Free Essay

The Controversy About a Possible Relationship Between Mobile Phone Use and Cancer

In:

Submitted By kakashiryu
Words 12276
Pages 50
Review
The Controversy about a Possible Relationship between Mobile Phone Use and Cancer
Michael Kundi
Institute of Environmental Health, Medical University of Vienna, Vienna, Austria

oBjective: During the last decade, mobile phone use increased to almost 100% prevalence in many countries of the world. Evidence for potential health hazards accumulated in parallel by epidemiologic investigations has raised controversies about the appropriate interpretation and the degree of bias and confounding responsible for reduced or increased risk estimates. data sources: Overall, I identified 33 epidemiologic studies in the peer-reviewed literature, most of which (25) were about brain tumors. Two groups have collected data for ≥ 10 years of mobile phone use: Hardell and colleagues from Sweden and the Interphone group, an international consortium from 13 countries coordinated by the International Agency for Research on Cancer. data synthesis: Combined odds ratios (95% confidence intervals) from these studies for glioma, acoustic neuroma, and meningioma were 1.5 (1.2–1.8); 1.3 (0.95–1.9); and 1.1 (0.8–1.4), respectively. conclusions: Methodologic considerations revealed that three important conditions for epidemiologic studies to detect an increased risk are not met: a) no evidence-based exposure metric is available; b) the observed duration of mobile phone use is generally still too low; c) no evidence-based selection of end points among the grossly different types of neoplasias is possible because of lack of etiologic hypotheses. Concerning risk estimates, selection bias, misclassification bias, and effects of the disease on mobile phone use could have reduced estimates, and recall bias may have led to spuriously increased risks. The overall evidence speaks in favor of an increased risk, but its magnitude cannot be assessed at present because of insufficient information on long-term use. key words: acoustic neuroma, brain tumors, causation, epidemiology, glioma, meningioma, mobile phones, risk assessment. Environ Health Perspect 117:316–324 (2009). doi:10.1289/ ehp.11902 available via http://dx.doi.org/ [Online 26 September 2008]

I will first give a brief overview of the results of epidemiologic investigations of mobile phone use and tumors of the head region. I will then address methodologic problems associated with these studies and, by application of the pragmatic approach proposed earlier (Kundi 2006), discuss whether epidemiologic evidence supports a causal interpretation of an association between mobile phone use and brain tumors.

Overview of Epidemiologic Studies
Table 1 presents an overview of results of epidemiologic studies on the association between brain tumors and mobile phone use. Other end points include salivary gland tumors (Hardell et al. 2004; Lönn et al. 2006; Sadetzki et al. 2008), uveal melanoma (Stang et al. 2001), non-Hodgkin lymphoma (Hardell et al. 2005c; Linet et al. 2006), facial nerve tumors (Warren et al. 2003), and testicular cancer (Hardell et al. 2007), but for these diseases the database is insufficient to date. Except for the early cohort study from the United States (Dreyer et al. 1999) that was stopped by the court after 1 year of follow-up and the Danish retrospective cohort study (Johansen et al. 2001; Schüz et al. 2006b), all investigations have been case–control studies. The “Overall results” from 25 epidemiologic studies listed in Table 1 do not demonstrate support for an increased risk. Only a few risk estimates are significantly elevated, and some are even significantly reduced. Risk estimates for longer duration of use are higher, on average, than overall estimates, and estimates for ipsilateral mobile phone use (i.e., use of the mobile phone on the same side where the tumor occurred), where available, tend to be even higher. Implications of these findings are discussed below.

Because of the enormous increase in mobile phone use starting in the mid-1990s and reaching almost 100% prevalence in many countries worldwide by now, concerns have been raised that even small risks for developing chronic diseases such as cancer from mobile phone use may have substantial impact on public health. In fact, never before in history has any device of comparative prevalent use been associated with such high exposure to high-frequency electromagnetic fields (EMFs). (In addition, exposure to extremely low-frequency magnetic fields occurs from battery discharge.) Although from the perspective of the thermal effects paradigm, the rate of energy deposition in tissues of the mobile phone user is below levels considered harmful, there has been debate since the 1930s that tissue heating may not be the only relevant effect elicited by exposure to high-frequency EMFs; thus, there may be a relevant risk that has not been established yet because of the scarcity of exposure conditions that are comparable across a significant proportion of the population. For all the diverse high-frequency exposures occurring in environmental and occupational settings ranging from long-waves [a type of amplitude modulation (AM) broadcasting with carrier frequencies between 153 and 280 kHz] to radar waves, only a few long-term observational studies have been published (for an overview, see Ahlbom et al. 2004; Krewski et al. 2001; Kundi et al. 2004). Similarly scarce are long-term animal studies of low-level exposures

in the pre–mobile phone era. Hence, at the time mobile phones were introduced, there were insufficient data to decide about health risks from low-level exposures, but the prevailing opinion that no relevant health effects occur at exposures below guideline levels led to the expectation that mobile phones are safe. However, the exponential growth of mobile phone use came as a surprise to the industry as well as to scientists involved in EMF risk assessment. Therefore, the existing gaps in knowledge should be addressed in both experimental as well as epidemiologic investigations focusing on exposures occurring in mobile telecommunications. Since the mid-1990s, many epidemiologic studies of mobile phone use have been conducted worldwide, most of them focusing on tumors of the head region. Despite the growing database, the concerns have not been settled and a controversy still exists about possible adverse health effects. Although some may be inclined to attribute the ongoing debate to the enormous economic impact of modern telecommunication during the past decade, this debate also mirrors fundamental difficulties and a permissible range of interpretation under circumstances of insufficient knowledge. Despite this state of affairs, not all arguments that have been put forward in this controversy are valid. In the following sections I will concentrate on epidemiologic findings and their interpretation, although experimental work deserves similar critical appraisal. volume Methodologic Problems
Although a number of established study designs in epidemiology have been successfully applied in thousands of investigations in the past 50 years, perhaps few epidemiologists are fully aware of the conditions necessary
Address correspondence to M. Kundi, Institute of Environmental Health, Medical University of Vienna, Kinderspitalgasse 15, 1095 Vienna, Austria. Telephone: 43 1 4277 64726. Fax: 43 1 4277 9647. E-mail: michael.kundi@meduniwien.ac.at The author declares he has no competing financial interests. Received 7 July 2008; accepted 26 September 2008.

316

117 | number 3 | March 2009 • Environmental Health Perspectives

Mobile phones and cancer

to detect an existing risk by application of these methodologies. Analytical epidemiology intends to estimate the risk as a function of exposure to an agent by application of one of three classical study types: cross-sectional, case–control, and cohort study designs. Theoretically, all three types are capable of detecting an existing risk under ideal conditions but differ in their sensitivity to the effects of extraneous and confounding factors. For all study designs, it has to be assumed that exposure to the agent can be assessed with a certain sensitivity and specificity. In the case of mobile phone use, the appropriate exposure metric is unknown. Absorption of electromagnetic energy in the body of the user depends on technical features of the phone and the network, as well as on anatomical features and habits of use. At first glance, this situation does not seem to be much different from, for example, an exposure to an air pollutant that also varies in time and space and which internal dose will depend on physiologic conditions. The problem with mobile telephones is much more profound. What aspect of the complex exposure condition described by “mobile phone use” could be responsible for an effect? It is obvious that given a certain indicator of mobile phone use, such as years of regular use, cumulative number of calls, cumulative hours of use, or cumulative absorption of electromagnetic energy in a certain area of the body, there are indefinitely many exposure conditions that are equivalent under the chosen metric and hence induce an equivalence relation in the space of exposure patterns that cannot be assessed as to its suitability with respect to any outcome measure without a sound mechanistic theory. Another essential problem is related to the long induction periods and latencies of tumors in the head and neck region. Mobile phone use that was insignificant before the mid-1990s could not be studied with respect to its influence during induction period because, in almost all users, malignant transformation has likely occurred long before exposure to mobile phones commenced. Although an influence during the initiation phase cannot be ruled out, many researchers have expressed the opinion that if there is an effect at all, it is an effect on tumor promotion or progression (Johansen et al. 2001; Muscat et al. 2000; Stang et al. 2001). If this is actually the case, several scenarios have to be considered. For example, in a summary of acoustic neuroma, Mohyuddin et al. (2003) found that tumor growth is exhibited in 48–70% of patients, stable tumor volumes in 27–50%, and involution in 2–10%. If mobile phone use influences growth rate, this influence could result in either restarting growth in stable tumors, an increase of growth rate in growing tumors, or an inhibition of involution. The net result would be an earlier onset of symptoms and an earlier diagnosis.
Environmental Health Perspectives

If there is an effect of the specified type, the age incidence function in the exposed population would be shifted compared with that of the unexposed population by a fraction of the exposure duration, as long as duration of use is short compared with the natural history of the disease. Given that the age incidence function has a positive slope, this shift is equivalent to an increased incidence in the exposed population for any given age. For simplicity, assume that the duration and effect of exposure is constant across all age groups. Taking the slope for the age incidence function for brain tumors as 0.04 [on the log incidence scale (Wrensch et al. 2002)], the estimated incidence of brain cancer at any given age would be proportional to exp(age × 0.04) in the population as a whole. If the exposed segment of the population has an age incidence function shifted by 2 years, the estimated incidence at any given age among the exposed would be proportional to exp[(age + 2) × 0.04], resulting in an OR of exp(2 × 0.04) = 1.08 for mobile phone use. Note that the result is independent of the exposure prevalence and depends only on the shift of the age incidence function and its slope. Given the lower number of mobile phone users at an older age, the expected ORs from studies with short exposure durations (and hence small shifts in the age incidence function) are too small to be detected with acceptable power. The third fundamental problem is related to the vast diversity of tumor types to be considered. The World Health Organization differentiates about 50 types of brain tumors (Kleihues and Cavenee 2000); there are more than a dozen different histologic salivary gland tumors, and so forth. Furthermore, in recent years molecular histopathology revealed many differences within certain types of tumors. For example, there are at least two clusters of glioblastoma multiforme, the most frequent malignant brain tumor in adults: one expresses loss of heterozygosity on chromosome 17p with mutation of the p53 tumor suppressor gene, and the other cluster is characterized by an amplification of the epidermal growth factor receptor oncogene (Schwartzbaum et al. 2006). Is it possible that all of these diverse types of tumors respond uniformly to mobile phone radiation? We cannot hope to extract sensitive types by epidemiologic investigations because of the small numbers for each distinct type. In summary, the three most important conditions for epidemiology to arrive at a conclusion concerning a potential risk of an agent are as follows: • It must be possible to measure (at least by a surrogate marker) the component of the agent that is related to the risk. • For agents that promote the disease in question, duration of exposure must be a substantial fraction of the history of the disease.

• There must be good a priori reason to select specific types of diseases that are sufficiently homogenous to support the assumption of more or less uniform etiology. None of these preconditions are met in the study of mobile phone use and cancer. As a consequence, even substantially increased risks might go undetected, and evidence will tend to be equivocal.

Do Mobile Phones Cause Brain Tumors?
To assess a possible causal relationship between an agent and cancer, a pragmatic dialog approach has been delineated (Kundi 2006). According to this procedure, epidemiologic evidence must be assessed concerning four aspects: temporal relation, association, environmental, and population equivalence. If there are no valid counter arguments against the evidence for an association, this suffices for a verdict of causation. If epidemiologic evidence is insufficient, other evidence that increases or decreases confidence in a causal relationship could be included to come to a conclusion. Temporal relation. Assessment of temporal relation is not a trivial problem. It is impossible to define the point in time when a brain tumor started. Before a tumor can be diagnosed, which in the case of brain tumors occurs either by coincidence, if it is detected by imaging techniques applied for other reasons, or because of symptoms produced by the growing cell mass, the tumor was present for many years or even decades. For meningioma, average induction periods of about 20–40 years have been calculated in adults, based on observations of patients exposed to ionizing radiation (Umansky et al. 2008). For acoustic neuroma, slow growth, with an average volume doubling time of about 1.7 years, suggests similar induction periods (Mohyuddin et al. 2003). For glioma, case reports (Kranzinger et al. 2001) and long-term follow-up after childhood radiation therapy of tinea capitis (Sadetzki et al. 2005) also suggest induction periods of decades. Considering a temporal relationship between exposure and the diverse steps of brain tumor development, the following four phases may be differentiated: 1) exposure commenced before the first step of malignant transformation; 2) exposure started during the induction phase, which could itself last for several years; 3) onset of exposure occurred during the non invasive growth phase; and 4) exposure started during final (autonomous) growth. In cases 1 and 2, exposure might influence malignant transformation itself and cause de novo occurrence of a brain tumor. In case 3, exposure might influence the fate of the deviating clone and could decrease latency or probability of spontaneous involution and therefore either increase incidence because of a shift of latency or because

• volume 117 | number 3 | March 2009

317

Kundi
Table 1. Overview of results [odds ratios or standardized incidence ratios (95% CIs)] from epidemiologic studies of mobile phone use and brain tumors. Study Dreyer et al. 1999 Hardell et al. 1999 Hardell et al. 2000 Hardell et al. 2001 Muscat et al. 2000 Inskip et al. 2001 Cohort Case–control 425 Controls Case–control Type Cohort size (no. of cases and controls) 133,423 Hand-held MP 152,138 Portable bag 209 Brain tumor cases Average duration of MP use (years) ~2 ~6 ~3 ~3 Overall results Results for longer duration of MP use Result for ipsilateral MP use — 2.42 (0.97–6.05) 2.62 (1.02–6.71) (multiv) 2.01 (0.92–5.89) 0.86 (0.65–1.35) (overall)

2 Brain tumor deaths — 4 Brain tumor deaths 0.98 (0.69–1.41) > 10 years, 1.20 (0.56–2.59) 0.85 (0.6–1.2) 1.0 (0.7–1.4) 0.8 (0.5–1.2) 0.8 (0.5–1.4) 1.0 (0.8–1.1) 1.0 (0.9–1.1) 1.6 (1.1–2.3) 0.9 (0.5–1.5) 0.68 (0.34–1.38) 1.3 (1.02–1.6) 1.0 (0.8–1.2) 1.0 (0.8–1.2) 1.1 (0.7–1.5) 0.8 (0.6–1.03) 0.9 (0.6–1.1) 3.5 (1.8–6.8) 1.2 (0.7–2.2) 1.0 (0.6–1.7) 1.13 (0.82–1.57) 1.13 (0.86–1.48) 1.13 (0.85–1.50) 0.90 (0.51–1.57) 1.0 (0.6–1.5) 0.83 (0.54–1.28) 1.08 (0.58–2.00) 0.58 (0.37–0.90) 2.6 (1.5–4.3) 1.9 (1.3–2.7) 2.1 (1.4–3.0) 1.7 (0.97–3.0) 1.3 (0.9–1.9) 1.3 (0.9–1.9) 4.2 (1.8–10) 2.0 (1.05–2.8) 1.5 (0.8–2.9) 0.8 (0.5–1.2) 0.8 (0.6–1.0) 0.7 (0.4–1.3) 0.6 (0.5–0.9) 0.9 (0.7–1.2) 0.9 (0.7–1.1) 0.94 (0.78–1.13) 0.98 (0.74–1.29) 0.84 (0.62–1.13) 0.6 (0.4–0.9) 0.8 (0.5–1.1) 0.5 (0.2–1.0) 0.73 (0.43–1.23) 0.78 (0.68–0.91) 0.67 (0.38–1.19) 1.15 (0.65–2.05) 0.92 (0.53–1.59) > 4 years, 1.96 (0.74–5.20) > 4 years, 0.66 (0.28–1.57) > 6.5 years, 0.60 (0.20–1.78) > 5.2 years, 1.05 (0.52–2.11) ≥ 10 years, 0.85 (0.57–1.26) ≥ 4 years, 0.7 (0.3–1.4) ≥ 5 years, 0.6 (0.3–1.4) 0.9 (0.3–2.7) 1.9 (0.6–5.9) ≥ 3 years, 1.2 (0.6–2.3) ≥ 10 years, 0.66 (0.44–0.95) > 2 years, 1.6 (0.9–2.8) 0.6 (0.1–4.5) ≥ 3 years, 1.7 (0.5–5.1) > 10 years, 1.8 (1.1–2.9) — 2.0 (0.5–8.0) > 10 years, 1.0 (0.1–16.0) — — > 10 years, 3.5 (0.7–16.8) — 2.0 (0.2–22.0) > 6 years, 1.17 (0.75–1.81) 1.71 (0.67–4.34) 1.56 (0.92–2.63) ≥ 10 years, 0.22 (0.04–1.11) ≥ 10 years, 1.9 (0.9–4.1) ≥ 10 years, 1.02 (0.32–3.24) 1.64 (0.44–6.12) 0.48 (0.19–1.26) > 10 years, 3.5 (2.0–6.4) 3.6 (1.7–7.5) 2.9 (1.6–5.2) > 10 years, 2.1 (1.1–4.3) 1.5 (0.6–3.9) 1.9 (0.97–3.6) > 10 years, 2.6 (0.9–8) 0.8 (0.1–6.7) 0.3 (0.03–2.2) ≥ 10 years, 0.8 (0.5–1.5) ≥ 5 years, 0.8 (0.6–1.2) ≥ 10 years, 0.9 (0.5–2.0) ≥ 5 years, 0.8 (0.5–1.2) ≥ 10 years, 1.1 (0.7–1.7) 0.7 (0.2–3.5) ≥ 10 years, 1.14 (0.74–1.73) ≥ 10 years, 2.20 (0.94–5.11) 1.09 (0.35–3.37) ≥ 6 years, 0.8 (0.5–1.2) 1.0 (0.6–1.8) 0.5 (0.2–1.5) ≥ 8 years, 0.79 (0.24–2.65) ≥ 10 years, 0.95 (0.74–1.23)

Johansen et al. 2001 Schüz et al. 2006b Auvinen et al. 2002 Muscat et al. 2002 Hardell et al. 2002a

469 Malignant brain tumor cases 422 Controls Case–control 489 Glioma 197 Meningioma 96 Acoustic neuroma 799 Controls Retrospective cohort 420,095 Subscribers Case–control Case–control Case–control 398 Brain tumor cases 1,986 Controls 90 Acoustic neuroma 86 Controls 1,303 Brain tumor cases 611 Meningioma 159 Acoustic neuroma

2001, ~ 3 2006, ~ 8 Analog, ~ 2.5 Digital, ~ 1 ~3 Analog, ~ 7 Digital, ~ 4, Cordless, ~ 6 Analog, ~ 7 Digital, ~ 4 Cordless, ~ 6 Analog, ~ 7 Digital, ~ 4 Cordless, ~ 6 Analog, ~ 7 Digital, ~ 4 Cordless, ~ 5 ~4 ~5 ~5

— — 0.55 (0.50–1.05) 1.8 (1.3–2.5) 1.3 (0.99–1.8) 1.3 (1.01–1.8) — — 1.80 (0.96–3.38) > 6 years 2.29 (0.59–8.93) 1.16 (0.55–2.46) 0.68 (0.58–0.90) 3.9 (1.6–9.5) ≥ 10 years

Hardell et al. 2002ba Christensen et al. 2004 Lönn et al. 2004a Christensen et al. 2005

Case–control Case–control Case–control Case–control

Hardell et al. 2006a Hardell et al. 2005a

Case–control Case–control

1,303 Controls 588 Malignant brain tumor cases 581 Controls 106 Acoustic neuroma 212 Controls 148 Acoustic neuroma 604 Controls 175 Meningioma 81 Glioma I-II 171 Glioma III-IV 822 Controls 317 Malignant brain tumor cases 692 Controls 305 Meningioma 84 Acoustic neuroma

Lönn et al. 2005

Case–control

692 Controls 371 Glioma 273 Meningioma 674 Controls 678 Acoustic neuroma 3,553 Controls 966 Glioma 1,716 Controls 366 Glioma 381 Meningioma 1,494 Controls 289 Glioma 207 Meningioma 45 Acoustic neuroma 358 Controls 101 Acoustic neuroma 339 Controls 1,521 Glioma 3,301 Controls 97 Acoustic neuroma 194 Controls 96 Glioma 96 Controls 109 Acoustic neuroma 214 Controls 88 Glioma 132 Meningioma 392 Controls 1,209 Meningioma 3,299 Controls

Schoemaker et al. 2005b Hepworth et al. 2006 Schüz et al. 2006a Klaeboe et al. 2007

Case–control Case–control Case–control Case–control

Analog, ~ 10 Digital, ~ 6 Cordless, ~ 6 Analog, ~ 9 Digital, ~ 5 Cordless, ~ 5 Analog, ~ 9 Digital, ~ 5 Cordless, ~ 5 Analog, ~ 9 Digital, ~ 3 Analog, ~ 9 Digital, ~ 3 Analog, ~ 8 Digital, ~ 4 ~5 ~4 ~4

3.1 (1.6–6.2) 2.6 (1.6–4.1) 2.9 (1.8–4.7) 1.6 (0.7–3.9) 1.5 (0.9–2.5) 1.6 (0.97–2.6) 5.1 (1.9–14) 2.9 (1.4–6.1) 2.4 (1.1–5.1) 1.6 (0.8–3.4) ≥ 10 years 1.3 (0.5–3.9) ≥ 10 years 1.8 (1.1–3.1) ≥ 10 years 1.24 (1.02–1.52)

1.2 (0.7–1.2) ≥ 6 years 1.4 (0.7–2.9) 0.7 (0.2–2.5) 0.90 (0.50–1.62) 1.39 (1.01–1.92) ≥ 10 years

Takebayashi et al. 2006 Lahkola et al. 2007c Schlehofer et al. 2007 Hours et al. 2007

Case–control Case–control Case–control Case–control

~4 ~6 ~4

Takebayashi et al. 2008 Lahkola et al. 2008d

Case–control Case–control

~4 5.5

1.22 (0.63–2.37) 0.70 (0.42–1.16) 0.76 (0.65–0.89)

1.24 (0.67–2.29) 1.14 (0.65–2.01) 0.99 (0.57–1.73) ≥ 10 years

Abbreviations: CI, confidence interval; MP, mobile phone; multiv, multivariate. aData are a subset from Hardell et al. (2002a). bIncludes data from Christensen et al. (2004) and Lönn et al. (2004a). cIncludes data from Lönn et al. (2005), Christensen et al. (2005), Hepworth et al. (2006), and Klaeboe et al. (2007). dIncludes data from Lönn et al. (2005), Christensen et al. (2005), and Klaeboe et al. (2007).

318

volume

117 | number 3 | March 2009 • Environmental Health Perspectives

Mobile phones and cancer

a tumor that would otherwise remain obscure during lifetime manifests itself clinically. In case 4, no contribution of exposure is possible. Unfortunately, little is known about the duration of these phases. Furthermore, there are likely gross differences between tumor types concerning absolute and relative length of these steps during natural history of the disease. Although slowly growing tumors such as most meningioma and schwannoma may have unchanged growth rates during prolonged periods of time, other brain tumors such as glioblastoma show an explosive final growth after possibly long periods of more stable behavior. For radiation-induced tumors, Cahan et al. (1948) proposed to allow at least 5 years for induction periods. As delineated above, for an effect during latent growth to be detected in epidemiologic studies, exposure must have been during a substantial proportion of growth phase. Therefore, for an influence both during the induction phase and on tumor growth rate, at least 5 years must be allowed for latency or duration of exposure, respectively, to fulfill the criterion of temporal relation. Because for virtually all carcinogens, repetitive or prolonged exposures are necessary to bring about an increased cancer incidence, it is necessary to consider not only time since first exposure but also duration of exposure. Number of calls and average duration of calls seem to be too difficult to remember for periods far in the past, but information about periods of regular use is more easily recalled and therefore could be the best choice for exposure determination. (In principle, it may even be validated by network provider data). Years of regular mobile or cordless phone use up to 5 years before diagnosis would possibly be the appropriate exposure meter for most slowly growing tumors. Because such evaluations have not been performed, I instead assessed exposure duration or latency of ≥ 10 years, as available, for Table 1. In these subjects, at least half of the exposure duration falls within an etiologically relevant period. Association. I computed a meta-analytical estimate of the risk for the different brain tumor types based on all independent studies reporting ORs for ≥ 10 years of mobile phone use. Heterogeneity was assessed by chi-square tests. A p-value < 0.05 was considered significant. If studies included the same population, only those without overlap were considered in the meta-analyses. Pooled effects and SEs were derived from the fixed-effect model because no significant heterogeneity was detected. Because basically all investigations used the same study design and the same end point definition, despite some deviations in exposure classification and proneness to misclassification and selection bias, no attempt was made in the meta-analysis to account for these differences. It should, however, be noted that correction for
Environmental Health Perspectives

selection and misclassification bias would lead to higher meta-analytical odds ratios. There is no publication bias in this case because all studies that are planned, ongoing, or completed are known to the scientific community. For glioma, I included three studies (Hardell et al. 2006c; Lahkola et al. 2007; Schüz et al. 2006a) reporting data on 233 exposed cases and 330 exposed controls among 2,792 glioma patients and 6,195 control subjects. I found no heterogeneity across studies, and the combined OR was 1.5 [95% confidence interval (CI), 1.2–1.8]. For acoustic neuroma, two independent pooled analyses (Hardell et al. 2006b; Schoemaker et al. 2005) gave an overall OR of 1.3 (95% CI, 0.95–1.9), based on 67 exposed cases and 311 exposed controls among 912 and 5,715 cases of acoustic neuroma and controls, respectively. Risk for meningioma from mobile phone use of ≥ 10 years was reported in two individual studies (Hardell et al. 2006b; Schüz et al. 2006a) and one pooled study (Lahkola et al. 2008), with an overall OR of 1.1 (95% CI, 0.8–1.4) evaluated from 116 exposed cases and 320 controls among a total of 2,506 meningioma patients and 6,223 control subjects. Hence, there is an increased risk for all of these end points from mobile phone use that is statistically significant for glioma. In these analyses mobile phone use was assumed to induce the neoplasia. If mobile phone use has an additional or exclusive effect on tumor growth, this analysis is not entirely appropriate because an effect on the growing tumor can only be exerted by exposure on the same side of the head where the tumor is located. Combined estimates for ipsilateral mobile phone use of ≥ 10 years give the ORs: 1.9 (95% CI, 1.4–2.4) for glioma; 1.6 (95% CI, 1.1–2.5) for acoustic neuroma; and 1.3 (95% CI, 0.9–1.9) for meningioma. Hence, there are clear indications of increased risks for all three end points. According to the dialogue approach, association must be assessed whether or not there are valid counterarguments, and especially those based on considerations about the impact of possible biases. Potential biases. As apparent from Table 1, ever (regular) use of a mobile phone rarely revealed increased risks for any type of brain tumor. Except for the Finnish study of Auvinen et al. (2002), only the Swedish group of Hardell and colleagues (e.g., Hardell et al. 2002a) reported significantly elevated estimates of relative risks. Most studies summarized in Table 1 were conducted based on the Interphone protocol (Cardis et al. 2007) that defined regular use as at least one outgoing or incoming call per week for at least 6 months, with ever-regular use starting 1 year before the reference date. Although the reference date was defined as date of diagnosis in cases and the same date of the matched

control, in studies not individually matched (e.g., Hepworth et al. 2006), there are problems in defining the reference date because of the interview lag time. Because of the rapid increase in mobile phone use during and before the study period, the methods applied to compute the reference date for controls could be a source of bias. Information provided in the study reports is insufficient to decide whether adjustments were biased. In some articles (Hepworth et al. 2006; Schoemaker et al. 2005), controls were allocated into categories of interview lag time at random without consideration of age and sex of the cases within these categories, whereas in others (Lönn et al. 2004a), average lag between diagnosis and identification in a matched set was subtracted from date of control identification. The first method introduces bias if distribution of age and sex within categories of lag times differs, and the second method introduces bias if the date of identification differs between cases and controls. To my knowledge, these possible biases have not been considered previously. In the Interphone study (Cardis et al. 2007), data were collected from the end of 2000 through the beginning of 2004, with some differences between countries. During this period, mobile phone penetration rate increased from about 60% to about 90% in the European Union, according to the International Telecommunication Union. Insufficient adjustment for differences in the interview date would result in underestimation of risk. In their studies, Hardell and colleagues disregarded mobile and cordless phone use within the last year before the reference date. Any use of a mobile or cordless phone was counted except when hands-free devices or external car antennas were used. The reference date for controls was set to the date of diagnosis of the matched case. In some reports of pooled data sets (e.g., Hardell et al. 2006c), individual matching was disregarded and controls from different studies were included. Insufficient adjustment of the reference date could have also led to bias in this case. Hardell and colleagues defined the unexposed subjects as those who have not used a mobile or cordless phone for ≥ 1 year before diagnosis (or reference date in controls). The Interphone group disregarded cordless phones in analyses of mobile phone use (and vice versa). Cardis et al. (2007) and Takebayashi et al. (2006) have argued that cordless phone use is associated with much lower exposure to microwaves and therefore cannot be counted in exposure assessments. This view is not correct. Average power levels are not much different between cordless phones (average levels of 10 mW) and mobile phones (median average output power 6–16 mW in urban areas) (Lönn et al. 2004b). Considering the typically longer duration of daily use of cordless

• volume 117 | number 3 | March 2009

319

Kundi

phones compared with mobile phones, it is not a rational procedure to exclude them from total exposure (for information on adolescent users, see Söderqvist et al. 2008). The fraction of cordless phone users among cases and controls not using mobile phones ranged from about 22% in the study of Hardell et al. (2006b) to almost 40% in the German Interphone study (Schüz et al. 2006a). If we arbitrarily assign sensitivity of exposure determination from omission of cordless phone use a value of 74% in cases and 78% in controls, assuming 100% specificity (in cases and controls) and an actual exposure prevalence of 54% [according to the data of Hardell et al. (2006b)], a true OR of 1.5 would be reduced to 1.2. Still greater reductions of the OR result if the differences in cordless phone use were actually greater. In the Interphone studies, data acquisition concerning exposure was predominantly done by computer-assisted personal interview (CAPI). In about 95% of glioma cases and controls, exposure assessment was based on CAPI (Cardis et al. 2007). Reports from five Nordic countries (Lahkola et al. 2007) reveal that > 40% of cases were interviewed in the hospital. However, this fraction ranges from almost 100% in Finland to 3% in the United Kingdom. Data acquisition was completely different in the studies of Hardell and colleagues (Hardell et al. 2002a). They sent a questionnaire to home addresses of cases and controls, and upon return, they evaluated the questionnaires for errors, omissions, and discrepancies. If necessary, additional information was sought by telephone interviews blinded to case status. Method of data acquisition could be important in several respects: a) interviews not blinded to case status may introduce a bias from the expectations of the interviewer; b) the interaction between interviewee and interviewer as such can lead to bias (Rosenthal effects); c) answering a questionnaire at home is less demanding (especially considering the conditions after surgery) than personal interviews; d) at home it is possible to check telephone bills or to inspect contracts with network providers to verify data. For these reasons, the questionnaire method seems to be superior to the interview technique. However, there are also advantages of the CAPI method: data can be immediately checked for errors and discrepancies, and the interviewer can explain points that are not clear and may help in recalling inquired items. Validation studies (Berg et al. 2005; Vrijheid et al. 2006a, 2006b) within the Interphone study showed only a moderate correlation between selfreported intensity of mobile phone use and traffic data from network operators, but confirmed usage data as valid proxy for microwave exposure. For the (not very important) recent mobile phone use, self reports seem to

be fairly accurate, but for earlier use, no data on reliability are available. Considering results from Christensen et al. (2005), memory performance is decreased especially in patients with high-grade glioma. Exposure assessment in these patients could be particularly biased if conducted by interviews compared with the questionnaire method. Bias often represents underreporting of mobile phone use, because it is more likely that a patient forgot using a mobile phone once years ago compared with falsely stating mobile phone use. In the Interphone studies, overall participation was 65% for glioma cases, 78% for meningioma, and 82% for acoustic neuroma (Cardis et al. 2007). For controls, participation was 53% but there was large variation across centers, ranging from 35% to 74%. Hardell et al. (2006d), using postal questionnaires, reported participation rates of 88–91% in cases and 84–92% in controls. The participation rate in cases was computed based on eligible cases that received a questionnaire, defined as those with ascertained primary brain tumors alive at the time of identification and whose participation was not denied by their physician. If the definition of eligible cases for the Interphone studies were applied, the participation rate would amount to about 65–85% in the different studies of Hardell and colleagues. In the Interphone studies, on average, 13% (range, 2–44%) of case interviews were performed as proxy interviews. As shown by Vrijheid et al. (2006a) in an Interphone validation study, response bias due to differential selection of groups of the population with higher prevalence of mobile phone use possibly has the highest impact, even outweighing recall bias. Lönn et al. (2005) showed that nonparticipating cases had almost the same proportion of mobile phone users (50% compared with 52% in participants), but nonparticipating controls differed markedly from participants (34% compared with 59%). Effect of this selection bias might be even greater if long-term use is considered. Consequence of selection bias can easily be determined because the biased OR is equal to the product of the true OR and the selection OR (cf. Rothman et al. 2008). Given the nonresponse analysis of Lönn et al. (2005), the selection OR is computed as 0.64 if overall participation rates of the Interphone studies are considered (72% and 53% in cases and controls, respectively). All 46 ORs in the report of Lönn et al. (2005), except one in the overall analysis of glioma and meningioma, were < 1. Assuming the selection OR is 0.75 as computed based on the participation rates for the Lönn study specifically (79% in cases and 70.5% in controls), almost all these ORs would increase above 1 and none would be significantly < 1 (as was the case for 7 of the 46 ORs). For example, the OR for > 10 years volume of mobile phone use for glioma, reported as 0.9, would increase to 1.2. As has been pointed out previously (Kundi 2004; Kundi et al. 2004; Schoemaker et al. 2005), early symptoms of a developing brain tumor may have influenced behavior regarding mobile phone use. In particular, growing acoustic neuroma are frequently associated with hearing problems and tinnitus. Such symptoms may lead to a restriction of use, change of the side of the head the phone is held during calls, and even to discontinuing mobile phone use. Lönn et al. (2004a), in their study of acoustic neuroma, assessed impact of hearing loss and tinnitus 5 years before reference date and reported no differences in risk estimates for patients with and without hearing loss. Whether this is an indication that such symptoms have no impact on mobile phone use and therefore do not bias risk estimates is difficult to assess because no data were reported. As noted by Schoemaker et al. (2005), 59% of regular phone users among controls reported predominantly rightsided use, 33% left-sided use, and 8% use on both sides. The authors argued that if mobile phones cause acoustic neuromas, one might expect a higher proportion of tumors on the right than on the left side of the head among regular phone users. However, this expectation is completely unfounded. In contrast, tumor growth may cause behavioral changes, as indicated by the distribution of mobile phone use in cases, with only 49% right-sided users, 40% left-sided users, and 11% that used the phone on both sides. Although response bias, misclassification bias, and insufficient correction of interview lag time between cases and controls will reduce risk estimates toward or even below unity, some biases could lead to a spuriously increased risk. One particular point has been raised frequently: Increased risk estimates of ipsilateral mobile phone use (Table 1) could be due to recall bias. If mobile phone use affects tumor development and growth, it is important to consider the side of the head to which the phone is held during calls. Cardis et al. (2008) reported that 97–99% of the total electromagnetic energy deposited in the brain is absorbed at the side of the head the phone is held during calls. Because of this asymmetry, an effect at the site of the growing tumor is expected only or primarily for ipsilateral use. There is no objective method to retrospectively assess side of the head the phone has been used. Asking a person about this aspect of use could result in bias. A person may be inclined to suspect mobile phone use as a causal factor and may therefore tend to report using it at the same side as the tumor has occurred. On the other hand, the reverse also may be claimed—that a person wants to dismiss the possibility that using the phone has anything to do with the disease and is therefore falsely

320

117 | number 3 | March 2009 • Environmental Health Perspectives

Mobile phones and cancer

reporting the opposite side of use. Even if a patient does not intentionally distort the answer, recent surgery may cause memory deficiencies leading to recall bias. Hepworth et al. (2006) argued that the reduced risk on the contralateral side indicates such recall bias. However, this risk reduction was due to an artifact of the method applied. Estimate of relative risk for contralateral mobile phone use was based on nonregular and ipsilateral phone users as reference. It follows that whenever the relative risk of ipsilateral phone use is > 1, the relative risk of contralateral use must be < 1 [the expected value of the OR in this case is (πo+πi)/(πo+ψπi), where πo is the proportion in the population of nonusers, πI is the proportion in the population of ipsilateral users, and ψ is the OR for ipsilateral use]. All meta-analytical ORs for ipsilateral mobile phone use are > 1, and those for glioma and acoustic neuroma are statistically significant. If there was no misclassification bias in controls and perfect sensitivity, then a small recall bias in the direction of a preference for reporting mobile phone use at the side of the tumor of about 3% would reduce these enhanced ORs for long-term (≥ 10 years) use to 1. However, considering overall results for ipsilateral use, recall bias must reduce specificity in cases by 30–40% to remove the observed enhanced risk. The specificity that reduces an observed increased OR to 1 is given by [1+(ψ*–1)π]–1, where ψ* is the observed OR and π is the exposure prevalence in the population, given that sensitivity in both cases and controls and specificity in controls are 1 (Rothman et al. 2008). For example, taking the study of Hardell et al. (2005a) with an overall OR for ipsilateral mobile and cordless phone use of approximately 3.0 for acoustic neuroma and a prevalence of about 23% of ipsilateral mobile or cordless phone use, the specificity must be as low as 68% to remove the observed effect. That is, 32% of those not exposed at all or not on the side of the tumor must have falsely stated they have been exposed. In other words, more than half of mobile phone users among cases and none among controls must have given the wrong side of the head for their predominant use to remove the observed increased risk. It should also be noted that the case-only approach of Takebayashi et al. (2008) cannot solve the problem of recall bias. As can be seen in Table 1, several Interphone groups (Christensen et al. 2005; Klaeboe et al. 2007; Lahkola et al. 2007, 2008; Lönn et al. 2005) reported ORs that were significantly < 1, implying a protective effect of mobile phone use for brain tumors. Although there is a remote possibility that mobile phone use may enhance apoptosis or activate DNA repair, such processes will hardly affect tumor development at an advanced stage; thus, this is not a valid explanation for these reduced
Environmental Health Perspectives

risk estimates but rather it suggests systematic bias. Selection bias, as delineated above, could explain some of these results. However, there are likely additional biases that contributed to the overall effect. Let us consider this aspect from the perspective of the ceteris paribus condition (i.e., that cases and controls have essentially the same features in all relevant aspects except exposure) and in particular from the condition of population equivalence (i.e., the condition that both cases and controls stem from populations that are equivalent for all attributes that are relevant for the disease under study). Mobile phone use is not randomly distributed within the population, but usage patterns will be associated with certain attributes such as occupation, sex, socioeconomic status (SES), and age. Some of these attributes can be accounted for by matching or during analysis, but there could be an association with the disease that cannot be removed by these procedures. Exploration of prior symptoms in brain tumor cases often reveals indications of the disease process many years in the past (e.g., epileptic seizures, personality changes, a variety of cognitive and perception problems). Some of these symptoms could reduce the probability that a person chooses to use a mobile phone—or a telephone in general. Such habit changes would have the greatest impact on measures of cumulative duration and intensity of use. If aspects of the disease influence mobile phone use, the ceteris paribus condition is violated from the very beginning. It is evident that this also implies a violation of the condition of temporal relation, because a reversal of cause and effect may occur. These difficulties are related to the generally short duration of mobile phone use. A solution could be inclusion of case history information and formation of distinct subgroups differing in duration of symptoms related to the developing disease. Obviously, influence of symptoms on mobile phone use will predominantly reduce risk estimates, because the odds for mobile phone use in cases will be lowered. Because of the mentioned biases that could operate in case–control studies, one may be inclined to put some weight on the only cohort study (Johansen et al. 2001; Schüz et al. 2006b) presently available. However, this investigation is severely flawed and cannot contribute to risk assessment and was therefore not included in the meta-analysis (see also Hardell et al. 2008). Confounding. Environmental equivalence seems to be sufficient in all investigations and confounding seems an unlikely cause of bias because there are only a few known risk factors for brain tumors that would induce bias if disregarded in the analyses. Therapeutic and, to a lesser degree, diagnostic X rays to the head region increase the risk for several types of brain tumors. In some studies this was considered a possible confounder but without effect

on the risk estimates (e.g., Hardell et al. 2001), indicating that there is no correlation between such irradiation and mobile phone use. Other possible confounders include neuro fibromatosis and tuberous sclerosis, family history of brain tumors, and medical treatment with growth factors, all of which are very rare conditions without reason to assume a relationship with mobile phone use. Age and sex are the most important confounders that have been considered in all studies either by matching or during analysis. SES has also been indicated as a possible confounder and was included in most analyses. However, including SES in the analysis will not remove selection bias that seems to be related to SES in some Interphone investigations (e.g., Hepworth et al. 2006). Assessment. Overall, arguments in favor of or against an association between mobile phone use and brain tumors are not equally strong. There is evidence for selection bias, exposure misclassification from excluding cordless phone use, reversal of cause and effect from neglecting early symptoms of the disease, and short exposure duration. All these factors lead to reduction of the observed risk estimates. The only strong argument against an association is the possible impact of recall bias. There may be underreporting especially of early mobile phone use because of memory deficits after surgery, but most concerns have been raised for a potential distortion in reporting side of the head the phone is held during calls. Handedness correlates not very highly with side of the head the phone is used [concordances of only about 60% have been determined (Hepworth et al. 2006)], and there are no other methods at hand to validate these data. There are, however, some arguments that speak against a decisive influence of recall bias: Most participants of the Interphone and Hardell studies were enrolled during 1997–2003 at a time when mobile phone use was not widely discussed as a potential risk factor for brain tumors. As reported by Hardell et al. (2002a), among 232 brain tumor cases who expressed their views about potential causes of their disease, only two named mobile phones. Lönn (2004) asked 70 brain tumor cases and controls whether they considered mobile phones as a risk factor for brain tumors and found no difference between both groups. Even if patients consider mobile phones a factor contributing to their disease, what would they gain if they gave the wrong side of use? Most people choose to use mobile phones, but do they want to blame themselves for their disease? In several studies, mobile phone use was only one of the different risk factors investigated (e.g., Lönn et al. 2006). Bias from patients’ attribution would likely extend also to these other factors, and spuriously increased risks should have been observed in some of them. Although reporting bias of this type may explain an increased

• volume 117 | number 3 | March 2009

321

Kundi

risk for long-term ipsilateral use, it is hardly an explanation for the increased overall risk of ipsilateral use revealed in several investigations (Hardell et al. 2002a, 2005a, 2006a; Hepworth et al. 2006). When considering the discussion above, the conditions for a causal interpretation of the observed association between mobile phone use and brain tumors are not fully met. However, discussion of the potential violation of the conditions of temporal relation as well as population and environmental equivalence revealed that it must in all likelihood have reduced the observed association. Because of the important result on ipsilateral mobile phone use, a spuriously increased risk due to recall bias cannot be completely dismissed. In such a situation, evidence from other sources may increase or decrease confidence in a causal relation between mobile phone use and cancer. Additional evidence. Since the 1930s there has been a scientific controversy about effects of high-frequency EMF other than thermal. Absorption of electromagnetic energy is now well understood and poses no principal difficulty of integration into Maxwell’s theory of electromagnetism. The rate of absorption of electromagnetic energy in a homogenous volume of biological tissue is proportional to the temperature increase within this volume. Therefore, at high levels of EMF exposure, significant heating occurs that can be dangerous to health. Exposure standards have been issued that limit exposure to thermally safe levels. However, telecommunication and broadcasting applications of high-frequency EMF are not only radiating energy but also information. This is done by modulation of the highfrequency carrier. The modulation frequencies could be of biological significance and evoke nonthermal effects. In addition, the high-frequency signal itself could cause effects at low levels in combination with the simultaneous presence of the earth magnetic field (Chiabrera et al. 2000). In principle, interactions between EMF and matter are subject to two descriptions, classical or quantum electrodynamics. In a living cell, many important processes occur by electron transfer across membrane structures in a well-organized manner, ions cross selective channels, proteins get activated and deactivated by cascades of precisely regulated enzymes—all processes that occur on a quantum scale. Recently, by application of “omics” research (for an overview, see Vanderstraeten and Verschaeve 2008), it has been shown that cells may respond with activation of proteins and genes at nonthermal levels of exposure, a process that was also observed in vivo (Karinen et al. 2008). However, because of the lack of a mechanistic model, these results are uncertain as to their interpretation with respect to relevant long-term health effects. It has long been speculated (Lai and Singh 1997; Phelan et al.

1992) that free radical formation is involved in EMF-induced health effects. Although there is some evidence of formation of free radicals at nonthermal levels of EMF (Simkó et al. 2006), there are many difficulties with this approach. It would be much too simplistic to assume that free radicals are directly produced by the interaction with the EMF. Rather, these radicals are produced by the cell itself as an intermediate step of the response to sensing the field (Friedman et al. 2007). Concerning prior epidemiologic evidence of a relationship between high-frequency EMF other than those used in mobile telecommunication and brain tumors, despite some reports of increased risk (Berg et al. 2006; Grayson 1996; Szmigielski 1996; Thomas et al. 1987), the evidence is inconclusive to date. Results of epidemiologic studies of mobile phone use summarized above indicate an association that is of moderate strength and in the range delineated for passive smoking and lung cancer. There is no meaningful indicator of exposure dose available, but longer latencies are associated with higher risk estimates, and there are indications that risk is higher in rural areas where phones typically radiate at higher intensities (Hardell et al. 2005b). These aspects do increase confidence in a causal relationship. In the case that epidemiology faces problems due to short exposure durations, lifetime animal bioassays gain importance in establishing a carcinogenic risk. Unfortunately, standard procedures cannot be applied for exposure to microwaves from mobile phones. In hundreds of animal carcinogenicity assays, it has been shown that even for DNA reactive agents, exposure during most of the life span of the animals at the maximum tolerated dose is necessary to significantly increase incidence. Such high exposures are impossible for high-frequency EMF because of interference with heating. Hence, it is necessary to apply levels that are much too low for an increased incidence to be expected. Some solutions to this problem have been proposed: a) coexposure or prior exposure to a known carcinogen; b) implantation of tumor cells; and c) use of animal strains with a habitually increased tumor incidence. Because of the unknown mechanism of action, none of these attempts can be evaluated according to their suitability. More than 30 long-term and medium-term animal assays have been published in the past decade, most of which do not comply with basic criteria (Kundi 2003), and there is no suitable model for brain tumors. Application of ethylnitrosourea during gestation results in an increased incidence of brain tumors but increases incidence of many other tumors as well. A further problem is the much smaller size of laboratory rodents, resulting in a completely different exposure pattern at telecommunication frequencies. In contrast to volume humans using a mobile phone with a localized exposure at the side of the head the phone is held, animal exposure is a whole-body exposure. Although devices have been constructed that result in a predominant head irradiation (Adey et al. 2000), pattern and distribution within the brain will still be completely different from human exposures. It is not surprising then that up to now only a few animal experiments have found some indication of an increased cancer risk (e.g., Hruby et al. 2008; Repacholi et al. 1997; Shirai et al. 2007). Interpretation of epidemiologic findings would be much easier if genotoxicity of mobile telecommunication signals could firmly be established. In vitro experiments have brought about diverse results that at present provide only equivocal evidence for genotoxic effects. Additionally, in the case of genotoxicity assays, procedures widely used for assessing environmental and nutritional toxicants may not be ideally suited for the study of EMF. For example, if, as suggested by Lai and Singh (2004), exposure-induced effects imply activation of the Fenton reaction, cells rich in free iron would be responsive while others would not. Hence, overall evidence without considering mechanistic hypotheses is of limited value. Nevertheless, about one quarter of published genotoxicity studies found an effect of lowlevel exposures (Vijayalaxmi and Obe 2004; Vijayalaxmi and Prihoda 2008). Overall, animal and in vitro experiments do not reduce confidence in a causal relationship but do not provide unequivocal support either. The main problem is the lack of a coherent research strategy that unifies strengths of different disciplines to unravel the intriguing problem of low-level EMF health effects from a biophysical perspective.

Conclusions
Epidemiologic evidence compiled in the past 10 years starts to indicate an increased risk, in particular for brain tumors (glioma, meningioma, acoustic neuroma), from mobile phone use. Considering biases that may have been operating in most studies, the risk estimates are rather too low, although recall bias could have increased risk estimates. The net result, when considering the different errors and their impact, is still rather an elevated risk. The magnitude of the brain tumor risk is moderate, but it has to be borne in mind that estimates are still from short durations of exposure. From the perspective of public health, an increase of brain tumor incidence of ≥ 50% poses substantial problems for neurosurgical care, but the individual perspective is less dramatic: in industrial countries, the lifetime brain tumor risk is 4–8 per 1,000. If mobile phone use should increase these figures to 6–12 per 1,000, the individual risk is still low.

322

117 | number 3 | March 2009 • Environmental Health Perspectives

Mobile phones and cancer

At present, evidence for a causal relationship between mobile phone use and brain tumors relies predominantly on epidemiology, in particular on the large studies of Hardell and colleagues, but there are no valid counter arguments and no strong evidence decreasing confidence in a causal relationship. Weak evidence in favor of a causal relationship is provided by some animal and in vitro studies, but overall, genotoxicity assays, both in vivo and in vitro, are inconclusive to date. RefeRences
Adey W, Byus C, Cain C, Higgins R, Jones R, Kean C, et al. 2000. Spontaneous and nitrosourea-induced primary tumors of the central nervous system in Fischer 344 rats exposed to frequency-modulated microwave fields. Cancer Res 60(7):1857–1863. Ahlbom A, Green A, Kheifets L, Savitz D, Swerdlow A. 2004. Epidemiology of health effects of radiofrequency exposure. Environ Health Perspect 112:1741–1754. Auvinen A, Hietanen M, Luukkonen R, Koskela R. 2002. Brain tumors and salivary gland cancers among cellular telephone users. Epidemiology 13(3):356–359. Berg G, Schüz J, Samkange-Zeeb F, Blettner M. 2005. Assessment of radiofrequency exposure from cellular telephone daily use in an epidemiological study: German validation study of the international case-control study of cancers of the brain—INTERPHONE—study. J Expo Anal Environ Epidemiol 15(3):217–224. Berg G, Spallek J, Schüz J, Schlehofer B, Böhler E, Schlaefer K, et al. 2006. Occupational exposure to radio frequency/microwave radiation and the risk of brain tumors: Interphone Study Group, Germany. Am J Epidemiol 164(6):538–548. Cahan W, Woodard H, Higinbotham N, Stewart F, Coley B. 1948. Sarcoma arising in irradiated bone. Report of eleven cases. Cancer 1:3–29. Cardis E, Deltour I, Mann S, Moissonnier M, Taki M, Varsier N, et al. 2008. Distribution of RF energy emitted by mobile phones in anatomical structures of the brain. Phys Med Biol 53:2771–2783. Cardis E, Richardson L, Deltour I, Armstrong B, Feychting M, Johansen C, et al. 2007. The INTERPHONE study: design, epidemiological methods, and description of the study population. Eur J Epidemiol 22(9):647–664. Chiabrera A, Bianco B, Moggia E, Kaufman J. 2000. ZeemanStark modeling of the RF EMF interaction with ligand binding. Bioelectromagnetics 21:312–324. Christensen H, Schüz J, Kosteljanetz M, Poulsen H, Boice J Jr, McLaughlin J, et al. 2005. Cellular telephones and risk for brain tumors: a population-based, incident case-control study. Neurology 64(7):1189–1195. Christensen H, Schüz J, Kosteljanetz M, Poulsen H, Thomsen J, Johansen C. 2004. Cellular telephone use and risk of acoustic neuroma. Am J Epidemiol 159(3):277–283. Dreyer N, Loughlin J, Rothman K. 1999. Cause-specific mortality in cellular telephone users. JAMA 282(19):1814–1816. Friedman J, Kraus S, Hauptman Y, Schiff Y, Seger R. 2007. Mechanism of short-term ERK activation by electromagnetic fields at mobile phone frequencies. Biochem J 405(3):559–568. Grayson J. 1996. Radiation exposure socioeconomic status and brain tumor risk in the U.S. Air Force: a nested casecontrol study. Am J Epidemiol 143:480–486. Hardell L, Carlberg M, Hansson Mild K. 2005a. Case-control study on cellular and cordless telephones and the risk for acoustic neuroma or meningioma in patients diagnosed 2000–2003. Neuroepidemiology 25(3):120–128. Hardell L, Carlberg M, Hansson Mild K. 2005b. Use of cellular telephones and brain tumour risk in urban and rural areas. Occup Environ Med 62(6):390–394. Hardell L, Carlberg M, Hansson Mild K. 2006a. Case-control study of the association between the use of cellular and cordless telephones and malignant brain tumors diagnosed during 2000–2003. Environ Res 100(2):232–241. Hardell L, Carlberg M, Hansson Mild K. 2006b. Pooled analysis of two case-control studies on the use of cellular and cordless telephones and the risk of benign brain tumours diagnosed during 1997–2003. Int J Oncol 28(2):509–518.

Hardell L, Carlberg M, Hansson Mild, K. 2006c. Pooled analysis of two case-control studies on use of cellular and cordless telephones and the risk for malignant brain tumours diagnosed in 1997–2003. Int Arch Occup Environ Health 79(8):630–639. Hardell L, Carlberg M, Hansson Mild K. 2008. Methodological aspects of epidemiological studies on the use of mobile phones and their association with brain tumors. Open Environ Sci 2:54–61. Hardell L, Carlberg M, Ohlson C, Westberg H, Eriksson M, Hansson Mild K. 2007. Use of cellular and cordless telephones and risk of testicular cancer. Int J Androl 30(2):115–122. Hardell L, Eriksson M, Carlberg M, Sundström C, Hansson Mild K. 2005c. Use of cellular or cordless telephones and the risk for non-Hodgkin’s lymphoma. Int Arch Occup Environ Health 78(8):625–632. Hardell L, Hallquist A, Hansson Mild K, Carlberg M, Gertzén H, Schildt E, et al. 2004. No association between the use of cellular or cordless telephones and salivary gland tumours. Occup Environ Med 61(8):675–679. Hardell L, Hallquist A, Hansson Mild K, Carlberg M, Påhlson A, Lilja A. 2002a. Cellular and cordless telephones and the risk for brain tumours. Eur J Cancer Prev 11(4):377–386. Hardell L, Hansson Mild K, Carlberg M. 2002b. Case-control study on the use of cellular and cordless phones and the risk for malignant brain tumours. Int J Radiat Biol 78(10):931–936. Hardell L, Hansson Mild K, Carlberg M, Söderqvist F. 2006d. Tumour risk associated with use of cellular telephones or cordless desktop telephones. World J Surg Oncol 4:74–83. Hardell L, Hansson Mild K, Påhlson A, Hallquist A. 2001. Ionizing radiation, cellular telephones and the risk for brain tumours. Eur J Cancer Prev 10(6):523–529. Hardell L, Nasman A, Påhlson A, Hallquist A. 2000. Case-control study on radiology work, medical X-ray investigations, and use of cellular telephones as risk factors for brain tumors. MedGenMed 2(2):E2. Hardell L, Näsman A, Påhlson A, Hallquist A, Hansson Mild K. 1999. Use of cellular telephones and the risk for brain tumours: a case-control study. Int J Oncol 15(1):113–116. Hepworth S, Schoemaker M, Muir K, Swerdlow A, van Tongeren, MJA, McKinney PA. 2006. Mobile phone use and risk of glioma in adults: case-control study. BMJ 332(7546):883–887. Hours M, Bernard M, Montestrucq L, Arslan M, Bergeret A, Deltour I, et al. 2007. Cell phones and risk of brain and acoustic nerve tumours: the French INTERPHONE casecontrol study [in French]. Rev Epidemiol Sante Publique 55(5):321–332. Hruby R, Neubauer G, Kuster N, Frauscher M. 2008. Study on potential effects of “902-MHz GSM-type Wireless Communication Signals” on DMBA-induced mammary tumours in Sprague-Dawley rats. Mutat Res 649:34–44. Inskip P, Tarone R, Hatch E, Wilcosky T, Shapiro W, Selker R, et al. 2001. Cellular-telephone use and brain tumors. N Engl J Med 344(2):79–86. Johansen C, Boice J Jr, McLaughlin J, Olsen J. 2001. Cellular telephones and cancer—a nationwide cohort study in Denmark. J Natl Cancer Inst 93(3):203–207. Karinen A, Heinävaara S, Nylund R, Leszczynski D. 2008. Mobile phone radiation might alter protein expression in human skin. BMC Genomics 9:77; doi:10.1186/1471-2164-9-77 [Online 11 February 2008]. Klaeboe L, Blaasaas K, Tynes T. 2007. Use of mobile phones in Norway and risk of intracranial tumours. Eur J Cancer Prev 16(2):158–164. Kleihues P, Cavenee WK, eds. 2000. World Health Organization Classification of Tumours. Pathology and Genetics: Tumours of the Nervous System. Lyon France:International Agency for Research on Cancer Kranzinger M, Jones N, Rittinger O, Pilz P, Piotrowski W, Manzl M, et al. 2001. Malignant glioma as a secondary malignant neoplasm after radiation therapy for craniopharyngioma: report of a case and review of reported cases. Onkologie 24(1):66–72. Krewski D, Byus C, Glickman B, Lotz W, Mandeville R, McBride M, et al. 2001. Recent advances in research on radiofrequency fields and health. J Toxicol Environ Health B Crit Rev 4(1):145–159. Kundi M. 2003. Comments on the responses of Utteridge et al. (Radiat. Res. 159, 277–278, 2003) to letters about their paper (Radiat. Res. 158, 357–364, 2002). Radiat Res 160(5):613–614. Kundi M. 2004. Mobile phone use and cancer. Occup Environ Med 61(6):560–570, 487.

Kundi M. 2006. Causality and the interpretation of epidemiologic evidence. Environ Health Perspect 114:969–974. Kundi M, Hansson Mild K, Hardell L, Mattsson M. 2004. Mobile telephones and cancer—a review of epidemiological evidence. J Toxicol Environ Health B Crit Rev 7(5):351–384. Lahkola A, Auvinen A, Raitanen J, Schoemaker M, Christensen H, Feychting M, et al. 2007. Mobile phone use and risk of glioma in 5 North European countries. Int J Cancer 120(8):1769–1775. Lahkola A, Salminen T, Raitanen J, Heinävaara S, Schoemaker M, Christensen HC, et al. 2008. Meningioma and mobile phone use—a collaborative case-control study in five North European countries. Int J Epidemiol 37(6):1304–1313. Lai H, Singh N. 1997. Melatonin and a spin-trap compound blocked radiofrequency radiation-induced DNA strand breaks in rat brain cells. Bioelectromagnetics 18:446–454. Lai H, Singh N. 2004. Magnetic-field-induced DNA strand breaks in brain cells of the rat. Environ Health Perspect 112:687–694. Linet M, Taggart T, Severson R, Cerhan J, Cozen W, Hartge P, et al. 2006. Cellular telephones and non-Hodgkin lymphoma. Int J Cancer 119(10):2382–2388. Lönn S. 2004. Mobile phone use and risk of intracranial tumors [Doctoral thesis]. Stockholm:Karolinska Institutet. Lönn S, Ahlbom A, Christensen H, Johansen C, Schüz J, Edström S, et al. 2006. Mobile phone use and risk of parotid gland tumor. Am J Epidemiol 164(7):637–643. Lönn S, Ahlbom A, Hall P, Feychting M. 2004a. Mobile phone use and the risk of acoustic neuroma. Epidemiology 15(6):653–659. Lönn S, Ahlbom A, Hall P, Feychting M. 2005. Long-term mobile phone use and brain tumor risk. Am J Epidemiol 161(6):526–535. Lönn S, Forssén U, Vecchia P, Ahlbom A, Feychting M. 2004b. Output power levels from mobile phones in different geographical areas; implications for exposure assessment. Occup Environ Med 61:769–772. Mohyuddin A, Vokurka E, Evans D, Ramsden R, Jackson A. 2003. Is clinical growth index a reliable predictor of tumour growth in vestibular schwannomas? Clin Otolaryngol Allied Sci 28(2):85–90. Muscat J, Malkin M, Shore R, Thompson S, Neugut A, Stellman S, et al. 2002. Handheld cellular telephones and risk of acoustic neuroma. Neurology 58(8):1304–1306. Muscat J, Malkin M, Thompson S, Shore R, Stellman S, McRee D, et al. 2000. Handheld cellular telephone use and risk of brain cancer. JAMA 284(23):3001–3007. Phelan A, Lange D, Kues H, Lutty G. 1992. Modification of membrane fluidity in melanin containing cells by low level microwave radiation. Bioelectromagnetics 13:131–146. Repacholi M, Basten A, Gebski V, Noonan D, Finnie J, Harris A. 1997. Lymphomas in E mu-Pim1 transgenic mice exposed to pulsed 900 MHZ MHz electromagnetic fields. Radiat Res 147:631–640. Rothman KJ, Greenland S, Lash TL. 2008. Modern Epidemiology. 3rd ed. Philadelphia:Lippincott Williams & Wilkins Sadetzki S, Chetrit A, Freedman L, Stovall M, Modan B, Novikov I. 2005. Long-term follow-up for brain tumor development after childhood exposure to ionizing radiation for tinea capitis. Radiat Res 163(4):424–432. Sadetzki S, Chetrit A, Jarus-Hakak A, Cardis E, Deutch Y, Duvdevani S, et al. 2008. Cellular phone use and risk of benign and malignant parotid gland tumors—a nationwide case-control study. Am J Epidemiol 167(4):457–467. Schlehofer B, Schlaefer K, Blettner M, Berg G, Böhler E, Hettinger I, et al. 2007. Environmental risk factors for sporadic acoustic neuroma (Interphone Study Group, Germany). Eur J Cancer 43(11):1741–1747. Schoemaker M, Swerdlow A, Ahlbom A, Auvinen A, Blaasaas K, Cardis E, et al. 2005. Mobile phone use and risk of acoustic neuroma: results of the Interphone case-control study in five North European countries. Br J Cancer 93(7):842–848. Schüz J, Böhler E, Berg G, Schlehofer B, Hettinger I, Schlaefer K, et al. 2006a. Cellular phones, cordless phones, and the risks of glioma and meningioma (Interphone Study Group, Germany). Am J Epidemiol 163(6):512–520. Schüz J, Jacobsen R, Olsen J, Boice J Jr, McLaughlin J, Johansen C. 2006b. Cellular telephone use and cancer risk: update of a nationwide Danish cohort. J Natl Cancer Inst 98(23):1707–1713. Schwartzbaum JA, Fisher JL, Aldape KD, Wrensch M. 2006. Epidemiology and molecular pathology of glioma. Nat Clin Pract Neurol 2(9):494–503. Shirai T, Ichihara T, Wake K, Watanabe S, Yamanaka Y, Kawabe M, et al. 2007. Lack of promoting effects of chronic exposure to 1.95-GHz W-CDMA signals for IMT-2000 cellular system on development of N-ethylnitrosourea-induced central

Environmental Health Perspectives

• volume 117 | number 3 | March 2009

323

Kundi

nervous system tumors in F344 rats. Bioelectromagnetics 28(7):562–572. Simkó M, Hartwig C, Lantow M, Lupke M, Mattsson M, Rahman Q, et al. 2006. Hsp70 expression and free radical release after exposure to non-thermal radio-frequency electromagnetic fields and ultrafine particles in human Mono Mac 6 cells. Toxicol Lett 161(1):73–82. Söderqvist F, Carlberg M, Hardell L. 2008. Use of wireless telephones and self-reported health symptoms: a populationbased study among Swedish adolescents aged 15–19 years. Environ Health 7:18; doi:10.1186/1476-069X-7-18 [Online 21 May 2008]. Stang A, Anastassiou G, Ahrens W, Bromen K, Bornfeld N, Jöckel K. 2001. The possible role of radiofrequency radiation in the development of uveal melanoma. Epidemiology 12(1):7–12. Szmigielski S. 1996. Cancer morbidity in subjects occupationally exposed to high frequency (radiofrequency and microwave) electromagnetic radiation. Sci Total Environ 180:9–17. Takebayashi T, Akiba S, Kikuchi Y, Taki M, Wake K, Watanabe S,

et al. 2006. Mobile phone use and acoustic neuroma risk in Japan. Occup Environ Med 63(12):802–807. Takebayashi T, Varsier N, Kikuchi Y, Wake K, Taki M, Watanabe S, et al. 2008. Mobile phone use, exposure to radiofrequency electromagnetic field, and brain tumour: a case-control study. Br J Cancer 98(3):652–659. Thomas T, Stolley P, Stemhagen A, Fontham E, Bleeker M, Stewart P. 1987. Brain tumour mortality risk among men with electrical and electronic jobs: a case-control study. J Natl Cancer Inst 79:233–238. Umansky F, Shoshan Y, Rosnethal G, Fraifeld S, Spektor S. 2008. Radiation-induced meningioma. Neurosurg Focus 24(5):E7; doi:10.3171/FOC/2008/24/5/E7. Vanderstraeten J, Verschaeve L. 2008. Gene and protein expression following exposure to radiofrequency fields from mobile phones. Environ Health Perspect 116:1131–1135. Vijayalaxmi, Obe G. 2004. Controversial cytogenetic observations in mammalian somatic cells exposed to radiofrequency radiation. Radiat Res 162:481–496.

Vijayalaxmi, Prihoda TJ. 2008. Genetic damage in mammalian somatic cells exposed to radiofrequency radiation: a metaanalysis of data from 63 publications (1990–2005). Radiat Res 169(5):561–574. Vrijheid M, Cardis E, Armstrong B, Auvinen A, Berg G, Blaasaas K, et al. 2006a. Validation of short term recall of mobile phone use for the Interphone study. Occup Environ Med 63(4):237–243. Vrijheid M, Deltour I, Krewski D, Sanchez M, Cardis E. 2006b. The effects of recall errors and of selection bias in epidemiologic studies of mobile phone use and cancer risk. J Expo Sci Environ Epidemiol 16(4):371–384. Warren H, Prevatt A, Daly K, Antonelli P. 2003. Cellular telephone use and risk of intratemporal facial nerve tumor. Laryngoscope 113(4):663–667. Wrensch M, Minn Y, Chew T, Bondy M, Berger M. 2002. Epidemiology of primary brain tumors: current concepts and review of the literature. Neuro Oncol 4(4):278–299.

324

volume

117 | number 3 | March 2009 • Environmental Health Perspectives

Similar Documents

Free Essay

Cell Phones and Brain Cancer

...Cell phones and brain cancer Now a days everyone has a cell phone. People of all ages rely on their cell phone for daily tasks. How many times a person feels absolutely powerless without a cell phone? Which leads us to a very important question. What type of damage does the use of cell phone causes in the brain? The purpose of this paper is to explore if there is evidence that will link cell phone use and brain cancer. There has been a tremendous increase of cell phone use that is starting at a young age. This means a longer exposure to cell phones throughout life. Currently, people do not seem preoccupied with the idea of brain cancer. There are no reports of cell phone use decrease from consumers. People start using cell phone at a younger age every year. Middle school admission paper work requests student’s cell phone number to inform emergency situations, which leads to pressure to parent to get the student a phone. Some people are abstaining from traditional phone line service in their homes. Phones lines can be seen as waste of money since cell phone use is more prominent in some households. University of Great Fall’s library research data base was used to search for evidence that will link cell phone use and brain cancer. The keywords used where “cell phone use and brain cancer.” The Bosnian Journal of Basic Medical Science reviewed total of 30 studies on mobile phones, all but one were case-control studies (Bayazit, Bayram, Pala, & Atan, 2010, p. 246). The...

Words: 678 - Pages: 3

Premium Essay

Las290 Final Research Essay

...5 Political and Legal Influences ……………………………………………6 Political Factors …………………………………………………..6 Government vs. Private Funding …………………………………7 Cost to Consumer …………………………………………………8 Availability to the Consumer …………………………………….9 Legal Influences ..............................................................................9 Wireless Security Issues ..................................................................10 Political Factors ................................................................................10 Government Policy …………………………………………………11 Public Safety Concerns ......................................................................13 Regulation Control and Government Oversight ………………….15 Potential Controversies ......………………………………………...15 Economic Questions and Considerations…………………………17 Research and Development ………………………………17. Initial and Operational Costs ………………………………17. Financial Hesitations and Impacts …………………………17 Psychological Consideration and Sociological Effects ……………17. Theories ..............……………………………………………18 Positive and Negative Effects ………………………………...…….19 Society’s Impact …………………………………………20. Cultural Context and Media Influences ………………………..23 Implications for the Environment …………………………………….23 Green Benefits....………………………………………………...23 Future Effects ………………………………………………….24 Environmental Issues ………………………………………...…24 Danger of Implications …………………………………………24 Moral and Ethical Implications ………………………………………...

Words: 8771 - Pages: 36

Premium Essay

Cellular Effects

...growth in technology and in personal use. Texting has now become the dominant means of communication and actual conversation skills are diminishing. Exposure to electromagnetic radiation emitted from cellular devices has become a concern and is being studied from many various angles to determine actual health risks from cell phone use. Whether it is for personal or business use, the cell phone has become a permanent fixture in our lives, could have potential health risks, and can become an addiction if necessary steps are not taken to control use and dependency.   Cellular Technology and Society Today At any store they are visible. They have their hand plastered to their ear or they are seemingly talking to no one. These are the cellphone users. Hayley Tsukayama, a writer for the Washington post asserts that according to the Pew Internet and Life Project only seventeen percent of Americans still have a land line and ninety percent own a cellphone. (Tsukayama, 2014) The largest contributor to this trend is convenience. Most cellular devices are small and easily portable, allowing the user to move around freely. The ability to be in contact on the road, at work, or out having fun has pushed the trend that the American consumer is converting to cellular devices. Adding to this trend is the ability of cellular devices to do more than just make a call. “Cell phones are not just about calling or texting – with expanding functionality, phones have become multimedia recording devices...

Words: 19048 - Pages: 77

Premium Essay

Pencils

...Samsung Galaxy S∏ 4 User Guide User Guide GH68-38773F Printed in USA Guía del Usuario A N D R O I D S M A R T P H O N E User Manual Please read this manual before operating your phone and keep it for future reference. GH68-38773F Printed in Korea Intellectual Property All Intellectual Property, as defined below, owned by or which is otherwise the property of Samsung or its respective suppliers relating to the SAMSUNG Phone, including but not limited to, accessories, parts, or software relating there to (the “Phone System”), is proprietary to Samsung and protected under federal laws, state laws, and international treaty provisions. Intellectual Property includes, but is not limited to, inventions (patentable or unpatentable), patents, trade secrets, copyrights, software, computer programs, and related documentation and other works of authorship. You may not infringe or otherwise violate the rights secured by the Intellectual Property. Moreover, you agree that you will not (and will not attempt to) modify, prepare derivative works of, reverse engineer, decompile, disassemble, or otherwise attempt to create source code from the software. No title to or ownership in the Intellectual Property is transferred to you. All applicable rights of the Intellectual Property shall remain with SAMSUNG and its suppliers. Open Source Software Some software components of this product, including but not limited to 'PowerTOP' and 'e2fsprogs', incorporate source...

Words: 55362 - Pages: 222

Premium Essay

Industry Analysis of Telecom Sector in Bangladesh

...Executive summary This term paper is prepared on the telecommunication sector of Bangladesh. Telecom sector of Bangladesh is a very fast growing and dynamic sector. This sector marked its inception in 1989. In this report we have analyzed the history, the value chain activities, the competitive forces, mobile network portability, and national telecom policy etc. We have conducted the risk analysis of the industry in terms of different analysis tools that is SWOT, PESTEL etc. We have covered in and out of the industry in the part A of our report. In part B, we have analyzed the industry players. We have done TTF of our respective company that we have chosen. Then in part C, we have written the case of the respective company that the group members have chosen. Bangladesh’s mobile phone market has achieved exceptional growth since the beginning of 2004, registering a massive 100% growth in its subscriber base during 2004, and 137% during 2005. In recent year of 2012, 15% has been observed in the industry. This trend is forecast to continue over the coming years as the 3G will further boost up the pace of subscribers’ acquisition Many foreign investors are now interested to do business in telecom sector in Bangladesh which reveals that Bangladesh has become a significant hub for telecoms. It has been forecasted that the average revenue from telecoms sector will be Tk 2500 crore a year. Bangladesh is a country which is densely populated and also is a flat and easily extends able...

Words: 27987 - Pages: 112

Free Essay

Entrepreneurial

...P O R T A B L E M O B I L E Q U A D - B A N D P H O N E User Manual Please read this manual before operating your phone, and keep it for future reference. Intellectual Property All Intellectual Property, as defined below, owned by or which is otherwise the property of Samsung or its respective suppliers relating to the SAMSUNG Phone, including but not limited to, accessories, parts, or software relating there to (the “Phone System”), is proprietary to Samsung and protected under federal laws, state laws, and international treaty provisions. Intellectual Property includes, but is not limited to, inventions (patentable or unpatentable), patents, trade secrets, copyrights, software, computer programs, and related documentation and other works of authorship. You may not infringe or otherwise violate the rights secured by the Intellectual Property. Moreover, you agree that you will not (and will not attempt to) modify, prepare derivative works of, reverse engineer, decompile, disassemble, or otherwise attempt to create source code from the software. No title to or ownership in the Intellectual Property is transferred to you. All applicable rights of the Intellectual Property shall remain with SAMSUNG and its suppliers. Samsung Telecommunications America (STA), LLC Headquarters: 1301 E. Lookout Drive Richardson, TX 75082 Toll Free Tel: Customer Care Center: 1000 Klein Rd. Plano, TX 75074 1.888.987.HELP (4357) Internet Address: http://www.samsung.com ©2011 Samsung...

Words: 63749 - Pages: 255

Free Essay

Managing Peolpe Mba

...General Guidelines Version 6.7 March 28, 2016 General Guidelines Overview .................................................................................. 4 0.1 The Purpose of Search Quality Rating .................................................................................................... 5 0.2 Raters Must Represent the User .............................................................................................................. 5 0.3 Browser Requirements ............................................................................................................................. 5 0.4 Ad Blocking Extensions ........................................................................................................................... 5 0.5 Internet Safety Information....................................................................................................................... 5 0.6 Releasing Tasks ........................................................................................................................................ 6 Part 1: Page Quality Rating Guideline .................................................................... 7 1.0 Introduction to Page Quality Rating................................................................................................................. 7 2.0 Understanding Webpages and Websites .............................................................................

Words: 51021 - Pages: 205

Premium Essay

Future of Technology

...THE FUTURE OF TECHNOLOGY OTHER ECONOMIST BOOKS Guide to Analysing Companies Guide to Business Modelling Guide to Business Planning Guide to Economic Indicators Guide to the European Union Guide to Financial Markets Guide to Management Ideas Numbers Guide Style Guide Dictionary of Business Dictionary of Economics International Dictionary of Finance Brands and Branding Business Consulting Business Ethics Business Strategy China’s Stockmarket Globalisation Headhunters and How to Use Them Successful Mergers Wall Street Essential Director Essential Economics Essential Finance Essential Internet Essential Investment Essential Negotiation Pocket World in Figures THE FUTURE OF TECHNOLOGY THE ECONOMIST IN ASSOCIATION WITH PROFILE BOOKS LTD Published by Profile Books Ltd 3a Exmouth House, Pine Street, London ec1r 0jh Copyright © The Economist Newspaper Ltd 2005 All rights reserved. Without limiting the rights under copyright reserved above, no part of this publication may be reproduced, stored in or introduced into a retrieval system, or transmitted, in any form or by any means (electronic, mechanical, photocopying, recording or otherwise), without the prior written permission of both the copyright owner and the publisher of this book. The greatest care has been taken in compiling this book. However, no responsibility can be accepted by the publishers or compilers for the accuracy of the information presented. Where opinion is expressed it is that of the author and does not necessarily...

Words: 128899 - Pages: 516

Premium Essay

Apple Inc. Group Case

...and marketing strategies. (CI #2) ECONOMIC: Domestic Market Apple (iOS capable) phones are losing market share to Android capable phones. (CI #3) EXTERNAL ENVIRONMENT: Foreign Markets The Apple Corporation is criticized for manufacture issues and the lack of ability to acquire market share from foreign competitors overseas. (CI #4) SOCIAL ENVIRONMENT: Technological The Apple Corporation constantly faces criticism for various technological and security issues. I. CURRENT SITUATION A. CURRENT PERFORMANCE 2013 was a rough year for the Apple Corporation, closing the fiscal year down 29.1%. Despite the record breaking release of the iPhone 5S and 5C and a total revenue of $170 Billion, up 9.55%, net income was only $37 Billion, down 11.25% from the previous year. The Apple Corporation’s market share in smartphones was only 12.9% and 18% in application downloads. The Apple Corporation controls only a relatively small section of the computer market, but its market share has grown with the decrease in sale of windows machines. B. STRATEGIC POSTURE 1. Mission a. As of 2013: “Apple designs Macs, the best personal computers in the world, along with OS X, iLife, iWork and professional software. Apple leads the digital music revolution with its iPods and iTunes online store. Apple has reinvented the mobile phone with its revolutionary iPhone and App Store, and is defining the future of mobile media and computing devices with iPad.” b. Prior to 2013: “The company is committed...

Words: 10734 - Pages: 43

Free Essay

A World Without Boundaries

...01-Aug-2014 Politics India President congratulates medal winners at CWG  President of India Pranab Mukherjee has congratulated the Indian players who won the medals at the Glasgow Commonwealth Games 2014. He gave his best wishes to Bijneesh Bajrang, Lalita, Navjot Kaur, Swati Singh and Lalita for their accomplishments in CWG. He wrote separate letters and said that the achievement of the players has proved that the Indian sportsmen are capable of reaching the top at the international level. 01-Aug-2014 Science and Technology Croma & Intel launch Windows 8.1 2-in-1 PC  Croma & Intel have jointly launched two Windows 8.1-based devices – a 10.1-inch 2-in-1 PC with an attached keyboard, along with a tablet with 8-inch display feature. The 10-inch model will be available at a price of Rs 21,990 while the 8-inch model will be priced at Rs 13,990. The 8-inch tablet will only be available in the Croma outlets and the retail site of the company only. 01-Aug-2014 Sports Dipa Karmakar creates history for India in CWG  After winning the Bronze medal in the artistic gymnastics event, Dipa Karmakar has created a history by becoming the first Indian woman and the second player from the country. Karmarkar collected 14.366 points to finish the third in the women’s vault final.England’s Claudia Fragapane bagged the gold medal with 14.633 and Silver went to Elsabeth Black. 01-Aug-2014 Business Jet Airways offers tickets for as low as Rs 1,499  Jet Airways declared a discounted...

Words: 27667 - Pages: 111

Free Essay

Research Paper

...Graduate School of Development Studies POP POLITICS: Popular culture as a democratic space for active citizenship among Filipino youth A Research Paper presented by: Marie Angelie Resurreccion Philippines in partial fulfillment of the requirements for obtaining the degree of MASTERS OF ARTS IN DEVELOPMENT STUDIES Specialization: Children and Youth Studies (CYS) Members of the examining committee: Dr. Linda Herrera [Supervisor] Prof. Dr. Ben White [Reader] The Hague, The Netherlands November, 2009 Disclaimer: This document represents part of the author’s study programme while at the Institute of Social Studies. The views stated therein are those of the author and not necessarily those of the Institute. Research papers are not made available for circulation outside of the Institute. Inquiries: Postal address: Institute of Social Studies P.O. Box 29776 2502 LT The Hague The Netherlands Kortenaerkade 12 2518 AX The Hague The Netherlands +31 70 426 0460 +31 70 426 0799 Location: Telephone: Fax: ii Contents Acknowledgments List of Figures List of Acronyms Abstract Key Words Chapter 1 Introduction 1.1 The Filipino Youth and Media in Recent Philippine History The Marcos Regime: Youth in the Streets The 1986 People Power: The Culmination of Protests The Post-1986 Youth: Disengaged or Disillusioned? People Power II: The Rise of GenTxt Postscript 2001: ‘People Power Fatigue?’ Media and Technology: Expanding Youth Spaces 1.2 Research Objectives and...

Words: 25337 - Pages: 102

Premium Essay

Interbrand-Best-Global-Brands-2013-Report

...offices, business schools, and conferences all over the world: What is leadership and how has it changed in the 21st century? Driven by rapid technological advancement, the digitization of nearly everything, and the ever more intricate interdependencies of the global market, the business landscape has transformed over the past two decades. Operating in a bewildering new environment in which little is certain, the pace is quicker and the dynamics more complex. Those who lead today’s brands can no longer rely on once immutable truths or principles of leadership honored in times past. It is a new world. And as purchasing increasingly shifts from a physical experience to a virtual one and transaction-based interactions between brands and consumers shift to relationship-based interactions, new skills and sensibilities are needed. Leadership roles are converging, traditional structures are crumbling, the consumer’s voice carries more weight than ever, and less tangible strengths like emotional intelligence and psychological insight are just as key to leading a brand today as the ability to generate high ROI and increased shareholder value. THE NEW RULES OF BRAND LEADERSHIP Best Global Brands 2013 Today’s leaders...

Words: 44812 - Pages: 180

Premium Essay

Emerging Patient Driven Health Care Model

...Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2009, 6, 492-525; doi:10.3390/ijerph6020492 OPEN ACCESS International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health ISSN 1660-4601 www.mdpi.com/journal/ijerph Article Emerging Patient-Driven Health Care Models: An Examination of Health Social Networks, Consumer Personalized Medicine and Quantified Self-Tracking Melanie Swan * Research Associate, MS Futures Group, P.O. Box 61258, Palo Alto, CA 94306, USA * Author to whom correspondence should be addressed; E-Mail: m@melanieswan.com; Tel.: +1-415505-4426; Fax: +1-504-910-3803 Received: 9 January 2009 / Accepted: 2 February 2009 / Published: 5 February 2009 Abstract: A new class of patient-driven health care services is emerging to supplement and extend traditional health care delivery models and empower patient self-care. Patient-driven health care can be characterized as having an increased level of information flow, transparency, customization, collaboration and patient choice and responsibility-taking, as well as quantitative, predictive and preventive aspects. The potential exists to both improve traditional health care systems and expand the concept of health care though new services. This paper examines three categories of novel health services: health social networks, consumer personalized medicine and quantified self-tracking. Keywords: Patient-driven health care; health social networks; personalized medicine; quantified self-tracking; health care delivery; predictive...

Words: 14859 - Pages: 60

Premium Essay

Global Brand

...offices, business schools, and conferences all over the world: What is leadership and how has it changed in the 21st century? Driven by rapid technological advancement, the digitization of nearly everything, and the ever more intricate interdependencies of the global market, the business landscape has transformed over the past two decades. Operating in a bewildering new environment in which little is certain, the pace is quicker and the dynamics more complex. Those who lead today’s brands can no longer rely on once immutable truths or principles of leadership honored in times past. It is a new world. And as purchasing increasingly shifts from a physical experience to a virtual one and transaction-based interactions between brands and consumers shift to relationship-based interactions, new skills and sensibilities are needed. Leadership roles are converging, traditional structures are crumbling, the consumer’s voice carries more weight than ever, and less tangible strengths like emotional intelligence and psychological insight are just as key to leading a brand today as the ability to generate high ROI and increased shareholder value. THE NEW RULES OF BRAND LEADERSHIP Best Global Brands 2013 Today’s leaders face...

Words: 44781 - Pages: 180

Premium Essay

Contemporary Business

...Learning Objectives Chapter 1 1 Distinguish between business and not-for-profit organizations. 2 Identify and describe the factors of production. 3 Describe the private enterprise system, including basic rights and entrepreneurship. 4 Identify the six eras of business, and explain how the relationship era—including alliances, technology, and environmental concerns—influences contemporary business. 5 Explain how today’s business workforce and the nature of work itself is changing. 6 Identify the skills and attributes managers need to lead businesses in the 21st century. 7 Outline the characteristics that make a company admired by the business community. iStockphoto The Changing Face of Business S nuggie: The Break-out Blanket Hit I f the first thing you do when preparing to curl up with a good textbook like this one is to reach for your Snuggie, you have plenty of company. Sales of the funky blanket with sleeves were expected to reach as high as 20 million units in just the second year the cozy accessory was available. Combined with the 5 million blankets sold in the first year, that volume will bring the value of total retail sales of the Snuggie to more than $300 million. That’s a respectable profit for an inexpensive product whose unique design and ungainly shape have made it the butt of YouTube parodies and late-night talkshow jokes. It means Allstar Marketing Group, which markets the Snuggie, is obviously doing something ...

Words: 18153 - Pages: 73