated by nonfiction text genres (e.g., Duke & BennettArmistead, 2003).
At the same time, the rise of standards-based education and the increased emphasis on annual testing in reading heightened educators’ interest in expository reading and writing, particularly as concern surfaced that the “fourth grade slump” might be attributable in part to lack of preparation for informational reading in grades K-3 (Moss, 2005). The rapid rise of in interest in the use of informational text in the elementary grades at the turn of the century is evident in professional publications for teachers. Moss (2005) analyzed the topics of articles appearing in the most prominent practitioner-oriented reading journal, The Reading Teacher, between 2000 and 2004. Moss found that, unlike during preceding decades, most articles clustered around two topics, one of which is the uses of informational trade books.
The new movement to include a greater diversity of genres, especially informational genres, in early reading instruction was accompanied by a move away from a generalist view of reading, in which reading is understood as a set of skills that can be applied to any text (T. Shanahan & C. Shanahan, 2008). As a result, advocates of informational text wanted not only to change the relative balance of text genres in elementary classrooms, they also wanted to reshape instruction to reflect the fact that different genres of texts should be read differently—that reading comprehension is dependent, in part, on an understanding of genre characteristics, such as text structure and text features.
Reading educators came to believe that different types of texts required different understandings, skills, and strategies, and, therefore, required different forms of instruction. The 2002 Rand report, Reading for Understanding, reflected this view, noting that “the features of text have a large effect on comprehension” (p. 14).
Research has supported the idea that some text genres are more difficult to comprehend than others. For example, Best, Floyd, and McNamara (2008) examined decoding and world knowledge as factors in third graders’ comprehension of narrative and expository texts. They found that students’ comprehension scores (multiple choice, free recall, and cued recall) were lower for an expository science text than for a narrative story. Scores on all three comprehension measures were predicted by world knowledge for the expository

533

text, but decoding ability was a more consistent predictor of comprehension for the narrative text.
Although genre is more appropriately thought of as a set of functional distinctions than organizational ones, much of the instructional work has focused on two salient organizational aspects of text genre—text structures and text features. As such, we address each of these separately, starting with text structure.
Research has also supported the idea that some students are more aware of some text structures than others (Englert & Hiebert, 1984) and that this awareness is related to students’ comprehension of text (e.g., Taylor & Samuels, 1983).
Richgels, McGee, Lomax, and Sheard (1987), for example, found that sixth-grade students demonstrated better awareness of comparison/contrast, collection, and problem/solution text structures than causation structures. Meyer, Brandt, and
Bluth (1980) found that ninth-grade students’ awareness of and use of text structure in organizing a recall from text was strongly related to the amount of information that the students recalled.
Given the role of text structure awareness in comprehension of expository texts, it stands to reason that providing instruction in text structures might improve students’ comprehension. Evidence on this score is mounting. Historically, in the late 1970s and 1980s, there was a short-lived but powerful burst of research on text structure instruction (see Pearson & Camparell, 1981, for an extensive review). More recently, attention to text structure has resurfaced with a resurgence in interest prompted, at least in part, by the Rand Report (2002) and, even more recently, by the What Works practice guide on reading comprehension in the primary grades (Shanahan et al., 2010). The conclusions of the What Works panel suggested that teaching students to use story maps while reading narratives
(Baumann & Bergeron,1993; Morrow, 1984, 1996; Reutzel, Smith, & Fawson,
2005; Williams et al., 2007) or particular expository structures, such as causeeffect (Reutzel et al., 2005; Williams et al., 2007), while reading expository texts yields moderate effects on reading comprehension. The text structure instructional research typically pairs text structure instruction with other instructional practices, such as comprehension strategies that are tailored to the text structure or discussion focused on the content. This sort of practice undoubtedly strengthens the pedagogical package, but such confounding makes it impossible to isolate the variable or variables in the package that might be serving as the active ingredient(s). Thus we can conclude only that text structure instruction, when offered in concert with X, or Y, or Z, has a positive impact on text comprehension.
A study by Williams, Hall Lauer, Stafford, DeSisto, and deCani (2005) serves as a typical and well-designed example of text structure instructional research. They tested an instructional program for second graders designed to teacher them to comprehend compare-contrast expository texts. Compared with a content-focused condition and a no-instruction control, second graders who

534

received the compare-contrast instruction improved in their ability to comprehend novel compare-contrast texts based on novel content.
The evidence regarding another aspect of text genre, text features, is less clear. Common features of expository texts include things like photographs with captions, tables of contents, timeless verbs, and bolded specialized vocabulary words. While a whole host of recent articles in practice-oriented reading journals advocate the teaching of expository text features (e.g., Bluestein, 2010; Fisher,
Frey, & Lapp, 2008; Kelley & Clausen-Grace, 2010), more research is needed regarding the efficacy of this approach on reading. Purcell-Gates, Duke, and
Martineau (2007) examined the role of explicit explanation of genre functions and features on second- and third-graders’ reading and writing of the genres. Neither access to explicit explanation or explicitness impacted students’ reading growth.
However, having authentic purposes for the use of reading and writing—reading to learn or investigate; writing to record and communicate—supported students’ growth in reading and writing informational text genres.
One promising approach to exploring genre-related text features with teachers and students follows a systemic functional linguistics perspective (SFL;
Schleppegrell & de Oliveira, 2006). SFL views the construction of texts and their grammars as related to contextual expressions of meaning. Schleppegrell and colleagues (Schlepegrell & de Oliveira, 2006; Fang & Schleppegrell, 2010) have applied this perspective to the teaching of content-areas as a means for helping teachers recognize the linguistic challenges of content-area texts.

Text Accessibility
A growing body of scholarship centers on the construct of text accessibility, that is, the factors that allow readers to read with accuracy, fluency, and comprehension. Scholarship around the construct has accelerated in recent years, driven at least in part by the central role played by text complexity in the recently developed and soon to be implemented Common Core Standards for English language arts (2010). Two major approaches have emerged for gauging a text’s accessibility: readability formulas (Klare, 1984) and leveling systems (e.g., Chall,
Bissex, Conrad & Harris-Sharples, 1996)—and a third, more multidimensional approach involving several more nuanced linguistic features is on the horizon (see
Graesser, McNamara, & Kulikovwich, 2011).

Readability Formulas
A variety of readability formulas are in wide use by educators and publishers as a means of selecting or guiding the development of accessible texts for particular readers. To this end, a long tradition of research has identified factors that can influence the success a particular reader may have with a

535

particular text (see Klare, 1984, for a review). The most robust of these factors— those that appear in nearly all readability formulas—are an index of word difficulty and an index of sentence complexity. For words, word length often serves as an alias for a deeper index of difficulty, for example, frequency of use in the language or conceptual complexity. For sentence complexity, sentence length often serves as an alias for a deeper index of complexity, for example, number of embedded clauses or propositions per sentence. (Chall & Dale, 1995; Fry, 1977;
Smith, Stenner, Horabin, & Smith, 1989; Spache, 1953).
Readability formulas have the benefit of being objective, highly replicable, and correlated with outcomes on reading achievement tests (Fry, 2002). The
Lexile approach (Smith et al., 1989), currently quite prevalent in schools, has the further advantage of being applied to an extremely large corpus of texts.
Moreover, it purports to place both text difficulty and student achievement (as measured by standardized tests) on the same underlying Lexile scale (Stenner,
Burdick, Sanford, & Burdick, 2006). However, as with other readability formulas, the Lexile approach fails to take into account any linguistic aspects of text beyond word frequency and sentence length, or any nonlinguistic features (e.g., illustrations or graphics) that tend to be prevalent in books, especially informational texts, for children. Moreover, like other quantitative indices of text accessibility, predicting difficulty is a less stable enterprise at the low end of the difficulty scale—where small variations in word or sentence difficulty can yield large differences in the prediction measures for very short texts (MetaMetrics,
2007; Stenner et al., 2006).

Leveling Systems
Leveling systems, which involve collective professional judgment, have been developed to address the lack of attention to more qualitative aspects of difficulty (e.g., a sense that the conceptual load of a book is high or that its engagingness is low) in readability formulas. There are two types of leveling systems: those that rely on a set of criteria applied to the text, and those that compare any given text to anchor passages that have already been assigned levels
(e.g., Chall et al., 1996). The most widely used leveling system (Fountas &
Pinnell, 1996, 1999) consists of a set of criteria that human judges, usually teachers, apply in assigning levels to texts. These criteria take into account the complexity of the language as indexed by readability formulas, as well as more qualitative factors, such as (a) the degree of connection between the text and the illustrations, (b) the arrangement of text on the page, (c) the length, repetition, or predictability of the text, and (d) the complexity of the subject matter. Leveling systems can be useful for teachers when applied strategically to the selection of books for instruction; however, in contrast to readability formulas, they rely on qualitative judgments (Fry, 2002) and thus are subject to all of the biases involved

536

in any aspect of human judgment. To date, guided reading levels have not been validated by empirical research that examines their potential to predict students’ ability to comprehend texts, and there is some concern about the reliability of leveling systems, as well as their over-application in classrooms (Dzaldov &
Peterson, 2005; Pitcher & Fang, 2007).

Multidimensional Approaches
Graesser, McNamara, and colleagues have been refining a multidimensional portfolio of linguistic indicators of text difficulty (e.g., Duran,
Bellissens, Taylor, & McNamara, 2007). The multidimensional character of their work sets it apart from most other measures. Specifically, it allows examination of the compensatory nature of linguistic factors (i.e., if you have more of X, you can get by with less of Y. For example, while narratives tend to have low coreferential cohesion (e.g., words in Sentence 1 tend not to be repeated in Sentence
2)—a situation that normally promotes difficulties in comprehension—narratives tend to have high causal and temporal cohesion (plots tend to be strung out along a causal-temporal chain), allowing readers to build a coherent mental model in the face of low co-referential cohesion. Recently, the Coh-Metrix group (Graesser et al., 2011) conducted a Principal Components Analysis of a large body of K-12 texts (the TASA corpus) varying in difficulty according to conventional formulas.
They determined that eight components accounted for 67% of the variance across texts. The eight are grouped into five theoretically meaningful indices.
1. Narrativity (genre) indexes storiness, with all its entailments of characters, events, and places. It is characterized by emphases on everyday language, familiar words, and common world knowledge.
2. Syntactic simplicity ranges from shorter, less syntactically complex, more familiar structures to longer, more complex structures with multiply embedded clauses.
3. Word concreteness reduces to something like the “imagability” of the average word in a sentence, and ranges from concrete to abstract.
4. Referential cohesion (textbase) assesses the degree of lexical/semantic overlap among sentences (how repetition and close lexical associations form explicit semantic threads).
5. Deep cohesion (situation model) is an index of the degree to which the causal, intentional, and temporal relationships among ideas are explicitly cued by connectives.
A rich body of research on accessibility notwithstanding, an even richer line of inquiry lies ahead of us, especially if we take seriously the challenge imposed by Common Core State Standards (2010) for a dramatic increase in the level of text complexity required of all students at every grade level. Two dilemmas stand out in this inquiry: (a) finding a valid and reliable way in which to

537

scale difficulty at the lower levels—where readability formulas, including lexiles, yield woefully unstable indices of difficulty, and (b) figuring out how to scaffold this increase in text complexity for a population of students who experience enormous difficulty with the current level of text challenge.

Embedding Text Level Instruction Within Disciplinary
Learning
As yet, empirical work on the instruction of disciplinary literacies is limited but growing. A body of work on content-area reading and writing does exist, but much of it is only peripherally linked to the idea of disciplinary participation; that is, it is more closely related to supporting students in reading content-area textbooks than to taking on the reading and reasoning practices of the disciplines.
In addition, in recent years there has been a preponderance of work on crossdisciplinary integration of instruction that has focused on science and literacy, particularly at the elementary level. In the main, this work had focused more on using science instruction to support comprehension of and engagement with multigenre texts (e.g., Guthrie & Ozgungor, 2002) and using literacy instruction to support science conceptual understandings and inquiry skills (e.g., Guzzetti &
Bang, 2011) than involving students in authentic forms of disciplinary reading.
Nevertheless, this work has demonstrated positive effects for the joining of science and literacy.
In particular, the Concept-Oriented Reading Instruction (CORI) project has yielded powerful evidence that connecting reading comprehension instruction to firsthand experiences in can engage students and support their reading growth.
CORI researchers have demonstrated across a series of studies with elementary students that subject-matter connections and firsthand experiences results in more motivated and strategic literacy behavior and improves reading comprehension
(Guthrie, Anderson, Alao, & Rinehart, 1999; Guthrie et al., 2006; Guthrie et al.,
2004). In addition, Romance and Vitale (1992, 2001) have consistently demonstrated positive effects for the In-Depth Expanded Applications of Science
(IDEAS) model, which replaces the time allocated for traditional literacy instruction with a 2-hour block of science instruction that includes attention to discussion, reading, concept mapping, and journal writing. Romance and Vitale have documented through a long program of research that IDEAS students across the elementary grades outpace students receiving their regular language arts and science programs on nationally normed standardized measures of science knowledge and reading comprehension.
Very recently, a few studies have taken a more disciplinary approach to reading and writing, with promising results. For example, De La Paz and Felton
(2010) taught a historical reasoning strategy to 11th-grade students as a way of supporting their ability to write argumentative texts on historical topics. The

538

researchers conceptualized reading and writing as closely linked, and part of the instructional intervention, therefore, involved reading historical texts using reading practices that reflect those of historians as described by Wineburg (1991). For example, students engaged in sourcing by using a set of “Consider the Author” questions, such as, “What do you know about the author? When was the document written? and How does the author’s viewpoint have an effect on his argument?”
(De La Paz & Felton, 2010, p. 182). Students who participated in this instruction produced historical writing that was better elaborated and more persuasive than students in a control group.
Greenleaf et al. (2011) examined the effects of the Reading Apprenticeship instructional framework on high school science students’ reading and content understanding. The Reading Apprenticeship framework is intended to help teachers integrate disciplinary literacy practices into high school science teaching.
While the approach is dedicated, in part, to helping student crack the code of content-area textbooks, it also focuses on the ways that scientists make sense of science texts and use them to inform investigations. The Reading Apprenticeship model is focused on the “metacognitive conversation,” in which teachers model and discuss how to read science texts, why people read science texts in these ways, and the content of the texts. The students use complex science texts as they engage in the intellectual work of science inquiry. Greenleaf et al. found that students in the Reading Apprenticeship classrooms made greater gains on standardized tests in reading and biology than students in control classrooms.
At the elementary level, the Seeds of Science/Roots of Reading
(Seeds/Roots) program has demonstrated positive effects for an integrated approach on students’ reading, writing, and science understanding. The
Seeds/Roots model positions literacy in support of students’ involvement in science inquiry. Students read to deepen their involvement in investigations in ways that are similar to the ways that scientists read, that is, to inform their inquiry methods and situate their investigations within the work of other scientists
(Cervetti & Barber, 2008). Across two studies with second through fifth grade students (Cervetti, Barber, Dorph, Pearson, & Goldschmidt, in press; Wang &
Herman, 2005), the Seeds/Roots approach has shown advantages for treatment students on measures of science understanding, science vocabulary acquisition, and science writing, with a less consistent advantage for reading comprehension.

539

Examining Policy Contexts for
Reading Research and Practice
As we asserted at the outset of this chapter, reading pedagogy has always been contested territory, with one version or another of a debate between progressive versus traditional, or child-centered versus curriculum-centered, or transmissionist versus constructivist perspectives (some would call them ideologies) playing out in virtually every decade of the past 100 plus years—7 score if one goes back Horace Mann and the Common School movement in
Boston (Mathews, 1967). Whether it is labeled as analytic versus synthetic phonics (as it was in the 1890s), phonics versus look-say (as it was around the time of WWI), code versus meaning (as it was in the 1960s), skills versus whole language (as it was in the 1980s), or common standards for all versus the accommodation of individual differences (which is what it really has come down to in the NCLB era), protagonists line up on one side or another of the line in the sand, on the lookout for cracks in the curricular framework or flaws in the pedagogical tools of their adversaries. Both sides seek the moral high ground of doing what is right and best for children and their families.
Over the past decade the debate was intensified because the pedagogical argument became completely entangled with a parallel debate about the character of research required to validate the efficacy of instructional approaches (see
Pearson, 2004, for an elaborate account of the issues and policy initiatives surrounding the research debate). The science card was first played at the federal level in the second term of the Clinton administration when the bill authorizing the
Reading Excellence Act (REA), which allocated $240,000,000 for staff development to promote reading reform, required that both state and local applications for funding base their programs on research that meets scientifically rigorous standards. The scientifically rigorous phrase was a late entry; in all but the penultimate version of the bill, the phrase was reliable, replicable research, which had been interpreted as a code word for experimental research. In last days of the Clinton administration, the term scientifically rigorous research was morphed into scientifically based reading research, and defined as research that meets four standards. It must:
1. Employ systematic, empirical methods that draw on observation or experiment. 2. Involve rigorous data analyses that are adequate to test the stated hypotheses and justify the general conclusions drawn.
3. Rely on measurements or observational methods that provide valid data across evaluators and observers and across multiple measurements and observations. 540

4. Have been accepted by a peer-reviewed journal or approved by a panel of independent experts through a comparably rigorous, objective, and scientific review.
As of early 1999, “phonics bills” (bills mandating either the use of phonics materials or some sort of teacher training to acquaint teachers with knowledge of the English sound-symbol system and its use in teaching) had been passed or were pending in 36 states (e.g., United States Department of Education, 1999). The No
Child Left Behind legislation of 2002 made this goal of “evidence-based practice” even more explicit, with the phrase scientifically based reading research appearing more than 110 times in the Reading First portion of this act reauthorizing Title I. The NCLB made this goal of evidence-based practice even more explicit, with the phrase scientifically based reading research appearing more than 110 times in the Reading First portion of this act reauthorizing Title I.
The problem in reading is that there was a natural confounding between the curricular position people took (whether they came down on highly structured approaches such as systematic early phonics or highly constructivist approaches such as literature-based reading or whole language) and their preferred epistemological and methodological approach to research. Constructivists tended to opt for ethnographic or other forms of qualitative research whereas those who favored systematic approaches tended toward experimental or at least quantitative approaches (see Pearson, 2004, or Pearson, 2007, for more elaborate accounts of the phenomenon). The net effect of this confounding has been, as it seems to be also in national politics in the early years of this decade of the teens, to close off the conversation between folks on either side of the line in the sand, with few opportunities for open debate and even fewer for rapprochement.
What has become difficult in this volatile context is to argue for the complementarity of methods and epistemologies in ways in which they exist in other fields in the basic sciences. Even the foremost research design methodology of the past half century, Donald Campbell recognized this need, arguing in 1984 that qualitative and quantitative approaches must be complementary:
To rule out plausible rival hypotheses we need situation-specific wisdom. The lack of this knowledge (whether it be called ethnography, program history, or gossip) makes us incompetent estimators of program impacts, turning out conclusions that are not only wrong, but are often wrong in socially destructive ways. . . .
There is the mistaken belief that quantitative measures replace qualitative knowledge. Instead, qualitative knowing is absolutely essential as a prerequisite for quantification in any science.
Without competence at the qualitative level, one’s computer printout is misleading or meaningless. (pp. 141–142)

We suspect that reading is not the only curricular landscape in which these tensions and these curricular/epistemological/methodological confounds are being

541

enacted. In fact, based on a chapter that one of us wrote with a mathematics education colleague (Schoenfeld & Pearson, 2009), we know that mathematics is as contested as reading on these matters. So we hope that a rapprochement can occur on the research front across several areas of scholarship so that we can disentangle our curricular from our epistemological perspectives and methodological preferences. That would be a good step in determining on what we do and do not agree. And that might even lead to a situation in which we can see the virtue in complementary and converging approaches to examining and solving the vexing educational problems that plague all research scholars regardless of their preferences for understanding and conducting research (Shavelson &Towne,
2002).

References
Adams, M. J. (1990). Beginning to read: Thinking and learning about print. Cambridge, MA:
MIT Press.
Alexander, P. A. (2003). Profiling the developing reader: The interplay of knowledge, interest, and strategic processing. In C. M. Fairbanks, J. Worthy, B. Maloch, J. V. Hoffman, & D.
L. Schallert (Eds.), The 52nd yearbook of the national reading conference (pp. 47–65).
Oak Creek, WI: National Reading Conference.
Alexander, P. A. (2005). The path to competence: A lifespan developmental perspective on
Reading. Journal of Literacy Research, 37, 413–436.
Alliance for Excellent Education. (2010). Policy brief: The federal role in confronting the crisis in adolescent literacy. Washington, DC: Alliance for Excellent Education. www.all4ed.org/files/FedRoleConfrontingAdolLit.pdf Anderson, J. R. (1983). The architecture of cognition. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University
Press.
Anderson, R. C., & Freebody, P. (1981). Vocabulary knowledge. In J. T. Guthrie (Ed.),
Comprehension and teaching: Research review (pp. 71–117). Newark, DE: International
Reading Association.
Anderson, R. C., & Pearson, P. D. (1984). A schema-theoretic view of basic processes in reading. In P. D. Pearson, R. Barr, M. L. Kamil, & P. Mosenthal (Eds.), Handbook of reading research. White Plains, NY: Longman.
Anderson, R., Hiebert, E., Scott, J., & Wilkinson, I. (1984). Becoming a nation of readers.
Champaign-Urbana, IL: Center for the Study of Reading.
Badian, N. A. (2001). Phonological and orthographic processing: Their roles in reading prediction. Annals of Dyslexia, 51, 179–202.
Baker, L., & Brown, A. L. (1984). Metacognitive skills and reading. In R. Barr, M. L. Kamil, &
P. Mosenthal (Eds.), Handbook of reading research (pp. 353–394). New York, NY:
Longman.

542

Baumann, J. F., & Bergeron, B. (1993). Story-map instruction using children’s literature: Effects on first graders’ comprehension of central narrative elements. Journal of Reading
Behavior, 25, 407–437.
Baumann, J. F., & Graves, M. F. (2010). What is academic vocabulary? Journal of Adolescent &
Adult Literacy, 54, 4–12.
Baumann, J. F., Bradley, B., Edwards, E. C., Font, G., & Hruby, G. (2000, December). Teaching generalizable vocabulary-learning strategies: A critical review of the literature. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the National Reading Conference, Scottsdale, AZ.
Baumann, J. F., Edwards, E. C., Boland, E. M., Olejnik, S., & Kame’enui, E. J. (2003).
Vocabulary tricks: Effects of instruction in morphology and context on fifth-grade students’ ability to derive and infer word meanings. American Educational Research
Journal, 40, 447–494.
Beck, I. L., & McKeown, M. G. (2006). Improving comprehension with questioning the author.
New York, NY: Scholastic.
Beck, I. L., McKeown, M. G., & Kucan, L. (2002). Bringing words to life: Robust vocabulary instruction. New York, NY: Guilford Press.
Beck, I., & McKeown, M. (1990). Conditions of vocabulary acquisition. In R. Barr, M. L.
Kamil, P. B. Mosenthal, & P. D. Pearson (Eds.), Handbook of reading research (Vol. 2, pp. 789–814). New York, NY: Longman.
Beck, I., Perfetti, C., & McKeown, M. (1982). The effects of long-term vocabulary instruction on lexical access and reading comprehension. Journal of Educational Psychology, 74,
506–521.
Berne, J. I., & Clark, K. F. (2008). Focusing literature discussion groups on comprehension strategies. Reading Teacher, 62, 74–79.
Best, R. M., Floyd, R. G., & McNamara, D. S. (2008). Differential competencies contributing to children’s comprehension of narrative and expository texts. Reading Psychology, 29,
137–164.
Billings, L., & Fitzgerald, J. (2002). Dialogic discussion and the Paideia seminar. American
Educational Research Journal, 39(4), 907–941.
Blaiklock, K. E. (2004). The importance of letter knowledge in the relationship between phonological awareness and reading. Journal of Research in Reading, 27, 36–57.
Block, C., & Pressley, M. (Eds.). (2002). Comprehension instruction: Research-based best practices. New York, NY: Guilford Press.
Bluestein, N. A. (2010). Unlocking text features for determining importance in expository text:
A strategy for struggling readers. Reading Teacher, 63, 597–600.
Bond, G. L., & Dykstra, R. (1967). The cooperative research program in first-grade reading instruction. Reading Research Quarterly, 32, 348–427.
Bowers, P. N., & Kirby, J. R. (2010). Effects of morphological instruction on vocabulary acquisition. Reading and Writing, 23, 515–537

543

Brown, A., Pressley, M., Van Meter, P., & Schuder, T. (l996). A quasi-experimental validation of transactional strategies instruction with low-achieving second grade readers. Journal of Educational Psychology, 88, 28–37.
Campbell, D. T. (1984). Can we be scientific in applied social science? In R. E. Conner, D. G.
Altman, & C. Jackson (Eds.), Evaluation studies: Review annual (Vol. 9, pp. 85–97).
Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.
Carlisle, J. F. (2007). Fostering morphological processing, vocabulary development, and reading comprehension. In R. K. Wagner, A. E. Muse, & K. R. Tannenbaum (Eds.), Vocabulary acquisition: Implications for reading comprehension (pp. 78–103). New York, NY:
Guilford Press.
Carlisle, J. F., (2010). Effects of instruction in morphological awareness on literacy achievement:
An integrative review. Reading Research Quarterly, 45, 464–487.
Carnegie Corporation of New York’s Council on Advancing Adolescent Literacy. (2010). Time to Act. New York, NY: Carnegie.
Cervetti, G. N., & Barber, J. (2008). Text in hands-on science. In E. H. Hiebert & M. Sailors
(Eds.), Finding the right texts: What works for beginning and struggling readers (pp. 89–
108). New York, NY: Guilford Press.
Cervetti, G. N., Barber, J., Dorph, R., Pearson, P. D., & Goldschmidt, P. G. (in press). The impact of an integrated approach to science and literacy in elementary school classrooms.
Journal of Research in Science Teaching.
Chall, J. S. (1967). Learning to read: The great debate. New York, NY: McGraw-Hill.
Chall, J. S., & Dale, E. (1995). Readability revisited. Cambridge, MA: Brookline.
Chall, J. S., Bissex, G. L., Conrad, S. S., & Harris-Sharples, S. (1996). Qualitative assessment of text difficulty: A practical guide for teachers and writers. Brookline, MA: Brookline
Books.
Chinn, C. A., Anderson, R. C., & Waggoner, M. A. (2001). Patterns of discourse in two kinds of literature discussion. Reading Research Quarterly, 36, 378–411.
Clark, A. M., Anderson, R. C., Archodidou, A., Nguyen-Jahiel, K., Kuo, L. -J., & Kim, I. (2003).
Collaborative reasoning: Expanding ways for children to talk and think in the classroom.
Educational Psychology Review, 15, 181–198.
Coltheart, M. (2005). Modeling reading: The dual-route approach. In M. J. Snowling & C.
Hulme (Eds.), The science of reading: A handbook (pp. 6–23). Oxford, UK: Blackwell.
Coltheart, M., Rastle, K., Perry, C., Langdon, R., & Ziegler, J. (2001). DRC: A dual route cascaded model of visual word recognition and reading aloud. Psychological Review,
108, 204–256.
Common Core Standards. (2010). Common core state standards for English language arts.
Washington, DC: Council of Chief State School Officers & National Governor’s
Association.
Cope, B., & Kalantzis, M. (Eds.). (1993). The powers of literacy: A genre approach to teaching writing. Pittsburgh, PA: University of Pittsburgh Press.

544

Cote, N., Goldman, S. R., & Saul, F. U. (1998). Student making sense of informational text:
Relations between processing and representation. Discourse Processes, 25, 1–53.
Coxhead, A. (2000). A new academic word list. TESOL Quarterly, 34, 213–238.
Cunningham, A. E., & Stanovich, K. E. (1997). Early reading acquisition and its relation to reading experience and ability 10 years later. Developmental Psychology, 33, 934–945.
Cunningham, A. E., Nathan, R. G., & Raher, K. S. (2011). Orthographic processing in models of word recognition. In M. L. Kamil, P. D. Pearson, E. B. Moje, & P. P. Afflerbach (Eds.),
Handbook of Reading Research (Vol. 4, pp. 259-285). New York, NY: Routledge.
Davis, C. J. (2010). The spatial coding model of visual word identification. Psychological
Review, 117, 713–758.
De La Paz, S., & Felton, M. K. (2010). Reading and writing from multiple source documents in history: Effects of strategy instruction with low to average high school writers.
Contemporary Educational Psychology, 35, 174–192.
Dole, J., Duffy, G. G., Roehler, L. R., & Pearson, P. D. (1991). Moving from the old to the new:
Research on reading comprehension instruction. Review of Educational Research, 61,
239–264.
Duke, N. D., Pearson, P. D., Strachan, S. L., & Billman, A. K. (2011). Essential elements of fostering and teaching reading comprehension. In S. J. Samuels & A. E. Farstrup (Eds.),
What research has to say about reading instruction (4th ed., pp. 51–93). Newark, DE:
International Reading Association
Duke, N. K., & Bennett-Armistead, S. A. (2003). Reading & writing informational text in the primary grades: Research-based practices. New York, NY: Scholastic.
Duke, N. K., & Pearson, P. D. (2001). How can I help children improve their comprehension? In
Teaching every child to read: Frequently-asked questions. Ann Arbor, MI: Center for the
Improvement of Early Reading Achievement.
Duke, N., & Pearson, P. D. (2002). Effective practices for developing reading comprehension. In
A. Farstrup & J. Samuels (Eds.), What research has to say about reading instruction (3rd ed., pp. 205–242). Newark, DE: International Reading Association.
Duran, N., Bellissens, C., Taylor, R., & McNamara, D. (2007). Qualifying text difficulty with automated indices of cohesion and semantics. Paper presented at the 25th Annual
Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society, Austin, TX.
Durrell, D. D., & Murphy, H. A. (1953). The auditory discrimination factor in reading readiness and reading disability. Education, 73, 556–560.
Dzaldov, B. S., & Peterson, S. (2005). Book leveling and readers. Reading Teacher, 59(3), 222–
229.
Eeds, M., & Wells, D. (1989). Grand conversations: An exploration of meaning construction in literature study groups. Research in the Teaching of English, 23(1), 4–29.
Ehri, L. (2005a). Learning to read words: Theory, findings and issues. Scientific Studies of
Reading, 9, 167–188

545

Ehri, L. (2005b). Development of sight word reading: Phases and findings. In M. Snowling & C.
Hulme (Eds.), The science of reading: A handbook (pp. 135–154). Oxford, UK:
Blackwell.
Ehri, L., Nunes, S., Willows, D., Schuster, B., Yaghoub-Zadeh, Z., & Shanahan, T. (2001).
Phonemic awareness instruction helps children learn to read: Evidence from the national reading panel’s meta-analysis. Reading Research Quarterly, 36, 250–287.
Englert, C. S., & Hiebert, E. H. (1984). Children’s sensitivity to expository text structure.
Journal of Educational Psychology, 76, 65–74.
Fang, Z., & Schleppegrell, M. J. (2010). Disciplinary literacies across content areas: Supporting secondary reading through functional language analysis. Journal of Adolescent and Adult
Literacy, 53, 587–597.
Fisher, D., & Frey, N. (2008). Word wise and content rich: Five essential steps to teaching academic vocabulary. Portsmouth, NH: Heinemann.
Fisher, D., Frey, N., & Lapp, D. (2008). Shared readings: Modeling comprehension, vocabulary, text structures, and text features for older readers. Reading Teacher, 61, 548–556.
Fountas, I. C., & Pinnell, G. S. (1996). Guided reading: Good first teaching for all children.
Portsmouth, NH: Heinemann.
Fountas, I. C., & Pinnell, G. S. (1999). Matching books to readers: Using leveled books in guided reading, K–3. Portsmouth, NH: Heinemann.
Fry, E. (1977). Fry’s readability graph: Clarification, validity, and extension to level 17. Journal of Reading, 21(3), 242–252.
Fry, E. (2002). Readability versus leveling. Reading Teacher, 56(3), 286–291.
Gambrell, L. B., & Morrow, L. M. (1996). Creating motivating contexts for literacy learning. In
L. Baker, P. Afflerbach, & D. Reinking (Eds.), Developing engaged readers in home and school communities. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Gerrig, R. J., & O’Brien, E. J. (2005). The scope of memory-based processing. Discourse
Processes, 39, 225–242.
Gerrig, R., & McKoon, G. (1998). The readiness is all: The functionality of memory-based text processing. Discourse Processes, 26, 67–86.
Goldenberg, C. (1993). Instructional conversations: Promoting comprehension through discussion. Reading Teacher, 46, 316–326.
Goldman, S. R. (1997). Learning from text: Reflections on the past and suggestions for the future. Discourse Processes, 23, 357–398.
Goldman, S. R., Graesser, A. C., & van den Broek, P. (1999). Narrative comprehension, causality, and coherence: Essays in honor of Tom Trabasso. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Gough, P. B., & Tunmer, W. E. (1986). Decoding, reading, and reading disability. Remedial and
Special Education, 7, 6–10.
Graesser, A. C., Gernsbacher, M. A., & Goldman, S. R. (1997). Cognition. In T. A. van Dijk
(Ed.), Discourse: A multidisciplinary introduction (pp. 292–319). London, UK: Sage.

546

Graesser, A. C., McNamara, D. S., & Kulikovich, J. M. (2011). Coh-Metrix: Providing multilevel analysis of text characteristics. Educational Researcher, 40(5), 223–234.
Graves, M. F. (2000). A vocabulary program to compliment and bolster a middle grade comprehension program. In B. M. Taylor, M. F. Graves, & P. van den Broek (Eds.),
Reading for meaning: Fostering comprehension in the middle grades (pp. 116–135).
Newark, DE: International Reading Association.
Great Books Foundation. (1987). An introduction to shared inquiry. Chicago, IL: Great Books
Foundation.
Greenleaf, C. L., Litman, C., Handon, T. L., Rosen, R., Boscardin, C. K., Herman, J.,… Jones,
B. (2011). Integrating literacy and science in biology: Teaching and learning impacts of reading apprenticeship professional development. American Educational Research
Journal, 48, 647–717.
Guthrie, J. T., & Ozgungor, S. (2002). Instructional contexts for reading engagement. In C. C.
Block & M. Pressley (Eds.), Comprehension instruction: Research-based best practices
(pp. 275–288). New York, NY: Guilford Press.
Guthrie, J. T., Anderson, E., Alao, S., & Rinehart, J. (1999). Influences of concept-oriented reading instruction on strategy use and conceptual learning from text. Elementary School
Journal, 99, 343–366.
Guthrie, J. T., Wigfield, A., Barbosa, P., Perencevich, K. C., Taboada, A., David, M. H.,…
Tonks, S. (2004). Increasing reading comprehension and engagement through conceptoriented reading instruction. Journal of Educational Psychology, 96, 403–423.
Guthrie, J. T., Wigfield, A., Humenick, N. M., Perencevich, K. C., Taboada, A., & Barbosa, P.
(2006). Influences of stimulating tasks on reading motivation and comprehension.
Journal of Educational Research, 99, 232–245.
Guzzetti, B., & Bang, E. (2011). The influence of literacy-based science instruction on adolescents’ interest, participation, and achievement in science. Literacy Research and
Instruction, 50, 46–67.
Hagiliassis, N., Pratt, C., & Johnston, M. (2006). Orthographic and phonological processes inreading. Reading and Writing, 19, 235–263.
Halliday, M. A. K. (1961). Categories of the theory of grammar. Word 17. Reprinted in Bertil
Malmberg (Ed.) 1972, Readings in modern linguistics (pp. 157–208). Stockholm,
Sweden: Läromedelsförlagen-Mouton.
Harmon, J. M., Wood, K. D., & Hedrick, W. B. (2008). Vocabulary instruction in middle and secondary content classrooms: Understandings and direction from research. In A. E.
Farstrup & S. J. Samuels (Eds.), What research has to say about vocabulary instruction
(pp. 150–181). Newark, DE: International Reading Association.
Hart, B., & Risley, T. (1995). Meaningful differences in everyday parenting and intellectual development in young American children. Baltimore, MD: Brookes.
Heller, R., & Greenleaf, C. L. (2007). Literacy instruction in the content areas: Getting to the core of middle and high school improvement. Washington, DC: Alliance for Excellent
Education.

547

Hiebert, E. H., & Lubliner, S. (2008). The nature, learning, and instruction of general academic vocabulary. In A. E. Farstrup & S. J. Samuels (Eds.), What research has to say about vocabulary instruction (pp. 106–129). Newark, DE: International Reading Association.
Hyland, K., & Tse, P. (2009). Academic lexis and disciplinary practice: Corpus evidence for specificity. International Journal of English Studies, 9, 111–130.
Johnson-Laird, P. N. (1983). Mental models: Towards a cognitive science of language, inference, and consciousness. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
Kamil, M., Pearson, P. D., Moje, E., & Afflerbach, P. (Eds.). (2011). Handbook of reading research (Vol. 4). London, UK: Routledge.
Kelley, M. J., & Clausen-Grace, N. (2010). Guiding students through expository text with text feature walks. Reading Teacher, 64, 191–195.
Kendeou, P., Savage, R., & van den Broek, P. (2009). Revisiting the simple view of reading.
British Journal of Educational Psychology, 79, 353–370.
Kendeou, P., van den Broek, P., White, M. J., & Lynch, J. (2009). Predicting reading comprehension in early elementary school: The independent contributions of oral language and code-related skills. Journal of Educational Psychology, 4, 765–778.
Kendeou, P., van den Broek, P., White, M., & Lynch, J. (2007). Preschool and early elementary comprehension: Skill development and strategy interventions. In D. S. McNamara (Ed.),
Reading comprehension strategies: Theories, interventions, and technologies (pp. 27–
45). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Kintsch, W. (1988). The use of knowledge in discourse processing: A construction-integration model. Psychological Review, 95, 163–182.
Kintsch, W. (1998). Comprehension: A paradigm for cognition. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge
University Press.
Kintsch, W. (2004). The construction-integration model of text comprehension and its implications for instruction. In R. B. Ruddell & N. J. Unrau (Eds.), Theoretical models and processes of reading. Newark, DE: International Reading Association.
Kintsch, W., & Kintsch, E. (2005). Comprehension. In S. G. Paris & S. A. Stahl (Eds.),
Children’s reading comprehension and assessment (pp. 71–92). Mahwah, NJ: CIERA.
Klare, G. (1984). Readability. In P. D. Pearson, R. Barr, M. L. Kamil, & P. Mosenthal (Eds.),
Handbook of reading research (pp. 681–744). New York, NY: Longman.
Klinger, J. K., & Vaughn, S. (1999). Promoting reading comprehension, content learning, and
English acquisition through collaborative strategic reading (CSR). Reading Teacher, 52,
738–747.
Kong, A., & Pearson, D. (2003). The road to participation: The construction of a literacy practice in a learning community of linguistically diverse learners. Research in the Teaching of
English, 38, 85–124.
Lee, C. D., & Spratley, A. (2010). Reading in the disciplines: The challenges of adolescent literacy. New York, NY: Carnegie.

548

Leinhardt, G., & Young, K. M. (1996). Two texts, three readers: Distance and expertise in reading history. Cognition and Instruction, 14, 441–486.
Linderholm, T., Virtue, S., van den Broek, P., & Tzeng, Y. (2004). Fluctuations in the availability of information during reading: Capturing cognitive processes using the landscape model. Discourse Processes, 37, 165-186.
Lupker, S. J. (2005). Visual word recognition. In M. J. Snowling & C. Hulme (Eds.), The science of reading: A handbook (pp. 39–60). Oxford, UK: Blackwell.
Marulis, L. M., & Neuman, S. B. (2010). The effects of vocabulary intervention on young children’s word learning: A meta-analysis. Review of Educational Research, 80, 300–
335.
Mathews, M. (1967). Teaching to read, historically considered. Chicago, IL: University of
Chicago Press.
McKeown, M. G., Beck, I. L., &. Blake, R. G. K. (2009). Rethinking comprehension instruction:
Comparing strategies and content instructional approaches. Reading Research Quarterly,
44, 218–253.
McKeown, M. G., Beck, I. L., Omanson, R. C., & Pople, M. T. (1985). Some effects of the nature and frequency of vocabulary instruction on the knowledge and use of words.
Reading Research Quarterly, 20, 522–535.
McNamara, T. P., Miller, D. L., & Bransford, J. D. (1991). Mental models and reading comprehension. In R. Barr, M. L. Kamil, P. Mosenthal, & P. D. Pearson (Eds.),
Handbook of reading research (Vol. 2, pp. 490–511). White Plains, NY: Longman.
MetaMetrics (2007). The Lexile framework for reading technical report. Durham, NC:
MetaMetrics.
Meyer, B. J. F., Brandt, D. M., & Bluth, G. J. (1980). Use of top-level structure in text: Key for reading comprehension of ninth-grade students. Reading Research Quarterly, 16, 72–
103.
Moje, E. B. (2008). Foregrounding the disciplines in secondary literacy teaching and learning: A call for change. Journal of Adolescent and Adult Literacy, 52(2), 96-107.
Moran, J., Ferdig, R. E., Pearson, P. D., Wardrop, J., & Blomeyer, R. L. (2008). Technology and reading performance in the middle-school grades: A meta-analysis with recommendations for policy and practice. Journal of Literacy Research, 40, 6–58.
Morrow, L. M. (1984). Reading stories to young children: Effects of story structure and traditional questioning strategies on comprehension. Journal of Reading Behavior, 16,
273–288.
Morrow, L. M. (1996). Motivating reading and writing in diverse classrooms: Social and physical contexts in a literature-based program (NCTE Research Report no. 28). Urbana,
IL: National Council of Teachers of English.
Moss, B. (2005). Making a case and a place for effective content area literacy instruction in the elementary grades. Reading Teacher, 59, 46–55.

549

Murphy, P. K., Wilkinson, I. A. G., Soter, A. O., Hennessey, M. N., & Alexander, J. F. (2009).
Examining the effects of classroom discussion on students’ high-level comprehension of text: A meta-analysis. Journal of Educational Psychology, 101, 740–764.
Nagy, W. E. (1988). Teaching vocabulary to improve reading comprehension. Urbana, IL:
NCTE.
Nagy, W. E., & Anderson, R. C. (1984). How many words are there in printed school English?
Reading Research Quarterly, 19, 304–330.
Nagy, W. E., Anderson, R. C., & Herman, P. A. (1987). Learning word meanings from context during normal reading. American educational Research Journal, 24, 237–270.
Nagy, W. E., Herman, P. A., & Anderson, R. C. (1985). Learning words from context. Reading
Research Quarterly, 20, 233–253.
National Early Literacy Panel. (2008). Developing early literacy: A report of the national early literacy panel. Washington, DC: National Institute for Literacy. www.nifl.gov/earlychildhood/NELP/NELPreport.html National Institute of Child Health and Human Development (NICHD). (2000). Report of the national reading panel: Teaching children to read: An evidence-based assessment of the scientific research literature on reading and its implications for reading instruction.
(NIH Publication No. 00–4769). Washington, DC: U.S. Government Printing Office.
Oakhill, J., Cain, K., & Bryant, P. (2003). The dissociation of word reading and text comprehension: evidence for component skills. Language and Cognitive Processes, 18,
443–468.
Osborne, J. (2010). Arguing to learn in science: The role of collaborative, critical discourse.
Science, 328, 463–466.
Paivio, A. (1990). Mental representations: A dual coding approach (2nd ed.). New York, NY:
Oxford University Press.
Palincsar, A. S., & Magnusson, S. J. (2001). The interplay of firsthand and text-based investigations to model and support the development of scientific knowledge and reasoning. In S. Carver & D. Klahr (Eds.), Cognition and instruction: Twenty-five years of progress (pp. 151–194). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Palincsar, A., & Brown, A. (1984). Reciprocal teaching of comprehension-fostering and comprehension-monitoring activities. Cognition and Instruction, 1, 117–175.
Paribakht, T. S., & Wesche, M. B. (1997). Vocabulary enhancement activities and reading for meaning in second language vocabulary development. In J. Coady & T. Huckin (Eds.),
Second language vocabulary acquisition: A rationale for pedagogy (pp. 174–200). New
York, NY: Cambridge University Press.
Paris, S. G., Lipson, M. Y., & Wixson, K. K. (1994). Becoming a strategic reader. In R. B.
Ruddell, M. R. Ruddell, & H. Singer (Eds.), Theoretical models and processes of reading
(4th ed., pp. 788–810). Newark, DE: International Reading Association.
Pearson, P. D. (2004). The reading wars: The politics of reading research and policy—1988 through 2003. Educational Policy, 18(1), 216–252.

550

Pearson, P. D. (2007). An historical analysis of the impact of educational research on policy and practice: Reading as an illustrative case. In D. W. Rowe, R. T.Jiménez, D. L. Compton,
D. K. Dickinson, Y. Kim, K. M. Leander, & V. J. Risko (Eds.), 56th yearbook of the national reading conference (pp. 14–40). Oak Creek WI: National Reading Conference.
Pearson, P. D. (2011). Toward the next generation of comprehension instruction: A Coda. In H.
Daniels (Ed.), Comprehension going forward: Where we are, what’s next. Portsmouth,
NH: Heinemann.
Pearson, P. D., & Camparell, K. (1981). Comprehension of text structures. In J. Guthrie (Ed.),
Comprehension and teaching (pp. 27–54). Newark, DE: International Reading
Association.
Pearson, P. D., & Fielding, L. (1991). Comprehension instruction. In R. Barr, M. L. Kamil, P. B.
Mosenthal, & P. D. Pearson (Eds.), Handbook of reading research (Vol. 2, pp. 815–860).
White Plains, NY: Longman.
Pearson, P. D., & Hiebert, E. (2010). National reports in literacy: Building a scientific base for practice and policy. Educational Researcher, 39(4), 286–294.
Perfetti, C. A. (1999). Comprehending written language: A blueprint of the reader. In C. Brown
& P. Hagoort (Eds.), The neurocognition of language (pp. 167–208). New York, NY:
Oxford University Press.
Pitcher, B., & Fang, Z. (2007). Can we trust levelled texts? An examination of their reliability and quality from a linguistic perspective. Literacy, 41(1), 43–51.
Pressley, M. (2000). What should comprehension instruction be the instruction of? In M. L.
Kamil, P. B. Mosenthal, P. D. Pearson, & R. Barr (Eds.), Handbook of reading research
(Vol. 3, pp. 545–561). Mahwah NJ: Erlbaum.
Pressley, M., Brown, R., El-Dinary, P., & Afflerbach, P. (1995). The comprehension instruction that students need: Instruction fostering constructively responsive reading. Learning
Disabilities Research & Practice, 10, 215–224.
Purcell-Gates, V., Duke, N. K., & Martineau, J. A. (2007). Learning to read and write genrespecific text: Roles of authentic experience and explicit teaching. Reading Research
Quarterly, 42, 8–45.
Rand Reading Study Group. (2002). Reading for understanding: Toward a research and development program in reading comprehension. Santa Monica, CA: Rand.
Raphael, T. E., & McMahon, S. I. (1994). Book club: An alternative framework for reading instruction. Reading Teacher, 48, 102–116.
Raphael, T. E., Florio-Ruane, S., & George, M. (2001). Book club plus: A conceptual framework to organize literacy instruction. Ann Arbor, MI: Center for the Improvement of Early
Reading Achievement Report. www.ciera.org/library/reports/inquiry-3/3–015/3–015.htm
Rapp, D.N., van den Broek, P., McMaster, K.L., Kendeou, P., & Espin, C.A. (2007). Higherorder comprehension processes in struggling readers: A perspective for research and intervention. Scientific Studies of Reading, 11, 289-312.

551

Reutzel, D. R., Smith, J. A., & Fawson, P. C. (2005). An evaluation of two approaches for teaching reading comprehension strategies in the primary years using science information texts. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 20, 276–305.
Richgels, D. J., McGee, L. M., Lomax, R. G., & Sheard, C. (1987). Awareness of four text structures: Effects in recall of expository text. Reading Research Quarterly, 22, 177–196.
Romance, N. R., & Vitale, M. R. (1992). A curriculum strategy that expands time for in-depth elementary science instruction by using science-based reading strategies: Effects of a year-long study in grade four. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 29(6), 545–554.
Romance, N. R., & Vitale, M. R. (2001). Implementing an in-depth expanded science model in elementary schools: Multi-year findings, research issues, and policy implications.
International Journal of Science Education, 23(4), 272–304.
Rosenshine, B., & Meister, C. (1994). Reciprocal teaching: A review of research. Review of
Educational Research, 64, 479–530.
Ruddell, R. B., & Unrau, N. J. (Eds.). (2004). Theoretical models and processes of reading (5th ed.). Newark, DE: International Reading Association.
Rueda, R., Goldenberg, C., & Gallimore, R. (1992). Rating instructional conversations: A guide.
(Research Report EPR4.) National Center for Research on Cultural Diversity and Second
Language Learning. www.ncela.gwu.edu/pubs/ncrcdsll/epr4.htm
Rumelhart, D. E., & Ortony, A. (1977). The representation of knowledge in memory. In R. C.
Anderson, R. J. Spiro, & W. E. Montague (Eds.), Schooling and the acquisition of knowledge. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Sadoski, M., & Paivio, A. (2001). Imagery and text: A dual coding theory of reading and writing. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Schleppegrell, M., & de Olivereira, L. C. (2006). An integrated language and content approach for history teachers. Journal of English for Academic Purposes, 5, 254–268.
Schoenfeld, A. H., & Pearson, P. D. (2009). The reading and math wars. In G. Sykes, B.
Schneider, & D. Plank (Eds.), Handbook of education policy research (pp. 560–580).
New York, NY: Routledge.
Shanahan, T., & Shanahan, C. (2008). Teaching disciplinary literacy to adolescents: Rethinking content-area literacy. Harvard Educational Review, 78, 40–59.
Shanahan, T., Callison, K., Carriere, C., Duke, N. K., Pearson, P. D., Schatschneider, C., &
Torgesen, J. (2010). Improving reading comprehension in kindergarten through 3rd grade: A practice guide (NCEE 2010–4038). Washington, DC: National Center for
Education Evaluation and Regional Assistance, Institute of Education Sciences, U.S.
Department of Education. whatworks.ed.gov/publications/practiceguides
Share, D. L. (1995). Phonological recoding and self-teaching: Sine qua non of readingacquisition. Cognition, 55, 151–218.
Sharp, A. M. (1985). Philosophy for children and the development of ethical values. Early Child
Development and Care, 197, 45–55.

552

Shavelson, R. J., & Towne, L. (Eds.). (2002). Scientific research in education. Washington, DC:
National Academy Press.
Short, K. G., & Pierce, K. M. (1990). Talking about books: Creating literature communities.
Portsmouth, NH: Heinemann.
Smith, D., Stenner, A. J., Horabin, I., & Smith, M. (1989). The Lexile scale in theory and practice: Final report. Washington, DC: MetaMetrics.
Snow, C. E. (2010). Academic language and the challenge of reading for learning about science.
Science, 328, 450–452.
Snow, C. E., Lawrence, J. F., & White, C. (2009). Generating knowledge of academic language among urban middle school students. Journal of Research on Educational Effectiveness,
2, 325–344.
Snow, C., Burns, M., & Griffin, P. (1998). Preventing reading difficulties in young children.
Washington, DC: National Academy Press.
Snowling, M., & Hulme, C. (Eds). (2005). The science of reading: A handbook. Oxford, UK:
Blackwell.
Soter, A. O., Wilkinson, I. A. G., Murphy, P. K., Rudge, L., Reninger, K., & Edwards, M.
(2008). What the discourse tells us: Talk and indicators of high-level comprehension.
International Journal Educational Research, 47, 372–391.
Spache, G. (1953). A new readability formula for primary-grade reading materials. Elementary
School Journal, 53(7), 410–413.
Stahl, S. A., & Fairbanks, M. M. (1986). The effects of vocabulary instruction: A model-based meta-analysis. Review of Educational Research, 56, 72–110.
Stahl, S. A., & Stahl, K. A. (2004). Word wizards all!: Teaching word meanings in preschool and primary education. In J. F. Baumann & E. J. Kame’enui (Eds.), Vocabulary instruction.
New York, NY: Guilford Press.
Stenner, A. J., Burdick, H., Sanford, E. E., & Burdick, D. S. (2006). How accurate are Lexile text measures? Journal of Applied Measurement, 7, 307–322.
Swanson, H. L., Trainin, G., Necoechea, D. M., & Hammill, D. D. (2003). Rapid naming, phonological awareness, and reading: A meta-analysis of the correlation evidence.
Review of Educational Research, 73, 407–440.
Taylor, B. M., & Samuels, S. J. (1983). Children’s use of text structure in the recall of expository material. American Educational Research Journal, 20, 517–528.
Taylor, B. M., Pearson, P. D., Peterson, D. S., & Rodriguez, M. C. (2003). Reading growth in high-poverty classrooms: The influence of teacher practices that encourage cognitive engagement in literacy learning. Elementary School Journal, 104, 3–28.
Tharp, R. G., & Gallimore, R. (1991). The instructional conversation: Teaching and learning in social activity. Washington, DC: Center for Applied Linguistics.
Thurlow, R. & van den Broek, P. (1997). Automaticity and inference generation during reading.
The Reading and Writing Quarterly, 13, 165-181.

553

Tierney, R. J., & Cunningham, J. W. (1984). In P. D. Pearson, R. Barr, M. L. Kamil, & P.
Mosenthal (Eds.), Handbook of reading research (pp. 609–655). New York, NY:
Longman.
Trabasso, T., Secco, T., & van den Broek, P. (1985). Causal cohesion and story cohesion. In H.
Mandl, N. L. Stein, & T. Trabasso (Eds.), Learning and comprehension of text (pp. 83–
111). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Trabasso, T., Secco, T., & van den Broek, P. W. (1984). Causal cohesion and story coherence.
In H. Mandl, N. L. Stein & T. Trabasso (Eds.), Learning and comprehension of text
(pp.83-111). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
United States Department of Education. (1999). Reading excellence act state competitive grant program: Non-regulatory guidance for state applicants – March 9, 1999. www2.ed.gov/inits/FY99/REAguidance/sectionB.html van den Broek, P. (1994). Comprehension and memory of narrative texts: Inferences and coherence. In M. A. Gernsbacher (Ed.), Handbook of Psycholinguistics (pp. 539-588).
New York: Academic Press. van den Broek, P. (2010). Using texts in science education: Cognitive processes and knowledge representation. Science, 328, 453–456. van den Broek, P. (April 23, 2010). Using Texts in Science Education: Cognitive Processes and
Knowledge Representation. Science, 328, 453-456. van den Broek, P. W. (1990). The Causal Inference Maker: Towards a process model of inference generation in text comprehension. In D. A. Balota, G. B. Flores d’Arcais, K.
Rayner (Eds.), Comprehension processes in reading (pp. 423-446). Hillsdale, NJ:
Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. van den Broek, P., Bohn-Gettler, C., Kendeou, P., Carlson, S., & White, M.J. (2011). When a reader meets a text: The role of standards of coherence in reading comprehension. In
M.T. McCrudden, J.P. Magliano, & G. Schraw (Eds.), Text Relevance and Learning from
Text (pp. 123-140). Greenwich, CT: Information Age Publishing. van den Broek, P., Fletcher, C. R., & Risden, K. (1993). Investigations of inferential processes in reading: A theoretical and methodological integration. Discourse Processes, 16, 169–
180.
van den Broek, P., Lorch, R. F. Jr, Linderholm, T., & Gustafson, M. (2001). The effects of readers’ goals on inference generation and memory for texts. Memory & Cognition, 29,
1081-1087.
van den Broek, P., White, M. J., Kendeou, P., & Carlson, S. (2009). Reading between the lines:
Developmental and individual differences in cognitive processes in reading comprehension. In R. Wagner, C. Schatschneider, & C. Phythian-Sence (Eds.), Beyond
Decoding: The Behavioral and Biological Foundations of Reading Comprehension
(pp.107-123), Guilford Publications, NY. van den Broek, P., Young, M., Tzeng, Y., & Linderholm, T. (1998/2004). The landscape model of reading: Inferences and the on-line construction of a memory representation. In H. van
Oostendorp & S. R. Goldman (Eds.), The construction of mental representations during reading (pp. 71–98). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. (Reprinted in R. B. Ruddell & N. J. Unrau

554

[Eds.], Theoretical models and processes of reading [pp. 1244–1269]. Newark, NJ:
International Reading Association)
Van Orden, G. C., & Kloos, H. (2005). The science of reading: A handbook. In M. S. Snowling
& C. Hulme (Eds.), The question of phonology and reading (pp. 61, 78). Oxford, UK:
Blackwell.
Vitale, M. R., & Romance, N. R. (2007). A knowledge-based framework for unifying contentarea reading comprehension and reading comprehension strategies. In D. S. McNamara
(Ed.), Reading comprehension strategies: Theories, interventions, and technologies (pp.
73–106). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Vygotsky, L. (1934/1987). Thinking and speech. New York, NY: Plenum.
Wang, J., & Herman, J. (2005). Evaluation of seeds of science/roots of reading project: Shoreline science and terrarium investigations. Los Angeles, CA: CRESST, UCLA http://scienceandliteracy.org/research/efficacy_studies Whitehurst, G., & Lonigan, C. (1998). Child development and emergent literacy. Child
Development, 69, 848–872.
Wilkinson. I. A. G., & Son. E. H. (2011). A dialogic turn in research on learning and teaching to comprehend. In M. L. Kamil, P. D. Pearson, E. Moje, & P. Afflerbach (Eds.), Handbook of reading research (Vol. 4, pp. 359–387). New York, NY: Routledge.
Williams, J. P., Hall, K. M., Lauer, K. D., Stafford, K. B., De Sisto, L. A., & deCani, J. S.
(2005). Expository text comprehension in the primary grade classroom. Journal of
Educational Psychology, 97, 538–550.
Williams, J. P., Nubla-Kung, A. M., Pollini, S., Stafford, K. B., Garcia, A., & Snyder, A. E.
(2007). Teaching cause-effect text structure through social studies content to at-risk second graders. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 40, 111–120.
Wineburg, S. S. (1991). On the reading of historical texts: Notes on the breach between school and the academy. American Educational Research Journal, 28, 495–519.

Similar Documents

Premium Essay

Psychology

...Define psychology: Psychology involves the study of human and behavior, experiences and the mind. Psychology can be implemented using slews of techniques and is analyzed from different positions. Psychology has had a dynamic history; it has evolved over decades and has grown into a dependable science. More former understandings of the philosophies and ideas played a prominent role by outlining contemporary concepts. “Psychology has a long past, but only a short history.”(Ebbinhuas, 1973) Wilhem Wundt’s’ work was the blueprint of modern psychology in 1879; he instituted the laboratory dedicated to psychology in Leipzig, Germany. Wundt had a particular interest about human behavior, emotions. His techniques had special emphasis on experimental evidence that endorsed explanatory theory. Today psychology is thought of as the scientific study of human behavior and mental process. However, this was not always the case. The soul of man was the leading interest of philosophers’, followed by the mind and conscious experience, and finally observable behavior. Problems arouse with the affiliation of body and soul. It remained unsolved by philosophers because it was founded on delusive dualism and involved a separate study of physical and spiritual development. Afterwards, the spiritual panorama was supplanted by a broader word “mind”. Modern psychology is disinterested in the study of mind, rather, the mental processes have deputized mind. The “mind approach” in psychology was rejected...

Words: 458 - Pages: 2

Premium Essay

Psychology

...The article that I chose is titled “Overview of Social Psychology; Sociology & Related Fields”. It begins with a detailed definition of Psychology; stating Psychology or the study of human behavior is an amply sized, expansive field that encompasses several distinct factions, each of which offers its own unique brand of specialization (Gibson, 1994). Throughout the articles overview it speaks on the different branches of psychology, counseling psychology, clinical psychology and developmental psychology. This article is a review of existing research and psychological experiments. I can tell that the information in this article is based from existing research due to the article making reference several times to different classic studies, insights and experiments that were conducted to implement the study of “Social Psychology”. The main purpose of this article was to conduct classic social psychological studies including Stanley Milgram’s experiment on obedience; the study also examined the Bystander Effect and also addressed cognitive dissonance to better understand how one’s behavior changes through a social psychological lens. Stanley Milgram’s Experiment on Obedience was titled “Behavioral Study of Obedience”, and was designed to uncover material encompassing the darker side of human behavior. Throughout the Milgram experiment they used fake actors who assumed various roles within the framework of a man made scenario to receive psychological insight towards human obedience...

Words: 569 - Pages: 3

Premium Essay

Psychology

...Biological Psychology Name Institution What scientific evidence exists to support the proposition that other animals are capable of language? 1. Student Linda Salazar According to Linda Salazar, promising results from the right on time creature language studies were tossed after disclosures of methodological issues were revealed. This lent confirmation to an necessary clarification of the advancement of language, championed by a few language specialists and analysts, who guarantee that semantic attitudes are remarkably human (Kalat, 2013). Later research, deliberately intended to overcome procedural blemishes has refueled the level headed discussion, by giving confirmation to the transformative clarification of language improvement, proposing that a graduation of linguistic abilities exists in human and nonhuman primates. Analysts and commentators alike are mindful in assessing these later studies (Toates, 2001). Numerous are concentrating on the inquiries that emerged from the dialect investigations of nonhuman primates, concerning cognizance and knowledge, and their relationship to phonetic/linguistic capacity. As supported by the book, Biological Psychology by James W. Kalat, human language is a repercussion of knowledge and its blemishes with this hypothesis (Garrett, 2009). Language should likewise have developed after some time as particular mind component. In light of acoustic instruments that control conduct, intentional and automatic frameworks, I accept...

Words: 1557 - Pages: 7

Premium Essay

Psychology

...Psychology Perspectives 5 main psychology approaches: * Behaviorist Perspective * Psychodynamic Perspective * Cognitive Psychology * Biological Psychology * Evolutionary Psychology Behaviorism – different from most other approaches because they view people (and animals) as controlled by their environment and specifically that we are the result of what we have learned from our environment. * Concerned with how environmental factors (stimuli) affect observable behavior (response)) 2 main processes learn from environment: * Classical conditioning * Operant conditioning Behaviorism- scientific study of observable behavior working on basis that behavior can reduced to learned S-R (Stimulus-Response) units. Classical conditioning- studied by Russian psychologist Ivan Pavlov. Looking into natural reflexes and neutral stimuli he managed to conditioning dogs to salivate to the sound of a bell through repeated associated of the sound of the bell and food. * Principles of CC applied in many therapies include systematic desensitization for phobias and aversion therapy. Operant conditioning- B.F. Skinner investigated voluntary and involuntary behavior. Skinner felt that some behavior could be explained by the person’s motive. Therefore behavior occurs for a reason, 3 main behavior shaping techniques: * Positive reinforcement * Negative reinforcement * Punishment Psychodynamic Perspective Sigmund Freud- many expressions from our daily...

Words: 1685 - Pages: 7

Premium Essay

Psychology

...Positive Psychology An Introduction Martin E. P. Seligman Mihaly Csikszentmihalyi A science of positive subjective experience, positive individual traits, and positive institutions promises to improve quali~.' of life and prevent the pathologies that arise when life is barren and meaningless. The exclusive focus on pathology that has dominated so much of our discipline results in a model of the human being lacking the positive features that make life worth living. Hope, wisdom, creativity, future mindedness, courage, spirituality, responsibility, and perseverance are ignored or explained as transformations of more authentic negative impulses. The 15 articles in this millennial issue of the American Psychologist discuss such issues as what enables happiness, the effects of autonomy and self-regulation, how optimism and hope affect health, what constitutes wisdom, and how talent and creativity come to fruition. The authors outline a framework .['or a science of positive psychology, point to gaps in our knowledge, and predict that the next century will see a science and profession that will come to understand and build the factors that allow individuals, communities, and societies to flourish. E ntering a new millennium, Americans face a historical choice. Left alone on the pinnacle of economic and political leadership, the United States can continue to increase its material wealth while ignoring the human needs of its people and those of the rest of the ...

Words: 8985 - Pages: 36

Premium Essay

Psychology

...Women in Psychology When one thinks about the field of psychology and those great names that made such great contributions to the field like; Freud and Skinner to name a few come to mind, one thinks of those men who made a important finding about psychology, nonetheless those names hold great meaning to the field, but along the way there have been several influential women who have also made such great contributions to the field, may who have been theorist, pioneers and counselors. Among some of these women there is a name that stands out the most, her name is Anna Freud. This paper will discuss Anna Freud’s background, her theoretical perspective and the contributions she was able to provide to the field of psychology. Anna Freud was born on Vienna, Austria on December 3, 1895, the youngest of six children of Sigmund Freud and Martha Freud. Anna Freud had a special bond with her father, more than with her mother or any siblings; she started reading her father’s work at the age of 15. Her relationship with her siblings wasn’t quite the best; in fact Anna refers to her 2 years older sister Sophie as her rival. Anna felt relieved when Sophie went out and got marry, as the relationship wouldn’t be as competitive as it had been most of their lives. Anna gained a special interest in the field of psychology such as her father and in a time were men had made significant contributions to the field of psychology, then comes a little a girl who since a very early age learned to...

Words: 1404 - Pages: 6

Premium Essay

Psychology

...biological basses of behavior and mental events Key question: how does the nervous system produce behavior in mental events Assumptions: there must be a relationship between nervous system behavior and mental events. What is biopsychology? * The branch of psychology that studies the relationship between nervous system activity and behavior and mental events. * A new branch of neurosciences. * Makes use of the findings discovered by other branches of psychology. Brain consists of neurons and glia. Other branches of the neurosciences are 1. Biological psychiatry is interested in treatment of biological treatments of 2. Developmental neurobiology is interested in how the nervous system changes and develops 3. Neuroanatomy is interested in the structure of the nervous system and hw the different areas are connected to one and another 4. Neurochemistry is interested in how the neurons work and how they communicate with one another 5. Neurobiology is interested the structure and the mechanics of the nervous system. Biological psychology focuses on behavior (the observative behavior and the unobservative behavior) Biopsychology has six branches 1. Physiological psychology the study of neural mechanism of behavior through the manipulation of the nervous system in controlled experiments either surgically, electrically or chemically. 2. Psychopharmacology concerned with the effects of drugs on brain and behavior. Research may involve...

Words: 516 - Pages: 3

Premium Essay

Psychology

...psychology C H A P T E R 1 Psychology and the Challenges of Life ❚ ❚ ❚ ❚ ❚ ❚ ❚ ❚ ❚ ❚ ❚ ❚ ❚ ❚ ❚ ❚ ❚ ❚ ❚ ❚ ❚ ❚ ❚ ❚ ❚ ❚ ❚ ❚ ❚ ❚ ❚ ❚ ❚ ❚ ❚ OUTLINE Did you know that… Module 1.1: Psychology and Adjustment Module 1.2: Human Diversity and Adjustment Module 1.3: Critical Thinking and Adjustment Module 1.4: How Psychologists Study Adjustment Module 1.5: Psychology in Daily Life: Becoming a Successful Student CHAPTER REVIEW RECITE! RECITE! RECITE! REFLECT REFLECT REFLECT YOUR PERSONAL JOURNAL Copyright © 2010 John Wiley & Sons, Inc. Did you know that… ❚ Multitasking while studying can damage your grades? (p. 6) ❚ Genetics influences many psychological traits and even our preferences for different types of occupations? (p. 8) ❚ White, Euro-Americans are now a minority in the nation’s most populous state? (p. 12) ❚ Women were once not permitted to attend college in the United States? (p. 13) ❚ You could survey a million voters and still not predict the outcome of a presidential election accurately? (p. 23) ❚ You are more likely to eventually get a divorce if you live together with your future spouse before getting married? (p. 27) ❚ Cramming for a test is not more likely to earn you a good grade than spacing your study sessions? (p. 33) Oleg Prikhodko/iStockphoto ❚ ❚ ❚ ❚ ❚ ❚ ❚ ❚ ❚ ❚ B eth, 22, a fourth-year chemistry major, has been accepted into medical school in Boston. She wants to do cancer research, but this goal means another seven or eight years at...

Words: 9594 - Pages: 39

Premium Essay

Psychology

...Clinical Psychology Final Project Virginia Berry June 15, 2011 PS380 Professor Husk Kaplan University Aspergers Syndrome is one of the most common autism spectrum disorders, (Hunt & Marshall, 2006). There are many symptoms that are common with children with Aspergers syndrome. One symptom may be showing intense obsession with specific subject like cars, bugs or the weather. Speaking in a monotone voice or speaking fast is another symptom of Asperger’s (MayoClinic, 2011). There are approximately 26 to 36 out of 10,000 school-age children that are diagnosed with Asperger’s syndrome (Ehlers & Gillberg, 1993). Like classic autism, there is no known factor that causes Asperger’s Syndrome. Though many parents believe there is link autism by childhood vaccinations, there is no documented proof that the two are related (Downs, 2011). Treatment for Asperger’s Syndrome can be reviewed with the four major schools of psychotherapy; structuralism, behaviorism, Gestalt psychology and psychoanalysis. Structuralism is to analyze the mind in simple components and find how the components fit into a complex form, (Britannica, 2011). Behaviorism is a theory that all behaviors are learned through conditioning. Gestalt psychology theory is based off perception, (Britannica, 2011). Psychoanalysis is based on observation of individuals that are unaware of their behavior and emotions, (APSAA, 2011). Individuals living with Asperger’s Syndrome may not benefit from all four major schools...

Words: 1078 - Pages: 5

Premium Essay

Psychology

...OF PSYCOLOGY* 1.GENERAL PSYCHOLOGY- field of psychology that explais the underline principles of human behavior-the study of how and why people behave this way or that way. 2.COMPARATIVE PSYCHOLOGY- traits of the behavior & mental processes of the different species. 3.DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY-is concern itself with the study of human behavior in all its aspects of growth & development. 4.CONSUMER PSYCHOLOGY-is concern with the investigation of the varied facts of marketing & buying behavior affects of advertising studies of mass media & other problems arising from the relationshipbetween the buyer & the seller. 5.EXPERIMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY-deals with observation and experiments in a psychological laboratory. 6.DIFFERENTIAL PSYCHOLOGY-is a branch of study which deals investigates differences & similarities existing among individuals groups and races. 7. PSYCHOLOGY-applied in medicine it concerned with the treatment of mental diseases. 8.CLINICAL PSYCHOLOGICAL-pertains to the diagnosis of psycho therapy of the milder behavior disorder. 9.EDUCATIONAL PSYCHOLOGY-deals with learning motivation & other subject in the actual eductional process. 10.SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY-is the study of the behavior of groups & individuals in their relationship to other group. 11.PERSONALITY PSYCHOLOGY-study between personality and behavior. 12.ADOLESCENE PSYCHOLOGY- study of behavior of man from poverty to later life approximately from 12-20 yrs.old. 13.SENESCENT PSYCHOLOGY- is the scientific of human...

Words: 566 - Pages: 3

Premium Essay

Psychology

...1—What Is Psychology? Learning Goals 1. 2. 3. 4. 5. Explain what psychology is and how it developed. Describe six contemporary approaches to psychology. Describe two movements that reflect a positive approach to psychology. Evaluate careers and areas of specialization in psychology. Apply some strategies that will help you succeed in psychology. After studying Chapter 1, you will be able to: Define psychology. Describe the influence that philosophy, biology, and physiology had on the beginnings of psychology as a science. Compare the two early scientific approaches in psychology: structuralism and functionalism. Describe the focus of each of the six contemporary approaches to psychology. Describe the positive psychology movement, and discuss why this movement recently emerged in psychology. Discuss career opportunities in psychology. Profile the main areas of specialization in psychology. Say how studying habits may be optimized. Understand how to be a critical thinker. CHAPTER 1: OUTLINE Psychology is a science dedicated to the study of behavior and mental processes. In this chapter you are introduced to the history of this science, a variety of contemporary perspectives in psychology, the positive psychology movement, and an overview of psychology-related careers. At the end of the chapter, the reader learns about the most effective methods of studying and learning. There are three concepts important to the definition of psychology: science...

Words: 11126 - Pages: 45

Premium Essay

Psychology

...Page 1 PSYCHOLOGY IN THE CONTEXT OF HEALTH AND SOCIAL CARE • What is ‘psychology’ and why is it so important in the context of health and social care? • What do we mean by ‘health’ and why is psychology central to the effective delivery of health and social care? • What are the main approaches to psychological thinking and research? • Who are psychologists and what do they contribute to the promotion of health and well-being? Introduction This chapter emphasizes the importance of psychology in the context of health and social care. For many years, psychology and the other social sciences were viewed by the medical profession as ‘soft sciences’, interesting but unimportant. With the advent of research into the links between physical and mental states in the late twentieth and early twenty-first centuries it is now possible to demonstrate that psychology can make a fundamental difference to physical as well as mental health. In this chapter, we explore the nature of psychology and its relevance to health and social care. We outline the different schools of thought and methods of inquiry in psychology. We seek to distinguish between psychology as an academic discipline and popular notions of psychology, and identify professionals whose practice is mainly concerned with the application of psychology. In order to show how psychology can be applied to health and social care, we introduce a family scenario whose characters appear in examples throughout the book. What is psychology...

Words: 6889 - Pages: 28

Free Essay

Psychology

...eBook Psychology Myers 7th Edition PDF at Our Huge Library PSYCHOLOGY MYERS 7TH EDITION PDF ==> Download: PSYCHOLOGY MYERS 7TH EDITION PDF PSYCHOLOGY MYERS 7TH EDITION PDF - Are you searching for Psychology Myers 7th Edition Books? Now, you will be happy that at this time Psychology Myers 7th Edition PDF is available at our online library. With our complete resources, you could find Psychology Myers 7th Edition PDF or just found any kind of Books for your readings everyday. You could find and download any of books you like and save it into your disk without any problem at all. We also provide a lot of books, user manual, or guidebook that related to Psychology Myers 7th Edition PDF, such as; - Experimental Psychology Seventh Edition - Social Psychology Myers 7th Edition free Ebooks download - EXPLORING PSYCHOLOGY 7th Edition in Modules David Myers - Psychology By David G Myers 7th Edition Online Pdf - Exploring Psychology 7th Edition David Myers Learning - Psychology Myers 10th Edition mybooklibraryCom - EXPLORING PSYCHOLOGY Personality Model of Mind The - EXPLORING PSYCHOLOGY 7th Edition David Myers Emotions - EXPLORING PSYCHOLOGY 7th Edition in Modules David Myers - Psychology David Myers 10th Edition Study Guide - Part 1 Psychology 8 Edition by David Myers Prologue and - myers exploring psychology memory chapter Bing - Experimental Psychology Seventh Edition - myers introduction to psychology Bing - psychology myers 7th edition Bing PDF Downloads Blog - Health Psychology 7th...

Words: 1662 - Pages: 7

Premium Essay

Psychology

...Social psychology is the scientific field that seeks to understand the nature and causes of the individual behavior and thought in social situation. Social psychology investigates the ways in which our thoughts, feelings, and actions are influenced by the social environments in which we live. Social interactions help to shape who we are and how we act in different situations. The factors affecting social interaction fall into five major categories. They are the actions and characteristics of others, basic cognitive processes, ecological variables, cultural context and biological factors. The Cognitive processes such as perception, memory and inferences play a key role on the understanding and behavior of every individual in the society. Reactions to certain situations by an individual strongly depend on the memories of others past behaviors and the inferences an individual formed about these behaviors. If anybody wants to clearly understand the causes of others behavior in a social situation it is a must that one should understand what went on in the thinking pattern and understanding process of those people when they behaved in a particular social situation. For example, if your friend sets an appointment with you in a particular time. You are waiting for him at a particular point in a particular time, if he comes late what would be your reaction. In such a situation, cognitive process plays a crucial role in the social behavior and social thoughts of every individual...

Words: 319 - Pages: 2

Premium Essay

Psychology

...Cognitive Psychology Definition Paper Jennifer Flynn PSY/360 October 27, 2014 Terry Blackmon Cognitive Psychology Definition Paper Cognitive psychology is the study among psychology that discovers the internal mental processes by encompassing learning, memory, problem solving, perception, thought or language (Galotti, 2014). While still relatively new comparatively as a formal branch of psychology, its roots extend back to Descartes who sought a way to explain how the mind worked, proposing the analogy of a “hydraulic system of nerve function” (Willingham, 2007, p. 26) after he observed animated statues in Saint-Germain-en-Laye. It has been the relentless pursuit of not only how did the mind work but also what exactly constituted the mind that eventually led the foundations of cognitive theory. As psychologists examined how mental processes produced behavior, it was evident a different approach would be needed. The school of thought that arises from tactic is called Cognitivism and is also interested in how people mentally represent information processing (Galotti, 2014). According to ScienceDaily (2014), Wilhelm Wundt, the Gestalt psychology of Max Wertheimer, Wolfgang Kohler and Kurt Koffka, and Jean Piaget was the foundations in this work. However, they presented the theory or segments that articulate children’s cognitive development and the two styles that cognitive psychologist use to realize, detect, and solve problems. These two approaches are psychophysical...

Words: 1038 - Pages: 5