Free Essay

The Life

In:

Submitted By analuciac
Words 16523
Pages 67
JOURNAL OF SCIENTIFIC EXPLORATION

A Publication of the Society for Scientific Exploration
Instructions to Authors

(Revised February 2013)
All correspondence and submissions should be directed to:
JSE Managing Editor, EricksonEditorial@gmail.com, 151 Petaluma Blvd. So., #227, Petaluma CA 94952
USA, (1) 415/435-1604, fax (1) 707/559-5030
Please submit all manuscripts at http://journalofscientificexploration.org/index.php/jse/login (please note that “www” is NOT used in this address). This website provides directions for author registration and online submission of manuscripts. Full Author Instructions are posted on the Society for Scientific Exploration’s website at http://www.scientificexploration.org/documents/instructions_for_authors.pdf for submission of items for publication in the Journal of Scientific Exploration (including “Writing the Empirical Journal
Article.” Before you submit a paper, please familiarize yourself with the Journal by reading JSE articles.
(Back issues can be browsed in electronic form with SSE membership login at http://journalofscientificexploration.org, click on Archive link; issues before 2008 are freely accessible at http://www.scientificexploration.org/journal/articles.html) Electronic files of text, tables, and figures at resolution of a minimum of 300 dpi (TIF or PDF preferred) will be required for online submission. You will also need to attest to a statement online that the article has not been previously published and is not submitted elsewhere.
AIMS AND SCOPE: The Journal of Scientific Exploration publishes material consistent with the Society’s mission: to provide a professional forum for critical discussion of topics that are for various reasons ignored or studied inadequately within mainstream science, and to promote improved understanding of social and intellectual factors that limit the scope of scientific inquiry. Topics of interest cover a wide spectrum, ranging from apparent anomalies in well-established disciplines to paradoxical phenomena that seem to belong to no established discipline, as well as philosophical issues about the connections among disciplines. The Journal publishes research articles, review articles, essays, book reviews, and letters or commentaries pertaining to previously published material.
REFEREEING: Manuscripts will be sent to one or more referees at the discretion of the Editor-in-Chief.
Reviewers are given the option of providing an anonymous report or a signed report.
In established disciplines, concordance with accepted disciplinary paradigms is the chief guide in evaluating material for scholarly publication. On many of the matters of interest to the Society for Scientific Exploration, however, consensus does not prevail. Therefore the Journal of Scientific Exploration necessarily publishes claimed observations and proffered explanations that will seem more speculative or less plausible than those appearing in some mainstream disciplinary journals. Nevertheless, those observations and explanations must conform to rigorous standards of observational techniques and logical argument. If publication is deemed warranted but there remain points of disagreement between authors and referee(s), the reviewer(s) may be given the option of having their opinion(s) published along with the article, subject to the Editor-in-Chief ’s judgment as to length, wording, and the like. The publication of such critical reviews is intended to encourage debate and discussion of controversial issues, since such debate and discussion offer the only path toward eventual resolution and consensus.
LETTERS TO THE EDITOR intended for publication should be clearly identified as such. They should be directed strictly to the point at issue, as concisely as possible, and will be published, possibly in edited form, at the discretion of the Editor-in-Chief.
PROOFS AND AUTHOR COPIES: Authors will receipt copyedited, typeset page proofs for review.
Print copies of the published Journal will be sent to all named authors.
COPYRIGHT: Authors retain copyright to their writings. However, when an article has been submitted to the Journal of Scientific Exploration for consideration, the Journal holds first serial (periodical) publication rights. Additionally, after acceptance and publication, the Society has the right to post the article on the Internet and to make it available via electronic as well as print subscription. The material must not appear anywhere else (including on an Internet website) until it has been published by the Journal (or rejected for publication). After publication in the Journal, authors may use the material as they wish but should make appropriate reference to the prior publication in the Journal. For example: “Reprinted from
[or From] “[title of article]”, Journal of Scientific Exploration, vol. [xx], no. [xx], pp. [xx], published by the
Society for Scientific Exploration, http://www.scientificexploration.org.”
DISCLAIMER: While every effort is made by the Publisher, Editors, and Editorial Board to see that no inaccurate or misleading data, opinion, or statement appears in this Journal, they wish to point out that the data and opinions appearing in the articles and announcements herein are the sole responsibility of the contributor concerned. The Publisher, Editors, Editorial Board, and their respective employees, officers, and agents accept no responsibility or liability for the consequences of any such inaccurate or misleading data, opinion, or statement.

Writing the Empirical Journal Article
Daryl J. Bem
Cornell University
Planning Your Article
Which Article Should You Write? 2
Analyzing Data 2
Reporting the Findings 3
How Should You Write? 3
For Whom Should You Write? 3

2

Writing Your Article
The Shape of An Article 4
The Introduction 5
The Opening Statements 5
Examples of Examples 6
The Literature Review 6
Citations 6
Criticizing Previous Work 7
Ending the Introduction 7
The Method Section 7
The Results Section 8
Setting the Stage 8
Presenting the Findings 9
Figures and Tables 10
On Statistics 10
The Discussion Section 10
The Title and Abstract 11

4

Rewriting and Polishing Your Article

12

Some Matters of Style
Omit Needless Words 13
Avoid Metacomments on the Writing 14
Use Repetition and Parallel Construction 14
Jargon 15
Voice and Self-Reference 15
Tense 15
Avoid Language Bias 16
Research Participants 16
Sex and Gender 16
Racial and Ethnic Identity 17
Sexual Orientation 17
Disabilities 18
Common Errors of Grammar and Usage 18
Compared with versus Compared to 18
Data 18
Different from versus Different than 18
Since versus Because 18
That versus Which 18
While versus Although, But, Whereas 18

13

Publishing Your Article

18

References

20

.

A version of this article appears in Darley, J. M., Zanna, M. P., & Roediger III, H. L. (Eds) (2003). The Compleat Academic:
A Practical Guide for the Beginning Social Scientist, 2nd Edition. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

Writing the Empirical Journal Article

2

You have conducted a study and analyzed the data. Now it is time to write. To publish. To tell the world what you have learned. The purpose of this article is to enhance the chances that some journal editor will let you do so.
If you are new to this enterprise, you may find it helpful to consult two additional sources of information. For detailed information on the proper format of a journal article, see the Publication Manual of the American Psychological Association
(APA, 2001) and recent articles in the journal to which you plan to submit your manuscript. For renewing your acquaintance with the formal and stylistic elements of English prose, you can read Chapter 2 of the Publication Manual or any one of several style manuals. I recommend The Elements of Style by Strunk and White (2000). It is brief, witty, and inexpensive.
Because I write, review, and edit primarily for journals in personality and social psychology, I have drawn most of my examples from those areas. Colleagues assure me, however, that the guidelines set forth here are also pertinent for articles in experimental psychology and biopsychology. Similarly, this article focuses on the report of an empirical study, but the general writing suggestions apply as well to the theoretical articles, literature reviews, and methodological contributions that also appear in our journals. (Specific guidance for preparing a literature review article for Psychological Bulletin can be found in
Bem, 1995.)

Planning Your Article
Which Article Should You Write?
There are two possible articles you can write: (a) the article you planned to write when you designed your study or (b) the article that makes the most sense now that you have seen the results. They are rarely the same, and the correct answer is
(b).
The conventional view of the research process is that we first derive a set of hypotheses from a theory, design and conduct a study to test these hypotheses, analyze the data to see if they were confirmed or disconfirmed, and then chronicle this sequence of events in the journal article. If this is how our enterprise actually proceeded, we could write most of the article before we collected the data. We could write the introduction and method sections completely, prepare the results section in skeleton form, leaving spaces to be filled in by the specific numerical results, and have two possible discussion sections ready to go, one for positive results, the other for negative results.
But this is not how our enterprise actually proceeds. Psychology is more exciting than that, and the best journal articles are informed by the actual empirical findings from the opening sentence. Before writing your article, then, you need to Analyze Your Data. Herewith, a sermonette on the topic.
Analyzing Data. Once upon a time, psychologists observed behavior directly, often for sustained periods of time. No longer. Now, the higher the investigator goes up the tenure ladder, the more remote he or she typically becomes from the grounding observations of our science. If you are already a successful research psychologist, then you probably haven’t seen a participant for some time. Your graduate assistant assigns the running of a study to a bright undergraduate who writes the computer program that collects the data automatically. And like the modern dentist, the modern psychologist rarely even sees the data until they have been cleaned by human or computer hygienists.
To compensate for this remoteness from our participants, let us at least become intimately familiar with the record of their behavior: the data. Examine them from every angle. Analyze the sexes separately. Make up new composite indexes. If a datum suggests a new hypothesis, try to find additional evidence for it elsewhere in the data. If you see dim traces of interesting patterns, try to reorganize the data to bring them into bolder relief. If there are participants you don’t like, or trials, observers, or interviewers who gave you anomalous results, drop them (temporarily). Go on a fishing expedition for something—anything —interesting.
No, this is not immoral. The rules of scientific and statistical inference that we overlearn in graduate school apply to the
“Context of Justification.” They tell us what we can conclude in the articles we write for public consumption, and they give our readers criteria for deciding whether or not to believe us. But in the “Context of Discovery,” there are no formal rules, only heuristics or strategies. How does one discover a new phenomenon? Smell a good idea? Have a brilliant insight into behavior? Create a new theory? In the confining context of an empirical study, there is only one strategy for discovery: exploring the data. To put it crudely, what you do with your data in the privacy of your bedroom is nobody’s business but your own. Although I have been advocating this practice for decades, it has become more kosher in recent years as textbooks and statistical packages have come on the market explicitly designed to teach and perform “Exploratory Data Analysis.”

Writing the Empirical Journal Article

3

Yes, there is a danger. Spurious findings can emerge by chance, and we need to be cautious about anything we discover in this way. In limited cases, there are statistical techniques that correct for this danger. But there are no statistical correctives for overlooking an important discovery because we were insufficiently attentive to the data. Let us err on the side of discovery.
Reporting the Findings. When you are through exploring, you may conclude that the data are not strong enough to justify your new insights formally, but at least you are now ready to design the “right” study. If you still plan to report the current data, you may wish to mention the new insights tentatively, stating honestly that they remain to be tested adequately. Alternatively, the data may be strong enough to justify recentering your article around the new findings and subordinating or even ignoring your original hypotheses.
This is not advice to suppress negative results. If your study was genuinely designed to test hypotheses that derive from a formal theory or are of wide general interest for some other reason, then they should remain the focus of your article. The integrity of the scientific enterprise requires the reporting of disconfirming results.
But this requirement assumes that somebody out there cares about the hypotheses. Many respectable studies are explicitly exploratory or are launched from speculations of the “I-wonder-if ...” variety. If your study is one of these, then nobody cares if you were wrong. Contrary to the conventional wisdom, science does not care how clever or clairvoyant you were at guessing your results ahead of time. Scientific integrity does not require you to lead your readers through all your wrongheaded hunches only to show— voila!—they were wrongheaded. A journal article should not be a personal history of your stillborn thoughts.
Your overriding purpose is to tell the world what you have learned from your study. If your results suggest a compelling framework for their presentation, adopt it and make the most instructive findings your centerpiece. Think of your dataset as a jewel. Your task is to cut and polish it, to select the facets to highlight, and to craft the best setting for it. Many experienced authors write the results section first.
But before writing anything, Analyze Your Data!
End of sermonette.
How Should You Write?
The primary criteria for good scientific writing are accuracy and clarity. If your article is interesting and written with style, fine. But these are subsidiary virtues. First strive for accuracy and clarity.
The first step toward clarity is good organization, and the standardized format of a journal article does much of the work for you. It not only permits readers to read the report from beginning to end, as they would any coherent narrative, but also to scan it for a quick overview of the study or to locate specific information easily by turning directly to the relevant section.
Within that format, however, it is still helpful to work from an outline of your own. This enables you to examine the logic of the sequence, to spot important points that are omitted or misplaced, and to decide how best to divide the labor of presentation between the introduction and final discussion (about which, more later).
The second step toward clarity is to write simply and directly. A journal article tells a straightforward tale of a circumscribed problem in search of a solution. It is not a novel with subplots, flashbacks, and literary allusions, but a short story with a single linear narrative line. Let this line stand out in bold relief. Don’t make your voice struggle to be heard above the ambient noise of cluttered writing. You are justifiably proud of your 90th percentile verbal aptitude, but let it nourish your prose, not glut it. Write simply and directly.
For Whom Should You Write?
Scientific journals are published for specialized audiences who share a common background of substantive knowledge and methodological expertise. If you wish to write well, you should ignore this fact. Psychology encompasses a broader range of topics and methodologies than do most other disciplines, and its findings are frequently of interest to a wider public.
The social psychologist should be able to read a Psychometrika article on logistic analysis; the personality theorist, a biopsychology article on hypothalamic function; and the congressional aide with a BA in history, a Journal of Personality and Social Psychology article on causal attribution.
Accordingly, good writing is good teaching. Direct your writing to the student in Psychology 101, your colleague in the
Art History Department, and your grandmother. No matter how technical or abstruse your article is in its particulars, intelligent nonpsychologists with no expertise in statistics or experimental design should be able to comprehend the broad outlines

Writing the Empirical Journal Article

4

of what you did and why. They should understand in general terms what was learned. And above all, they should appreciate why someone—anyone—should give a damn. The introduction and discussion sections in particular should be accessible to this wider audience.
The actual technical materials—those found primarily in the method and results sections—should be aimed at a reader one level of expertise less specialized than the audience for which the journal is primarily published. Assume that the reader of your article in Psychometrika knows about regression, but needs some introduction to logistic analysis. Assume that the reader of the Journal of Personality and Social Psychology knows about person perception but needs some introduction to dispositional and situational attributions.
Many of the writing techniques suggested in this article are thus teaching techniques designed to make your article comprehensible to the widest possible audience. They are also designed to remain invisible or transparent to your readers, thereby infusing your prose with a “subliminal pedagogy.” Good writing is good teaching.

Writing Your Article
The Shape of an Article
An article is written in the shape of an hourglass. It begins with broad general statements, progressively narrows down to the specifics of your study, and then broadens out again to more general considerations. Thus:

The introduction begins broadly:

“Individuals differ radically from one another in the degree to which they are willing and able to express their emotions.”

It becomes more specific:

“Indeed, the popular view is that such emotional expressiveness is a central difference between men and women.... But the research evidence is mixed...”

And more so:

“There is even some evidence that men may actually...”

Until you are ready to introduce your own study in concep- “In this study, we recorded the emotional reactions of both tual terms: men and women to filmed...”
The method and results sections are the most specific, the (Method) “One hundred male and 100 female undergradu“neck” of the hourglass: ates were shown one of two movies...”
(Results) “Table 1 shows that men in the father-watching condition cried significantly more...”
The discussion section begins with the implications of your “These results imply that sex differences in emotional exstudy: pressiveness are moderated by two kinds of variables...”
It becomes broader:

“Not since Charles Darwin’s first observations has psychology contributed as much new...”

And more so:

“If emotions can incarcerate us by hiding our complexity, at least their expression can liberate us by displaying our authenticity.” This closing statement might be a bit grandiose for some journals—I’m not even sure what it means—but if your study is carefully executed and conservatively interpreted, most editors will permit you to indulge yourself a bit at the two broad ends of the hourglass. Being dull only appears to be a prerequisite for publishing in the professional journals.

Writing the Empirical Journal Article

5

The Introduction
The Opening Statements. The first task of the article is to introduce the background and nature of the problem being investigated. Here are four rules of thumb for your opening statements:
1. Write in English prose, not psychological jargon.
2. Do not plunge unprepared readers into the middle of your problem or theory. Take the time and space necessary to lead them up to the formal or theoretical statement of the problem step by step.
3. Use examples to illustrate theoretical points or to introduce unfamiliar conceptual or technical terms. The more abstract the material, the more important such examples become.
4. Whenever possible, try to open with a statement about people (or animals), not psychologists or their research (This rule is almost always violated. Don’t use journals as a model here.)
Examples of Opening Statements:
Wrong: Several years ago, Ekman (1972), Izard (1977), Tomkins (1980), and Zajonc (1980) pointed to psychology’s neglect of the affects and their expression. [Okay for somewhere in the introduction, but not the opening statement.]
Right: Individuals differ radically from one another in the degree to which they are willing and able to express their emotions.
Wrong: Research in the forced-compliance paradigm has focused on the effects of predecisional alternatives and incentive magnitude.
Wrong: Festinger’s theory of cognitive dissonance received a great deal of attention during the latter part of the twentieth century. Right: The individual who holds two beliefs that are inconsistent with one another may feel uncomfortable. For example, the person who knows that he or she enjoys smoking but believes it to be unhealthy may experience discomfort arising from the inconsistency or disharmony between these two thoughts or cognitions. This feeling of discomfort was called cognitive dissonance by social psychologist Leon Festinger (1957), who suggested that individuals will be motivated to remove this dissonance in whatever way they can.
Note how this last example leads the reader from familiar terms (beliefs, inconsistency, discomfort, thoughts) through transition terms (disharmony, cognitions) to the technical term cognitive dissonance, thereby providing an explicit, if nontechnical, definition of it. The following example illustrates how one might define a technical term (ego control) and identify its conceptual status (a personality variable) more implicitly:
The need to delay gratification, control impulses, and modulate emotional expression is the earliest and most ubiquitous demand that society places upon the developing child. And because success at so many of life’s tasks depends critically upon the individual’s mastery of such ego control, evidence for life-course continuities in this central personality domain should be readily obtained.

And finally, here is an example in which the technical terms are defined only by the context. Note, however, that the technical abbreviation, MAO, is still identified explicitly when it is first introduced.
In the continuing search for the biological correlates of psychiatric disorder, blood platelets are now a prime target of investigation. In particular, reduced monoamine oxidase (MAO) activity in the platelets is sometimes correlated with paranoid symptomatology, auditory hallucinations or delusions in chronic schizophrenia, and a tendency towards psychopathology in non-clinical samples of men. Unfortunately, these observations have not always replicated, casting doubt on the hypothesis that MAO activity is, in fact, a biological marker in psychiatric disorder. Even the general utility of the platelet model as a key to central nervous system abnormalities in schizophrenia remains controversial. The present study attempts to clarify the relation of MAO activity to symptomatology in chronic schizophrenia.

This kind of writing would not appear in Newsweek, and yet it is still comprehensible to an intelligent layperson who may know nothing about blood platelets, MAO activity, or biological markers. The structure of the writing itself adequately defines the relationships among these things and provides enough context to make the basic idea of the study and its rationale clear. At the same time, this introduction is not condescending nor will it bore the technically sophisticated reader. The peda-

Writing the Empirical Journal Article

6

gogy that makes this introduction accessible to the nonspecialist will not only be transparent to the specialist, but will enhance the clarity of the article for both readers.
Examples of Examples. When developing complex conceptual arguments or introducing technical materials, it is important not only to provide your readers with illustrative examples, but to select the examples with care. In particular, you should try to compose one or two examples that anticipate your actual findings and then use them recurrently to make several interrelated conceptual points. For example, in one of my own studies of trait consistency, some participants were consistently friendly but not consistently conscientious (Bem & Allen, 1974). Accordingly, we used examples of friendliness and conscientiousness throughout the introduction to clarify and illustrate our theoretical points about the subtleties of trait consistency.
This pedagogical technique strengthens the thematic coherence of an article and silently prepares the reader for understanding the results. It also shortens the article by removing the need to explain the theory once in the introduction with hypothetical examples and then again in the context of the actual results.
This article you are now reading itself provides examples of recurring examples. Although you do not know it yet, the major example will be the fictitious study of sex differences in emotional expression introduced earlier to illustrate the hourglass shape of an article. I deliberately constructed the study and provided a sufficient overview of it at the beginning so that I could draw upon it throughout the article. Watch for its elaboration as we proceed. I chose dissonance theory as a second example because most psychologists are already familiar with it; I can draw upon this shared resource without having to expend a lot of space explaining it. But just in case you are not familiar with it, I introduced it first in the context of “examples of opening statements” where I could bring you in from the beginning—just as you should do with your own readers. And finally, the Bem-Allen article on trait consistency, mentioned in the previous paragraph, has some special attributes that will earn it additional cameo appearances as we continue.
The Literature Review. After making the opening statements, summarize the current state of knowledge in the area of investigation. What previous research has been done on the problem? What are the pertinent theories of the phenomenon? Although you will have familiarized yourself with the literature before you designed your own study, you may need to look up additional references if your results raise a new aspect of the problem or lead you to recast the study in a different framework.
For example, if you discover an unanticipated sex difference in your data, you will want to determine if others have reported a similar sex difference or findings that might explain it. If you consider this finding important, discuss sex differences and the pertinent literature in the introduction. If you consider it to be only a peripheral finding, then postpone a discussion of sex differences until the discussion section.
The Publication Manual gives the following guidelines for the literature review:
Discuss the literature but do not include an exhaustive historical review. Assume that the reader is knowledgeable about the field for which you are writing and does not require a complete digest. . . . [C]ite and reference only works pertinent to the specific issue and not works of only tangential or general significance. If you summarize earlier works, avoid nonessential details; instead, emphasize pertinent findings, relevant methodological issues, and major conclusions. Refer the reader to general surveys or reviews of the topic if they are available. (APA, 2001, p. 16)

The Publication Manual also urges authors not to let the goal of brevity mislead them into writing a statement intelligible only to the specialist. One technique for describing even an entire study succinctly without sacrificing clarity is to describe one variation of the procedure in chronological sequence, letting it convey the overview of the study at the same time.
(You can use the same technique in your own method section.) Here, for example, is a possible summary of a complicated but classic experiment on cognitive dissonance theory (Festinger & Carlsmith, 1959):
Sixty male undergraduates were randomly assigned to one of three conditions. In the $1 condition, the participant was first required to perform long repetitive laboratory tasks in an individual experimental session. He was then hired by the experimenter as an “assistant” and paid $1 to tell a waiting fellow student (a confederate) that the tasks were fun and interesting. In the $20 condition, each participant was hired for $20 to do the same thing. In the control condition, participants simply engaged in the tasks.
After the experiment, each participant indicated on a questionnaire how much he had enjoyed the tasks. The results showed that
$1 participants rated the tasks as significantly more enjoyable than did the $20 participants, who, in turn, did not differ from the control participants.

This kind of condensed writing looks easy. It is not, and you will have to rewrite such summaries repeatedly before they are both clear and succinct. The preceding paragraph was my eighth draft.
Citations. The standard journal format permits you to cite authors in the text either by enclosing their last names and the year of publication in parentheses, as in A below, or by using their names in the sentence itself, as in B.

Writing the Empirical Journal Article

7

A. “MAO activity in some individuals with schizophrenia is actually higher than normal (Tse & Tung, 1949).”
B. “Tse and Tung (1949) report that MAO activity in some individuals with schizophrenia is actually higher than normal.”
In general, you should use form A, consigning your colleagues to parentheses. Your narrative line should be about MAO activity in individuals with schizophrenia, not about Tse and Tung. Occasionally, however, you might want the focus specifically on the authors or researchers: “Theophrastus (280 B.C.) implies that persons are consistent across situations, but Montaigne (1580) insists that they are not. Only Mischel (1968), Peterson (1968), and Vernon (1964), however, have actually surveyed the evidence in detail.” The point here is that you have a deliberate choice to make. Don’t just intermix the two formats randomly, paying no attention to the narrative structure.
Criticizing Previous Work. If you take a dim view of previous research or earlier articles in the domain you reviewed, feel free to criticize and complain as strongly as you feel is commensurate with the incompetence you have uncovered. But criticize the work, not the investigators or authors. Ad hominem attacks offend editors and reviewers; moreover, the person you attack is likely to be asked to serve as one of the reviewers of your article. As a consequence, your opportunity to address—let alone, offend—readers will be nipped in the bud. I could launch into a sermonette on communitarian values in science, but I shall assume that this pragmatic warning is sufficient.
Ending the Introduction. End the introduction with a brief overview of your own study. This provides a smooth transition into the method section, which follows immediately:
Because this sex difference remains elusive, it seemed desirable to test Zanna’s parental-role theory of emotional expression in a more realistic setting. Accordingly, in the study to be presented here, we exposed men and women to filmed scenes designed to evoke either negative or positive emotions and assessed their emotional reactions when they thought they were being observed by one or both of their parents. We also sought to examine the relation of emotional expression to self-esteem.

The Method Section
The Publication Manual spells out in detail what needs to be included in the method section of an article. Here are some additional stylistic suggestions.
If you conducted a fairly complex experiment in which there was a sequence of procedures or events, it is helpful to lead the reader through the sequence as if he or she were a participant. First give the usual overview of the study, including the description of participants, setting, and variables assessed, but then describe the experiment from the participant’s vantage point. Provide summaries or excerpts of what was actually said to the participant, including any rationale or “cover story” that was given. Describe the relevant aspects of the room. Show sample items from questionnaires, labels on attitude scales, copies of stimulus materials or pictures of apparatus. If you administered a standard personality test or attitude scale, describe its general properties unless it is very familiar (e.g., the MMPI or the F scale). For example: “Participants then filled out the
Marlowe-Crowne Social Desirability Scale, a true-false inventory that measures the degree to which persons describe themselves in socially desirable terms (e.g., ‘I have never lied’).”
The purpose of all this is to give your readers a feel for what it was like to be a participant. (This is true even if you used non-human participants. Thus it is more important to describe the schedule of reinforcement and the inner dimensions of the
Skinner Box—what the animal actually experienced—then its outer dimensions and the voltage of the power supply.) Such information often bears importantly on the interpretation of the behavior observed, and readers should be in a position to arrive at their own judgments about your conclusions.
Name all groups, variables, and operations with easily recognized and remembered labels. Do not use abbreviations (the
AMT5% group) or empty labels (Treatment 3). Instead, tell us about the success group and the failure group, the fatherwatching condition and the mother-watching condition, the teacher sample and the student sample, and so forth. It is also better to label groups or treatments in operational rather than theoretical terms. It is difficult to remember that it was the high dissonance group that was offered the small incentive and the low dissonance group that was offered the large incentive. So tell us instead about the $1 group and the $20 group. You can remind us of the theoretical interpretation of these variables later when you discuss the results.
The method and results sections share the responsibility for presenting certain kinds of data that support the reliability and validity of your substantive findings, and you must judge where this information fits most smoothly into the narrative and when the reader can most easily assimilate it. For example, if you constructed a new personality scale, you need to tell us about its internal homogeneity and other psychometric properties. If you employed observers, tell us about interjudge agree-

Writing the Empirical Journal Article

8

ment. If you mailed survey questionnaires, give us the return rate and discuss the possibility that non-respondents differed from respondents. If you discarded certain participants, tell us why and how many and discuss the possibility that this limits or qualifies the conclusions you can draw. In particular, assure us that they were not all concentrated in the same experimental condition. (Participants discarded during data analysis should be discussed in the results section.)
Discuss participant dropout problems and other difficulties encountered in executing the study only if they might affect the validity or the interpretation of your results. Otherwise spare us your tales of woe. Do tell us that some participants fled your high-stress treatment before you could assess their physiological response, but do not tell us that your dog ate your pigeon and you had to redo the experiment or that you couldn’t run participants Tuesday night because the custodian inadvertently locked the building.
Manipulations and procedures that yielded no useful information should be mentioned if they were administered before you collected your main data; their presence could have affected your findings. Usually it will be sufficient to say that they yielded no information and will not be discussed further. You probably do not need to mention them at all if they were administered after you collected your main data unless you think that other investigators might try to pursue the same fruitless path. Sometimes, however, a “null” result is surprising or of interest in its own right. In this case, it should be treated as a regular datum in your results section.
After presenting the methods you used in your study, discuss any ethical issues they might raise. If the research design required you to keep participants uninformed or even misinformed about the procedures, how did you tell them about this afterwards? How did you obtain their prior consent? Were they free to withdraw at any time? Were they subjected to any embarrassment or discomfort? What steps were followed to protect their anonymity? Were you observing people who were unaware of that fact?
If your study raises any of these issues, you should be prepared to justify your procedures. Moreover, you need to assure us that your participants were treated with dignity and that they left your study with their self-esteem intact and their respect for you and psychology enhanced rather than diminished. If you used non-human participants—especially dogs, cats, or primates—then you need to address analogous questions about their care and treatment.
End the method section with a brief summary of the procedure and its overall purpose. Your grandmother should be able to skim the method section without reading it; the final paragraph should bring her back “on line.”
The Results Section
In short articles or reports of single empirical studies, the results and discussion are often combined. But if you need to integrate several different kinds of results or discuss several general matters, then prepare a separate discussion section. There is, however, no such thing as a pure results section without an accompanying narrative. You cannot just throw numbers at readers and expect them to retain them in memory until they reach the discussion. In other words, write the results section in
English prose.
Setting the Stage. Before you can present your results, there are two preliminary matters that need to be handled. First, you should present evidence that your study successfully set up the conditions for testing your hypotheses or answering your questions. If your study required you to produce one group of participants in a happy mood and another in a depressed mood, show us in this section that mood ratings made by the two groups were significantly different. If you divided your participants into groups, assure us that these groups did not differ on some unintended variable that might bear upon the interpretation of your results (e.g., social class, intelligence). If your study required you to misinform participants about the procedures, how do you know that they were not suspicious, that participants who participated earlier had not informed participants who participated later, and that your “cover story” produced the state of belief required for the test of your hypotheses?
Here is also where you can put some of the data discussed in “The Method Section”: Reliabilities of testing instruments, judges, and observers; return rates on mail surveys; and participant dropout problems.
Not all of these matters need to be discussed at the beginning of the results section. In addition to data you think fit better in the method section, some of these other matters might better be postponed until the discussion section when you are considering alternative explanations of your results (e.g., the possibility that some participants became suspicious). Again, the decision of what to include is very much a matter of judgment. It is an important step, but do not overdo it. Get it out of the way as quickly as possible and get on with your story.
The second preliminary matter to deal with is the method of data analysis. First, describe any overall procedures you used to convert your raw observations into analyzable data. How were responses to your mail survey coded for analysis?

Writing the Empirical Journal Article

9

How were observers’ ratings combined? Were all measures first converted to standard scores? Some of these may also fit better into the method section and need not be repeated. Similarly, data-combining procedures that are highly specific can be postponed. If you combined three measures of anxiety into a single composite score for analysis, tell us about that later when you are about to present the anxiety data.
Next, tell us about the statistical analysis itself. If this is standard, describe it briefly (e.g., “All data were analyzed by two-way analyses of variance with sex of participant and mood induction as the independent variables”). If the analysis is unconventional or makes certain statistical assumptions your data might not satisfy, however, discuss the rationale for it, perhaps citing a reference for readers who wish to check into it further. If your method of analysis is new or likely to be unfamiliar to readers of the journal, you might need to provide a full explanation of it. Sometimes the quantitative treatment of data is a major part of an article’s contribution. Variations of multidimensional scaling, causal modeling, and circumplex representations of personality data, for example, have been more important in some articles than the data to which they were applied. In these cases, the method of analysis and its rationale have the same epistemological status as a theory and should be presented in the introduction to the article.
And finally, if the results section is complicated or divided into several parts, you may wish to provide an overview of the section: “The results are presented in three parts. The first section presents the behavioral results for the men, followed by the parallel results for the women. The final section presents the attitudinal and physiological data for both sexes combined.”
But as I shall argue, this kind of “metacommentary” should be used sparingly. In most cases, the prose itself should make it unnecessary. Presenting the Findings. The general rule in reporting your findings is to give the forest first and then the trees. This is true of the results section as a whole: Begin with the central findings, and then move to more peripheral ones. It is also true within subsections: State the basic finding first, and then elaborate or qualify it as necessary. Similarly, discuss an overall measure of aggression or whatever first, and then move to its individual components. Beginning with one of your most central results, proceed as follows:
1. Remind us of the conceptual hypothesis or the question you are asking: “It will be recalled that the men are expected to be more emotionally expressive than the women.” Or, “We ask, first, whether the men or the women are more emotionally expressive?” Note that this is a conceptual statement of the hypothesis or question.
2. Remind us of the operations performed and behaviors measured: “In particular, the men should produce more tears during the showing of the film than the women.” Or, “Do the men produce more tears during the showing of the film than the women?” Note that this is an operational statement of the hypothesis or question.
3. Tell us the answer immediately and in English: “The answer is yes.” Or, “As Table 1 reveals, men do, in fact, cry more profusely than the women.”
4. Now, and only now, speak to us in numbers. (Your grandmother can now skip to the next result in case she has forgotten her statistics or her reading glasses.): “Thus the men in all four conditions produced an average of 1.4 cc more tears than the women, F (1,112) = 5.79, p < .025.”
5. Now you may elaborate or qualify the overall conclusion if necessary: “Only in the father-watching condition did the men fail to produce more tears than the women, but a specific test of this effect failed to reach significance, t =1.58, p < .12.”
6. End each section of the results with a summary of where things stand: “Thus, except for the father-watching condition, which will be discussed below, the hypothesis that men cry more than women in response to visually-depicted grief appears to receive strong support.”
7. Lead into the next section of the results with a smooth transition sentence: “Men may thus be more expressive than women in the domain of negative emotion, but are they more expressive in the domain of positive emotion? Table 2 shows they are not...” (Again, the “bottom line” is given immediately.) As the results section proceeds, continue to summarize and
“update” the reader’s store of information frequently. The reader should not have to keep looking back to retrieve the major points of your plot line.
By structuring the results section in this way, by moving from forest to trees, by announcing each result clearly in prose before wading into numbers and statistics, and by summarizing frequently, you permit a reader to decide just how much detail he or she wants to pursue at each juncture and to skip ahead to the next main point whenever that seems desirable.

Writing the Empirical Journal Article

10

Figures and Tables. Unless a set of findings can be stated in one or two numbers, results that are sufficiently important to be stressed should be accompanied by a figure or table summarizing the relevant data. The basic rule of presentation is that a reader be able to grasp your major findings either by reading the text or by looking at the figures and tables. Thus, figures and tables must be titled and labeled clearly and completely, even if that means constructing a lengthy title or heading
(“Mean number of tears produced by two affective films as a function of affect valence, participant sex, parental observation, and self-esteem”). Within the text itself, lead the reader by the hand through a table to point out the results of interest: “As shown in the first column of Table 2, men produce more tears (2.33 cc) than women (1.89 cc).... Of particular interest is the number of tears produced when both father and mother watch (rows 3 and 4)...” Do not just wave in the general direction of the table and expect the reader to ferret out the information. For detailed information on figures and tables, see the Publication Manual (APA, 2001).
On Statistics. As you know, every comparison between groups or relationship between variables should be accompanied by its level of statistical significance. Otherwise, readers have no way of knowing whether the findings could have emerged by chance. But despite the importance of inferential statistics, they are not the heart of your narrative and should be subordinated to the descriptive results. Whenever possible, state a result first and then give its statistical significance, but in no case should you ever give the statistical test alone without interpreting it substantively. Do not tell us that the three-way interaction with sex, parent condition, and self-esteem was significant at the .05 level unless you tell us immediately and in English that men are less expressive than women in the negative conditions if father watches—but only for men with low self-esteem.
If your experiment used an analysis of variance design, your data analysis will automatically display the effects of several independent variables on a single dependent variable. If this organization is consonant with a smooth presentation of your results, lucky you. Go with it. But do not be a prisoner of ANOVA! If the narrative flows more smoothly by discussing the effects of a single independent variable on several conceptually related dependent variables, tear your ANOVA results apart and reorganize them. Statistical designs are all right in their place, but you—and your prose—are master; they are slave.
Just as your method section should give readers a feel for the procedures used, so too, the results section should give them a feel for the behavior observed. Select descriptive indexes or statistics that convey the behavior of your participants as vividly as possible. Tell us the percent of children in your study who hit the Bobo doll or the mean number of times they did so. Remind us that a score of 3.41 on your 5-point rating scale of aggression lies between “slightly aggressive” and “moderately aggressive.”
Do this even if the statistical analyses must be performed on some more indirect datum (e.g., the arcsin transform of the number of Bobo hits or the sum of three standardized aggression scores.) Display these indirect indexes, too, if you wish, but give the readers’ intuitions first priority. For example, in our study of trait consistency, we analyzed a standard-score index of individual consistency, but we discussed the results in terms of the more familiar correlation coefficient—on which no legitimate statistical analysis could be performed (Bem & Allen, 1974).
After you have presented your quantitative results, it is often useful to become more informal and briefly to describe the behavior of particular individuals in your study. Again, the point is not to prove something, but to add richness to your findings, to share with readers the feel of the behavior: “Indeed, two of the men used an entire box of Kleenex during the showing of the heart operation but would not pet the baby kitten owned by the secretary.”
The Discussion Section
As noted earlier, the discussion section can either be combined with the results section or appear separately. In either case, it forms a cohesive narrative with the introduction, and you should expect to move materials back and forth between these two sections as you rewrite and reshape the report. Topics that are central to your story will appear in the introduction and probably again in the discussion. More peripheral topics may not be brought up at all until after the presentation of the results. The discussion is also the bottom of the hourglass-shaped format and thus proceeds from specific matters about your study to more general concerns (about methodological strategies, for example) to the broadest generalizations you wish to make. The sequence of topics is often the mirror image of the sequence in the introduction.
Begin the discussion by telling us what you have learned from the study. Open with a clear statement on the support or nonsupport of the hypotheses or the answers to the questions you first raised in the introduction. But do not simply reformulate and repeat points already summarized in the results section. Each new statement should contribute something new to the reader’s understanding of the problem. What inferences can be drawn from the findings? These inferences may be at a level quite close to the data or may involve considerable abstraction, perhaps to the level of a larger theory regarding, say, emotion or sex differences. What are the theoretical, practical, or even the political implications of the results?

Writing the Empirical Journal Article

11

It is also appropriate at this point to compare your results with those reported by other investigators and to discuss possible shortcomings of your study, conditions that might limit the extent of legitimate generalization or otherwise qualify your inferences. Remind readers of the characteristics of your participant sample, the possibility that it might differ from other populations to which you might want to generalize; of specific characteristics of your methods that might have influenced the outcome; or of any other factors that might have operated to produce atypical results.
But do not dwell compulsively on every flaw! In particular, be willing to accept negative or unexpected results without a tortured attempt to explain them away. Do not make up long, involved, pretzel-shaped theories to account for every hiccup in the data. There is a -.73 correlation between the clarity of an investigator’s results and the length of his or her discussion section. Do not contribute to this shameful statistic.
Ah, but suppose that, on the contrary, your results have led you to a grand new theory that injects startling clarity into your data and revolutionizes your view of the problem area. Doesn’t this justify a long discussion section? No. In this case, you should write the article so that you begin with your new theory. As noted above, your task is to provide the most informative and compelling framework for your study from the opening sentence. If your new theory does that, don’t wait until the discussion section to spring it on us. A journal article is not a chronology of your thought processes.
The discussion section also includes a consideration of questions that remain unanswered or that have been raised by the study itself, along with suggestions for the kinds of research that would help to answer them. In fact, suggesting additional research is probably the most common way of ending a research report.
Common, but dull. The hourglass shape of an article implies that your final words should be broad general statements of near-cosmic significance, not precious details of interest only to psychologists. Thus the statement, “Further research will be needed before it is clear whether the androgyny scale should be scored as a single continuous dimension or partitioned into a
4-way typology,” might well be appropriate somewhere in a discussion section, but, please, not your final farewell. In my opinion, only Montaigne was clever enough to end an article with a statement about further research: “Because [the study of motivation] is a high and hazardous undertaking, I wish fewer people would meddle with it” (1580/1943).
You should probably settle for more modest injunctions: “If gender schema theory has a political message, it is...that... human behaviors and personality attributes should no longer be linked with gender, and society should stop projecting gender into situations irrelevant to genitalia. The feminist prescription, then, is not that the individual be androgynous, but that the society be gender-aschematic” (S. Bem, 1985).
In any case, end with a bang, not a whimper.
The Title and Abstract
The title and abstract of your article permit potential readers to get a quick overview of your study and to decide if they wish to read the article itself. Titles and abstracts are also indexed and compiled in reference works and computerized databases. For this reason they should accurately reflect the content of the article and include key words that will ensure their retrieval from a database. You should compose the title and abstract after you have completed the article and have a firm view of its structure and content.
The recommended length for a title is 10 to 12 words. It should be fully explanatory when standing alone and identify the theoretical issues or the variables under investigation. Because you will not be able to mention all the features of your study in the title (or even in the abstract), you must decide which are most important. Once again, the data should guide you.
For example, the most instructive findings from our fictitious study on emotional expression should determine which of the following is the most appropriate title: “Laughing versus Crying: Sex Differences in the Public Display of Positive and Negative Emotions”; “Effects of Being Observed by Parents on the Emotional Responses of Men and Women to Visual Stimuli”;
“Emotional Responses to Visual Stimuli as a Function of Sex and Self-Esteem”; “Sex Differences in the Public Display of
Emotion as a Function of the Observing Audience”; “Public versus Private Displays of Emotion in Men and Women.”
The abstract of an empirical article should not exceed 120 words. It should contain the problem under investigation (in one sentence if possible); the participants, specifying pertinent characteristics, such as number, type, age, sex, and species; the experimental method, including the apparatus, data-gathering procedures, and complete test names; the findings, including statistical significance levels; and the conclusion and the implications or applications.
Clearly the abstract must be compact, and this requirement leads many inexperienced writers to make it unintelligible.
Remove unnecessary words and eliminate less important details of method and results. But then let it breathe. In particular,

Writing the Empirical Journal Article

12

allow yourself the space to make the problem under investigation clear to a casually browsing reader. Often you can plagiarize and abbreviate key statements from the article itself. Here is an example:
When are men more emotionally expressive than women? One hundred male and 100 female undergraduates were individually shown a sad or a happy film, while being observed by one or both of their parents. Judges blind to condition rated participants’ facial expressions, and a Lachrymeter measured their tear volume. Men cried more during the sad movie but laughed less during the happy movie than did the women (interaction, p < .02). However, men in the father-watching condition with low self-esteem
(Darley Self-Concept Scale) cried less than all other participants (p < .05). It is suggested that sex differences in emotional expression are moderated by the valence of the emotion and—for men—by self-esteem and conditions of being observed.

If the conceptual contribution of your article is more important than the supporting study, this can be reflected in the abstract by omitting experimental details and giving more space to the theoretical material. Here is the title and abstract from our Psychological Review article on trait consistency (revised to conform to the new 100 word limit on abstracts of reviews and theoretical articles):
On Predicting Some of the People Some of the Time: The Search for Cross-Situational Consistencies in Behavior
The recurring controversy over the existence of cross-situational consistencies in behavior is sustained by the discrepancy between our intuitions, which affirm their existence, and the research literature, which does not. It is argued that the nomothetic assumptions of the traditional research paradigm are incorrect and that higher validity coefficients would be obtained if the idiographic assumptions used by our intuitions were adopted. A study is reported which shows it is possible to predict who will be cross-situationally consistent and who will not. Personality assessment must not only attend to situations—as has been recently urged—but to persons as well (Bem & Allen, 1974).

Rewriting and Polishing Your Article
For many authors revising an article is unmitigated agony. Even proofreading is painful. And so they don’t. So relieved to get a draft done, they send it off to the journal thinking that they can clean up the writing after it has been accepted. Alas, that day rarely comes. Some may find solace in the belief that the manuscript probably would have been rejected even if it had been extensively revised and polished; after all, most psychology journals accept only 15% to 20% of all manuscripts submitted. But from my experience as an editor, I believe that the difference between the manuscripts accepted and the top 15 to 20% of those rejected is frequently the difference between good and less good writing. Moral: Do not expect journal reviewers to discern your brilliance through the smog of polluted writing. Revise your manuscript. Polish it. Proofread it.
Then submit it.
Rewriting is difficult for several reasons. First, it is difficult to edit your own writing. You will not notice ambiguities and explanatory gaps because you know what you meant to say and you understand the omitted steps. One strategy for overcoming this difficulty is to lay your manuscript aside for awhile and then return to it later when it has become less familiar.
Sometimes it helps to read it aloud. But there is no substitute for practicing the art of taking the role of the nonspecialist reader, for learning to role-play grandma. As you read, ask yourself, “Have I been told yet what this concept means?” Has the logic of this step been demonstrated?” “Would I know what the independent variable is at this point?” This is precisely the skill of the good lecturer in Psychology 101, the ability to anticipate the audience’s level of understanding at each point in the presentation. Good writing is good teaching.
But because this is not easy, you should probably give a fairly polished copy of the manuscript to a friend or colleague for a critical review. (If you get a critique from two colleagues you will have simulated a trial run of a journal’s review process.) The best readers are those who have themselves published in the psychological journals, but who are unfamiliar with the subject of your article. (A student from Psychology 101 would probably be too intimidated to give usefully critical feedback; grandma will be too kind.)
If your colleagues find something unclear, do not argue with them. They are right: By definition, the writing is unclear.
Their suggestions for correcting the unclarities may be wrong, even stupid. But as unclarity detectors, readers are never wrong. Also resist the temptation simply to clarify their confusion verbally. Your colleagues do not want to offend you or appear stupid, and so they will simply mumble “oh yes, of course, of course” and apologize for not having read carefully enough. As a consequence, you will be pacified, and your next readers, the journal reviewers, will stumble over the same problem. They will not apologize; they will reject.

Writing the Empirical Journal Article

13

Rewriting is difficult for a second reason: It requires a high degree of compulsiveness and attention to detail. The probability of writing a sentence perfectly the first time is vanishingly small, and good writers rewrite nearly every sentence of an article in the course of polishing successive drafts. But even good writers differ from one another in their approach to the first draft. Some spend a long time carefully choosing each word and reshaping each sentence and paragraph as they go. Others pound out a rough draft quickly and then go back for extensive revision. Although I personally prefer the former method, I think it wastes time. For journal articles in particular, I think most authors should get the first draft done as quickly as possible without agonizing over stylistic niceties. But once it is done, compulsiveness and attention to detail become the required virtues. Finally, rewriting is difficult because it usually means restructuring. Sometimes it is necessary to discard whole sections of an article, add new ones, go back and do more data analysis, and then totally reorganize the article just to iron out a bump in the logic of the argument. Do not get so attached to your first draft that you are unwilling to tear it apart and rebuild it.
(This is why the technique of crafting each sentence of a first draft wastes time. That beautiful turn of phrase that took me 40 minutes to shape gets discarded when the article gets restructured. Worse, I get so attached to the phrase that I resist restructuring until I can find a new home for it.) A badly constructed building cannot be salvaged by brightening up the wallpaper.
A badly constructed article cannot be salvaged by changing words, inverting sentences, and shuffling paragraphs.
Which brings me to the word processor. Its very virtuosity at making these cosmetic changes may tempt you to tinker endlessly, encouraging you in the illusion that you are restructuring right there in front of the monitor. Do not be fooled. You are not. A word processor—even in conjunction with a fancy “outline mode”—is not an adequate restructuring tool. Moreover, it can produce flawless, physically beautiful drafts of wretched writing, encouraging you in the illusion that they are finished manuscripts ready to be submitted. Do not be fooled. They are not. If you are blessed with an excellent memory (or a very large monitor) and are confident that you can get away with a purely electronic process of restructuring, fine, do it. But do not be ashamed to print out a complete draft of your manuscript, spread it out on table or floor, take pencil, scissors, and scotch tape in hand, and then, all by your low-tech self, have at it.

Some Matters of Style
Omit Needless Words
Virtually all experienced writers agree that any written expression that deserves to be called vigorous writing, whether it is a short story, an article for a professional journal, or a complete book, is characterized by the attribute of being succinct, concise, and to the point. A sentence—no matter where in the writing it occurs—should contain no unnecessary or superfluous words, words that stand in the way of the writer’s direct expression of his or her meaning and purpose. In a very similar fashion, a paragraph—the basic unit of organization in English prose—should contain no unnecessary or superfluous sentences, sentences that introduce peripheral content into the writing or stray from its basic narrative line. It is in this sense that a writer is like an artist executing a drawing, and it is in this sense that a writer is like an engineer designing a machine. Good writing should be economical for the same reason that a drawing should have no unnecessary lines, and good writing should be streamlined in the same way that a machine is designed to have no unnecessary parts, parts that contribute little or nothing to its intended function.
This prescription to be succinct and concise is often misunderstood and requires judicious application. It certainly does not imply that the writer must make all of his or her sentences short and choppy or leave out all adjectives, adverbs, and qualifiers. Nor does it mean that he or she must avoid or eliminate all detail from the writing and treat his or her subjects only in the barest skeleton or outline form. But the requirement does imply that every word committed to paper should tell something new to the reader and contribute in a significant and non-redundant way to the message that the writer is trying to convey. ***
You have just read a 303 word essay on brevity. It is not a terrible first draft, but a good writer or copy editor would take its message to heart and, by crossing out all the non-italicized words, cut it by 81%. Savor the result:
Vigorous writing is concise. A sentence should contain no unnecessary words, a paragraph no unnecessary sentences, for the same reason that a drawing should have no unnecessary lines and a machine no unnecessary parts. This requires not that the writer make all sentences short or avoid all detail and treat subjects only in outline, but that every word tell. [59 words]

Writing the Empirical Journal Article

14

This essay on brevity was written by Strunk and White (2000, p. 23) under the heading: “Omit Needless Words.” Obey their injunction, because it is the most important piece of advice in this article. Journal articles should also omit needless concepts, topics, anecdotes, asides, and footnotes. Clear any underbrush that clutters your narrative. If a point seems peripheral to your main theme, remove it. If you cannot bring yourself to do this, put it in a footnote. Then when you revise your manuscript, remove the footnote.
Copy editing other people’s writing is good practice for improving your own. It is also less painful than editing your own and much easier than actually writing. Any piece of prose will do. Here was an exercise for my writing class; it was part of a letter Cornell sent out to potential graduate applicants. You may wish to try your hand at it.
Psychology is a wide field of study, and we are not equally strong in all parts of it. At present, we regard our major strengths as lying in three broadly defined domains in which we have many faculty, and a couple of smaller areas in which also have appreciable resources. The three primary areas are Biopsychology, Experimental Psychology, and Personality and Social Psychology; the others are Mathematical/Differential Psychology and Experimental Psychopathology. The areas and the relevant faculty are listed below. Please note that this listing is informal; it does not imply that the listed faculty members have no other interests or can readily be fitted into predefined areas. The actual network of faculty interests and responsibilities is too subtle to be described in a letter such as this. The listing is just a rough and ready way to tell you what the Field of Psychology at Cornell is like. [149 words] Here is a reasonable revision:
Psychology is a wide field and our major strengths are Biopsychology, Experimental Psychology, and Personality and Social
Psychology. We also have resources in Mathematical/Differential Psychology and Experimental Psychopathology. The following list of faculty within areas provides a rough guide to the Field of Psychology at Cornell. Faculty interests are broader than this list implies, however, and do not always neatly fit the predefined areas. [65 words, a savings of 56%]

To maintain the vigor of your prose, try to spend at least 15 minutes each day omitting needless words. Your goal should be to eliminate at least 30% of all words encountered. (Copy edited versions of this article will be returned unopened.)
Avoid Metacomments on the Writing
Expository writing fails its mission if it diverts the reader’s attention to itself and away from the topic; the process of writing should be transparent to the reader. In particular, the prose itself should direct the flow of the narrative without requiring you to play tour guide by commenting on it. Do not say, “Now that I have discussed the three theories of emotion, we can turn to the empirical work on each of them. I will begin with the psychoanalytic account of affect...” Instead, move directly from your discussion of the theories into the literature review with a simple transition sentence such as, “Each of these three theories has been tested empirically. Thus, the psychoanalytic account of affect has received support in studies that...” Do not say, “Now that we have seen the results for negative affect, we are in a position to examine men’s and women’s emotional expression in the realm of positive affect. The relevant data are presented in Table 2...” Instead use a transition sentence that simultaneously summarizes and moves the story along: “Men may thus be more expressive than women in the domain of negative emotion, but are they also more expressive in the domain of positive emotion? Table 2 shows that they are not...”
Any other guideposts needed can be supplied by using informative headings and by following the advice on repetition and parallel construction given in the next section.
If you feel the need to make metacomments to keep the reader on the narrative path, then your plot line is probably already too cluttered, the writing insufficiently linear. Metacomments will only oppress the prose further. Instead, copy edit.
Omit needless words; don’t add them!
Use Repetition and Parallel Construction
Inexperienced writers often substitute synonyms for recurring words and vary their sentence structure in the mistaken belief that this is more creative, stylish, or interesting. Instead of using repetition and parallel construction, as in “Men may be more expressive than women in the domain of negative emotion, but they are not more expressive in the domain of positive emotion,” they attempt to be more creative: “Men may be more expressive than women in the domain of negative emotion, but it is not true that they are more willing and able than the opposite sex to display the more cheerful affects.”
Such creativity is hardly more interesting, but it is certainly more confusing. In scientific communication, it can be deadly. When an author uses different words to refer to the same concept in a technical article—where accuracy is paramount—readers will justifiably wonder if different meanings are implied. The example above is not disastrous, and most

Writing the Empirical Journal Article

15

readers will be unaware that their understanding flickered momentarily when the prose hit a bump. But consider the cognitive burden carried by readers who must hack through this “creative” jungle:
The high-dissonance participants were paid a small sum of money while being given a free choice of whether or not to participate, whereas the participants we randomly assigned to the large-incentive treatment (the low-dissonance condition) were not offered the opportunity to refuse.

This (fictitious) author should have written:
High dissonance participants were paid a small sum of money and were not required to participate; low-dissonance participants were paid a large sum of money and were required to participate.

The wording and grammatical structure of the two clauses are held rigidly parallel; only the variables vary. Repetition and parallel construction are among the most effective servants of clarity. Don’t be creative, be clear.
Repetition and parallel construction also serve clarity at a larger level of organization. By providing the reader with distinctive guideposts to the structure of the prose, they can diminish or eliminate the need for metacomments. Here, for example, are the opening sentences from three of the paragraphs in the earlier section on rewriting:
2nd paragraph: “Rewriting is difficult for several reasons. First...”
5th paragraph: “Rewriting is difficult for a second reason:”
6th paragraph: “Finally, rewriting is difficult because it...”
If I had substituted synonyms for the recurring words or varied the grammatical structure of these opening sentences, their guiding function would have been lost, the reader’s sense of the section’s organization blurred. (I try so hard to be helpful, and I bet you didn’t even notice. That, of course, is the point.)
Finally, repetition and parallel construction can serve style and creativity as well as clarity. They can provide rhythm and punch: “A sentence should contain no unnecessary words, a paragraph no unnecessary sentences for the same reason that a drawing should have no unnecessary lines and a machine no unnecessary parts.” They can establish metaphor: “A badly constructed building cannot be salvaged by brightening up the wallpaper. A badly constructed article cannot be salvaged by changing words, inverting sentences, and shuffling paragraphs.” They can add humor: “The word processor encourages you in the illusion that you are restructuring. Do not be fooled. You are not. The word processor encourages you in the illusion that your drafts are finished manuscripts. Do not be fooled. They are not.”
Jargon
Jargon is the specialized vocabulary of a discipline, and it serves a number of legitimate functions in scientific communication. A specialized term may be more general, more precise, or freer of surplus meaning than any natural language equivalent (e.g., the term disposition encompasses, and hence is more general than, beliefs, attitudes, moods, and personality attributes; reinforcement is more precise and freer of surplus meaning than reward). And the technical vocabulary often makes an important conceptual distinction not apprehended by the layperson’s vocabulary (e.g., genotype versus phenotype).
But if a jargon term does not satisfy any of these criteria, opt for English. Much psychological jargon has become second-nature to us in the profession and serves only to muddy our prose for the general reader. (I once had to interrogate an author at length to learn that a prison program for “strengthening the executive functions of the ego” actually taught prisoners how to fill out job applications.) Unless the jargon term is extremely well known (e.g., reinforcement), it should be defined— explicitly, implicitly, or by example—the first time it is introduced. (See the sample opening statements earlier in this article for ways to do this.)
Voice and Self-Reference
In the past, scientific writers used the passive voice almost exclusively and referred to themselves in the third person:
“This experiment was designed by the authors to test ...” This practice produces lifeless prose and is no longer the norm. Use the active voice unless style or content dictates otherwise; and, in general, keep self-reference to a minimum. Remember that you are not the topic of your article. You should not refer to yourself as “the author” or “the investigator.” (You may refer to
“the experimenter” in the method section, however, even if that happens to be you; the experimenter is part of the topic under discussion there.) Do not refer to yourself as “we” unless there really are two or more authors. You may refer to yourself as

Writing the Empirical Journal Article

16

“I,” but do so sparingly. It tends to distract the reader from the topic, and it is better to remain in the background. Leave the reader in the background, too. Do not say, “The reader will find it hard to believe that ... “ or “You will be surprised to learn...” (This article violates the rule because you and your prose are the topic.) You may, however, refer to the reader indirectly in imperative, “you-understood” sentences: “Consider, first, the results for women.” “Note particularly the difference between the means in Table 1.”
In some contexts, you can use “we” to refer collectively to yourself and your readers: “We can see in Table 1 that most of the tears...” The Publication Manual, however, emphasizes that the referent of “we” must be unambiguous; for example, copy editors will object to the sentence “In everyday life, of course, we tend to overestimate...” because it is not clear just who is meant by “we.” They will accept “In everyday life, of course, we humans tend to overestimate...” or “In everyday life, of course, human decision makers often make errors; for example, we tend to overestimate...”
Tense
Use the past or present perfect tense when reporting the previous research of others (“Bandura reported...” or “Hardin has reported…”) and past tense when reporting how you conducted your study (“Observers were posted behind...”) and specific past behaviors of your participants (“Two of the men talked...”). Use the present tense for results currently in front of the reader (“As Table 2 shows, the negative film is more effective ...”) and for conclusions that are more general than the specific results (“Positive emotions, then, are more easily expressed when...”).
Avoid Language Bias
Like most publishers, the APA now has extensive guidelines for language that refers to individuals or groups. If your article requires you to discuss any of the groups mentioned in this section, you should probably consult the detailed advice in the Publication Manual (APA, 2001, pp. 61-76)
Research Participants. One distinctive group of people who appear in our journal articles are those whom we study. It is no longer considered appropriate to objectify them by calling them subjects. Instead use descriptive terms that either identify them more specifically or that acknowledge their roles as partners in the research process, such as college students, children, individuals, participants, interviewees, or respondents. You may still use the terms subjects, subject variables, and subject sample when discussing statistics or (at least for now) when referring to non-human participants.
Sex and Gender. The issue of language bias comes up most frequently with regard to sex or gender, and the most awkward problems arise from the use of masculine nouns and pronouns when the content refers to both sexes. The generic use of man, he, his, and him to refer to both sexes is not only misleading in many instances, but research shows that readers think of male persons when these forms are used (Martyna, 1978). Sometimes the results are not only sexist, but humorous in their naive androcentrism: “Man’s vital needs include food, water, and access to females” (Quoted in Martyna, 1978).
In most contexts, the simplest alternative is the use of the plural. Instead of writing, “The individual who displays prejudice in his personal relations...,” write “Individuals who display prejudice in their personal relations are ...” Sometimes the pronoun can simply be dropped or replaced by a sex-neutral article (the, a, or an). Instead of writing, “The researcher must avoid letting his preconceptions bias his interpretation of results,” you can write, “The researcher must avoid letting preconceptions bias the interpretation of results.”
If it is stylistically important to focus on the single individual, the use of “he or she,” “him or her,” and so forth is acceptable but clumsy if used often. Alternating he and she is both confusing and distracting. Similarly, alternatives like he/she or s/he are unpronounceable and grate on the eye. Do not use them.
You may find it instructive to review how I have dealt with the pronoun problem in this article. In particular, note the many references to the “reader” or “readers.” Sometimes the plural worked fine (e.g., “Don’t plunge readers into the middle of your problem. Take the time to lead them...”), but in other instances the imagery of the sentence required the stylistic use of the singular (e.g., “Lead the reader by the hand through a table...”). In these cases, I have tried to minimize the use of the awkward “he or she” construction.
Stylistic matters aside, however, you must be accurate in your use of pronouns when you describe your research or that of others. Readers must be explicitly told the sex of experimenters, observers, and participants. When referring to males, use male pronouns; when referring to females use female pronouns. (See, for example, the earlier description of the FestingerCarlsmith study, which used male participants.) Under no circumstances should you omit or hide sex identity in a misguided attempt to be unbiased.

Writing the Empirical Journal Article

17

The problems of gender reference become easier when we move away from pronouns. Words like man and mankind are easily replaced by terms like people and humanity. Instead of manning projects, we can staff them. The federal government has already desexed occupational titles so that we have letter carriers rather than mailmen; in private industry we have flight attendants rather than stewardesses. And in life, children need nurturing or parenting, not just mothering. In all these cases, you will find it easy to discover the appropriate sex-neutral term if you think of the activity rather than the person doing it.
Next, watch out for plain old stereotyping. The author who asserts that “research scientists often neglect their wives” fails to acknowledge that women as well as men are research scientists. If the author meant specifically male research scientists, he (she?) should have said so. Do not talk about ambitious men and aggressive women or cautious men and timid women if the use of different adjectives denotes not different behaviors on the part of men and women but your stereotyped interpretation of those behaviors. Do not make stereotyped assumptions about marital sex-roles by saying that “The client’s husband lets her teach part-time” if all you know is that the client teaches part-time. If the bias is not yours but someone else’s, let the writing make that clear: “The client’s husband ‘lets’ her teach part-time.” “The husband says he ‘lets’ the client teach part-time.” “The client says her husband lets her teach part-time.” “The client says sarcastically that her husband ‘lets’ her teach part-time.” The client and her husband are allowed to say such things. You are not.
Finally, select examples with care. Beware of your assumptions about the sex of doctors, homemakers, nurses, and so forth. Why not: “The athlete who believes in her ability to succeed...”? Let our writing promote the view that woman’s vital needs are the same as man’s: food, water, and access to equality.
Racial and Ethnic Identity. Preferences for names referring to racial and ethnic groups change over time. For example,
African American and Black are currently acceptable terms, whereas Negro and Afro-American are now obsolete. Similarly,
Asian and Asian American are currently acceptable designations, but Oriental is not. As these examples illustrate, hyphens should not be used in multiword designations, and terms such as Black and White are considered proper nouns and should be capitalized. Depending on their historical countries of origin, individuals may prefer to be called Hispanic, Latino, or Chicano
(Latina and Chicana for women). American Indian and Native American are both accepted terms for referring to indigenous people of North America, but technically only the latter category includes Hawaiians and Samoans. Native peoples of northern Canada, Alaska, eastern Siberia, and Greenland may prefer Inuk (plural, Inuit) to Eskimo. Alaska Natives include many groups in addition to Eskimos. It is often relevant to be more specific in describing your participants. For example, it may be pertinent to know that they were Cuban, not just Hispanic; Chinese, Vietnamese, or Korean, not just Asian.
If you are uncertain about how to describe your research participants, ask them how they prefer to be identified.
Sexual Orientation. Like terms referring to racial and ethnic identity, terms referring to sexual orientation also change over time. For example, the term sexual orientation itself is now preferred to the older sexual preference—which implies a temporary free choice rather than an enduring disposition. Although terms such as homosexual and homosexuality are still technically correct and may be used in phrases such as “a homosexual orientation” or “more Americans now accept homosexuality,” they should be avoided when referring to individuals or groups. Instead of referring to homosexuals or bisexuals, use lesbians, gay men, or bisexual men and women. In some contexts, the word gay can be used to include both men and women (e. g., “the gay rights movement”), but when referring to individuals or groups, retain the distinction between gay men and lesbians.
Sexual orientation is not the same as sexual behavior. Not everyone who engages in a sexual act with a person of the same sex should be considered gay or lesbian, and hence you should not use the terms homosexual behavior or heterosexual behavior. Instead, describe specific instances of sexual behavior using terms such as male-male, female-female, male-female, or same-gender sexual behavior.
Note the use of the word gender in the previous sentence. Although there are differences in usage across and within disciplines, the term sex is usually considered to refer to biology; thus male and female are terms referring to sex. In contrast, gender usually refers to the cultural interpretation or elaboration of sex; thus masculine and feminine are terms referring to gender. This is not a hard and fast rule, however. For example, because the term sex can be confused with sexual behavior, the term same-gender sexual behavior was used in the previous paragraph even though it refers to a sexual interaction between two people of the same biological sex. The following example from the Publication Manual also illustrates the use of gender in a context where the term sex might be confusing: “In accounting for attitudes toward the bill, sexual orientation rather than gender accounted for most of the variance. Most gay men and lesbians were for the proposal; most heterosexual men and women were against it (APA, 2001, p. 63).”

Writing the Empirical Journal Article

18

Disabilities. When referring to individuals with disabilities, maintain their integrity as individuals and human beings by avoiding language that equates them with their conditions. Don’t use nouns such as neurotics, schizophrenics, manicdepressives, the mentally retarded, or even the disabled. Also avoid terms that imply victimization (e.g., “suffers from schizophrenia,” “AIDS victim”) or that can be interpreted as a slur (e.g., cripple). In general, the preferred forms of description are “person with _______” or “person living with ______” or “person who has________.” Challenged and special are often considered euphemistic and should be used only if preferred by those who participate in your study.
There is one exception to these guidelines: the Deaf. (Note the capital “D.”) Although some individuals with reduced hearing may find the term hearing impaired acceptable, many Deaf individuals do not. They regard themselves as members of a distinctive linguistic culture that communicates in a manual (sign) language. Accordingly, they take pride in referring to themselves as Deaf and do not regard themselves as impaired or disabled. Do not use the terms hard of hearing or deaf mute to describe them. If your study involved Deaf participants, ask them how they prefer to be described.
Common Errors of Grammar and Usage
The following errors seem to me to be the most frequent in journal writing:
Compared with versus Compared to. Similar orders of things are compared with one another; different orders of things are compared to one another: “Let me not compare thee with previous lovers I have had; rather, let me compare thee to a summer’s day.” “Mischel’s articles are often compared with Bandura’s articles; Bem’s articles are often compared to Mozart’s sonatas.”
Data. The word data is plural: “Analyze those data thoroughly.”
Different from versus Different than. The first is correct, the second, incorrect (although, alas for us purists, very common and gaining respectability). The confusion arises because than correctly follows comparative adjectives. Thus you are correct to suppose that life is more than psychology, that living a good life is harder in many respects than writing a good article, and that living well requires broader skills than does writing well. Just remember that life is different from psychology, that living a good life is different in many respects from writing a good article, and that living well requires skills different from those required for writing well.
Since versus Because. Since means “after that.” It should not be used as a substitute for because if there is any ambiguity of interpretation. Wrong (but at least not ambiguous): “Since the study of motivation is a high and hazardous undertaking, I wish fewer people would meddle with it.” Better: “Because the study of motivation is a high and hazardous undertaking, I wish fewer people would meddle with it.” Ambiguous: “Since I read Montaigne, I have been tempted to abandon the study of motivation.” This last case is correct if the writer is using since in the temporal sense: “Ever since reading Montaigne, I have been tempted ...” It is incorrect if the writer means because.
That versus Which. That clauses (called restrictive) are essential to the meaning of the sentence; which clauses (called nonrestrictive) merely add additional information. The following example illustrates the correct use of both words: “Dissonance theory, which has received major attention, is one of the theories that postulates a motivational process. Thus, if a person holds two cognitions that are inconsistent...” Most which’s in journal writing are incorrect. You should go on a which hunt in your own manuscripts and turn most of them into that’s.
While versus Although, But, Whereas. While means “at the same time” and in most cases cannot substitute for these other words. Wrong: “While inferential statistics are important, descriptive statistics are the heart of your narrative.” Right: “Although inferential statistics are important, descriptive statistics are the heart of your narrative.” Or, “Inferential statistics are important, but descriptive statistics are the heart of your narrative.” Wrong: “While I like personality traits, Mischel prefers a social learning approach.” Right: “Whereas I like personality traits, Mischel prefers a social learning approach.” On the other hand, the following usage is correct: “While I like personality traits, I find merit in Mischel’s social learning approach.” This can be seen by substituting “at the same time” for “while”: “I like personality traits; at the same time, I find merit in
Mischel’s social learning approach.”
Whoever versus. Whomever. Even those who use “who” and “whom” correctly often have difficulties with “whoever” and “whomever” in sentences like “I plan to vote for [whoever/whomever] puts forth the best policies.” The rule is that the object of the preposition is the entire phrase “whoever puts forth the best policies,” and “whoever” is the correct choice here because it refers to the candidate “who puts forth the best policies.” In contrast, “whomever” is correct in “I will vote for whomever the party nominates,” because it refers to the person “whom the party nominates.”

Writing the Empirical Journal Article

19

I, me, we, us. There are several grammatical errors that are actually committed more frequently by those who aspire to grammatical literacy than by those who just don’t know or care. These are individuals who, when as children, said to their parents “Me and Billy want to go out and play” were immediately corrected: “You should say that ‘Billy and I want to go out to play.’” Instead of inferring the correct rule—that the use of “I” and “me” does not change when these words appear in a compound subject or object—these children simply learned that the word “me” is forbidden in compound phrases—even when the compound is used as direct or indirect object. Thus as adults, they now say (incorrectly) that “The family invited my brother and I to stay for the weekend.” That is, they scrupulously avoid the correct “The family invited my brother and me to stay for the weekend” even though their ear would immediately alert them to the fact that “They invited I to stay for the weekend” is incorrect and “They invited me to stay for the weekend” is correct. They make the same error in sentences in which the compound is the object of a preposition: “The host was very gracious to my sister and I.” Then there are some folks who are unsure of which is correct, so they avoid the decision by saying incorrectly “The host was very gracious to my sister and myself.” “ A similar error arises in the use of “we” and “us,” where “we” is treated as a good word and “us” a bad word, leading to: “The war has had a negative effect on we Americans.” Again the ear would immediately catch the error if not for the extra word “American”: “The war has had a negative effect on we.”
.

Publishing Your Article
Long ago and far away, a journal editor allegedly accepted a manuscript that required no revisions. I believe the author was William James. In other words, if your article is provisionally accepted for publication “pending revisions in accord with the reviewers’ comments,” you should be deliriously happy. Publication is now virtually under your control. If your article is rejected but you are invited to resubmit a revised version, you should still be happy—if not deliriously so—because you still have a reasonable shot at getting it published.
But this is the point at which many authors give up. As one former editor noted, in my experience as an associate editor, I thought a good deal of variance in predicting eventual publication came from this phase of the process. Authors are often discouraged by negative feedback and miss the essential positive fact that they have been asked to revise! They may never resubmit at all, or may let an inordinate amount of time pass before they do (during which editors and reviewers become unavailable, lose the thread of the project, and so forth). An opposite problem is that some authors become defensive and combative, and refuse to make needed changes for no reason.

So do not give up at this point. Feel free to complain to your colleagues or rail at your poodle because the stupid reviewers failed to read your manuscript correctly. But then turn to the task of revising your manuscript with a dispassionate, problem-solving approach. First, pay special attention to criticisms or suggestions made by more than one reviewer or highlighted by the editor in the cover letter. These must be addressed in your revision—even if not in exactly the way the editor or reviewers suggest.
Second, look carefully at each of the reviewers’ misreadings. I argued earlier that whenever readers of a manuscript find something unclear, they are right; by definition, the writing is unclear. The problem is that readers themselves do not always recognize or identify the unclarities explicitly. Instead, they misunderstand what you have written and then make a criticism or offer a suggestion that makes no sense. In other words, you should also interpret reviewers’ misreadings as signals that your writing is unclear.
Think of your manuscript as a pilot experiment in which the pilot participants (reviewers) did not understand the instructions you gave them. Analyze the reasons for their misunderstanding and then rewrite the problematic sections so that subsequent readers will not be similarly misled. Reviewers are almost always more knowledgeable about your topic, more experienced in writing manuscripts themselves, and more conscientious about reading your article than the average journal reader.
If they did not understand, neither will that average reader.
Third, when you send in your revised manuscript, tell the editor in a cover letter how you have responded to each of the criticisms or suggestions made by the reviewers. If you have decided not to adopt a particular suggestion, state your reasons, perhaps pointing out how you remedied the problem in some alternative way.
Here are some fictitious examples of cover-letter responses that also illustrate ways of responding to certain kinds of criticisms and suggestions within the revision itself.

Writing the Empirical Journal Article

20

1. Wrong: “I have left the section on the animal studies unchanged. If Reviewers A and C can’t even agree on whether the animal studies are relevant, I must be doing something right.”
Right: “You will recall that Reviewer A thought that the animal studies should be described more fully, whereas Reviewer C thought they should be omitted. A biopsychologist in my department agreed with Reviewer C that the animal studies are not really valid analogs of the human studies. So I have dropped them from the text but cited Snarkle’s review of them in an explanatory footnote on page 26.”
2. Wrong: “Reviewer A is obviously Deborah Hardin, who has never liked me or my work. If she really thinks that behavioral principles solve all the problems of obsessive-compulsive disorders, then let her write her own article. Mine is about the cognitive processes involved.”
Right: “As the critical remarks by Reviewer A indicate, this is a contentious area, with different theorists staking out strong positions. Apparently I did not make it clear that my article was intended only to cover the cognitive processes involved in obsessive-compulsive disorders and not to engage the debate between cognitive and behavioral approaches. To clarify this, I have now included the word ‘cognitive’ in both the title and abstract, taken note of the debate in my introduction, and stated explicitly that the article will not undertake a comparative review of the two approaches. I hope this is satisfactory.”
3. Right: “You will recall that two of the reviewers questioned the validity of the analysis of variance, with Reviewer B suggesting that I use multiple regression instead. I agree with their reservations regarding the ANOVA but believe that a multiple regression analysis is equally problematic because it makes the same assumptions about the underlying distributions. So,
I have retained the ANOVA, but summarized the results of a nonparametric analysis, which yields the same conclusions. If you think it preferable, I could simply substitute this nonparametric analysis for the original ANOVA, although it will be less familiar to the journal’s readers.”
Above all, remember that the editor is your ally in trying to shape a manuscript that will be a credit both to you and the journal. So, cooperate in the effort to turn your sow’s ear into a vinyl purse. Be civil and make nice. You may not live longer, but you will publish more.

References
American Psychological Association. (2001). Publication manual of the American Psychological Association (5th ed.).
Washington, DC: Author.
Bem, D. J. (1995) Writing a review article for Psychological Bulletin. Psychological Bulletin, 118, 172-177.
Bem, D. J. & Allen, A. (1974). Predicting some of the people some of the time: The search for cross-situational consistencies in behavior. Psychological Review, 81, 506-520.
Bem, S. L. (1985). Androgyny and gender schema theory: A conceptual and empirical integration. In T. B. Sonderegger
(Ed.), Nebraska symposium on motivation 1984: The psychology of gender. Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press.
Festinger, L. A. (1957). A theory of cognitive dissonance. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.
Festinger, L. & Carlsmith, J. M. (1959). Cognitive consequences of forced compliance. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 58, 203-210.
Martyna, W. (1978). What does “He” mean? Journal of Communication, 28, 131-138. de Montaigne, M. (1943). Of the inconsistency of our actions. In D. M. Frame (Trans.), Selected essays, translated and with introduction and notes by Donald M. Frame (pp. 119-126). Roslyn, NY: Walter J. Black. (Original work published
1580)
Strunk, W., Jr., & White, E. B. (2000). The elements of style (4th ed.). Boston: Allyn and Bacon.

Similar Documents

Free Essay

Life

...FROM SATAN I saw you yesterday as you began your daily chores. You awoke without kneeling to pray. As a matter of fact, you didn't even bless your meals, or pray before going to bed last night. You are so unthankful, I like that about you. I cannot tell you how glad I am that you have not changed your way of living, Fool, you are mine. Remember, you and I have been going steady for years, and I still don't love you yet. As a matter of fact, I hate you, because I hate God. He kicked me out of heaven, and I'm going to use you as long as possible to pay him back. You see, Fool, GOD LOVES YOU and HE has great plans in store for you. But you have yielded your life to me, and I'm going to make your life a living hell. That way, we'll be together twice. This will really hurt God. Thanks to you, I'm really showing Him who's boss in your life with all of the good times we've had. We have been... watching dirty movies, cursing people out, loving worldly things, having bad influences, stealing, lying, being hypocritical, fornicating, overeating, telling dirty jokes, gossiping, being judgmental, back stabbing people, disrespecting adults, and those in leadership positions, no respect for the Church, bad attitudes. SURELY you don't want to give all this up. Come on, Fool, let's burn together forever. I've got some hot plans for us. This is just a letter of appreciation from me to you. I'd like to say 'THANKS' for letting me use you for most of your...

Words: 556 - Pages: 3

Free Essay

Life

...Life in People’s Eyesight Life is what we can define. There are lots of facts we can associate and describe about it, based on different aspects and point of views of different kind of people. Well, Life is beautiful but not always easy, it has trials too and the challenges lies in dealing it with an exert effort and courage. There’s no human in this world that’s enjoying success without even conquering any hardships and failures. No wonder the saying “No Pain, No Gain”. Happiness and Victory, Sorrow and Defeat, are the two sides of life and what matter is choosing what side you prefer. There are people who see life with full of pleasures and adventures, a part of discoveries and innovations, a prospect for success and prosperity. Most of those people enjoy and live their lives to the edge. They take obstacles in life as their path through what they want. There are also people who see life with full of failures, burdens and bad omens. They think that their life is horrendous and miserable. They take it so bad, as if there’s nothing good about it. Some of them delight doing crimes as far as committing suicides, a permanent solution to a problem that’s merely temporary. There are times that other people lead them to do it, maybe because they suffered too much bullies and sarcasms that they can’t handle the pain so they decides to do unlikely things. Life shouldn’t control us, instead we should control it. We should choose the right to path, not the wrong one. But there...

Words: 423 - Pages: 2

Premium Essay

Life

...LIFE As a parent, I believed that my son would outlive me and have sons and daughters of his own, so we could grow old watching them have families of their own, because that was the way of life. I had hopes and dreams for my son. When he died, it completely destroyed my world and left so many unanswered questions. I was completely lost and bereaved. Rebuilding my life and becoming one of the living again has been a long, hard struggle. The death of my son changed my life and me as a person forever. My son chose to end his life on October 3rd 2008. He was only 29 years old. Nothing in my life prepared me for his death. My world ended and everything that I have known in life was shattered. Life as I knew it had changed forever. I couldn’t think or feel anything other than for the pain that was in my heart. My family and my friends worried for me and wanted me to come and stay with them. How could I go and stay with anyone, when I was like a zombie or the living dead. The days did not mean anything to me anymore. Everything I did was like a robot on autopilot. I ate because someone put food in front me and slept when my mind was so exhausted I had to sleep. I knew I had to try to get back into life and live again and rediscover myself as a person. I knew that was what my son would have wanted me to do. I was a completely different person when my son was alive. I loved life, laughed a lot and was happy. I greeted everyday as an adventure and with joy in my...

Words: 895 - Pages: 4

Premium Essay

Life

...to give a presentation on megaliving- how to lead a perfect life. Let’s make this an interactive session. So, hers a question I invite you to consider on. How many of you out here think that you are leading a perfect life? Or before this can any one of you describe what a perfect life is? Before answering this question, let’s do an exercise, Everyone shut your eyes, take two deep breaths and picture this scene taking place many years into the future: you are in an elegant dining hall, surrounded by those closest to you (who are dressed beautifully, looking their best). The candles shimmer on every table and the importance of the evening wafts through the air like the aromas from the kitchen. This is your testimonial dinner, an opportunity for the people who know you best to speak about you as a person, your achievements and your contributions to those you love and to society in general. Just for a moment, reflect on what you would like them to say. This reflection my friends; is the perfect life you want for yourself. And how many of us are actually living our perfect life..? There are two competing schools of thought. One says go out there live your dream, be the best, play at your highest potential. And the other says that the purpose of life is to simply enjoy the journey, simply be happy, and live in the moment. When you think about these two schools, you realize that both are equally important, life is all about the balance. It’s important to reach for the mountain...

Words: 2106 - Pages: 9

Free Essay

Life

...thinking of what is life, why we have been here on this earth and what is the purpose of life? As most of you I believe, during my whole life I have been thinking of these questions and have never been able to come across the right answer for this. A poor man is always worried about feeding himself and his family. The middle class people are always worried about arranging basic comforts of life that covers house, car, clothing etc. The upper class of the society is worried about the amount of wealth they are accumulating and the power they have in their hands. You take the case of any one of these and wonder why are we doing this. I partially support the poor class because food is something we cannot avoid in our lives. But how about the others? What are they running after? Atleast I have never been able to support the answer on this. Some may argue and I do partially agree that we do need to something to live this life. But the fact that upsets me the most is what we are doing is really that which we are supposed to do on this earth. Every religion on this earth in one form or the other guides us to do our work and perform duties and leave rest to the God. But, what kind of duties and why do those? Human thinking has been made narrow to the extent that we always think about future and desperately work, always think and devote our life to future. But what future is? It is nothing but death. Then why are we running after future. Why cannot we live in present? Life is surely very...

Words: 421 - Pages: 2

Premium Essay

Life

...I can remember as a child always asking myself the "why" questions of life. Why are we here? What is the purpose of life? Why do certain things happen? And is there really a God? I had always kept these questions to myself and eventually pushed them out of my mind altogether. I was raised in a Christian household and you just were not allowed to ask questions of that nature and doubt the faith. The world is the way it is because God made it that way and that is all there is to it. I was really excited to take this class because it would finally give me the opportunity to exercise my personal thoughts and beliefs. I have come to agree with Socrates that "the unexamined life is not worth living." In my opinion life is a combination of philosophical ideas such as morality, respect…………. The study of philosophy is a very complex and complicated task. There are so many different questions on many different topics and philosophy tries to explain them all. It tries to provide answers to the many questions that science and religion cannot explain. And from this it urges you to think about issues that may otherwise be ignored. I agree with the goal of autonomy, that philosophy is having the freedom to make your own decisions and beliefs by using your own reasoning capabilities. I believe that we all have the God-like quality of reasoning and therefore can make our own educated decisions. And because we can make our own decisions, we are fully responsible for our actions. This responsibility...

Words: 356 - Pages: 2

Free Essay

Life

...LIFE. Life may have many definitions. Through out the centuries, people have sought to understand life in different ways. People through their cultural practices, tries to explain and give meaning to life. Others through religion; their way of worship, explain what life is. Philosophers are in no doubt, a people who through the ages have embarked on this long search. However, with observation of nature, times and seasons, one can make an analogy to understand a section of life. The morning stage is the early part of the day or life. This can signify the period of conception in ones mothers worm till birth. This is also a stage where one begins a social orientation of knowing the parents and the family, learning to speak, walk and embracing the environment around him. We can also classify the time when one begins elementary school to graduating from the university and securing your first job as the morning stage of life. The age range is 1-25 years. The mid-day of life is when one has become a full adult. The person could at this stage be married and have children or his or her own nuclear family. One at this stage is independent and makes his own decisions; self actualization can also be found at this stage. Many times, the focus of people at this stage is their family and work. Acquiring houses, cars and all the luxury of life is very predominant at the mid-day of life. The age range is 35 to 60 years The evening stage of life is when people are taking rest from their labour...

Words: 437 - Pages: 2

Premium Essay

Life

...for superior and passionate patient service, clinical excellence, as the health care employer of choice, and the preferred partner of physicians. Our Values - Our values, which flow from our mission and Catholic tradition, ,ust have meaning for every one of us. Through them we put the healing minsitry of Jesus into practice throughout out organizational. The following are behaviors that are expected of all associates, physicians, volunteers, and anyone else acting on behalf of the organization. We hope these behaviors will influence for the better every person whose life we touch. Respect - we value each person as sacred, created, in the image and likeness of God, which gives worht and meaning to each person's life. Integrity - we value honesty and words and actions that build trust. Development - we value personal and professional rowth that bines the physical, emotional, spritual, and relational aspects of life and work. Excellence - we value superior performance in our work and service. Stewardship - we value our responsibility to use human, financial, and natural resources entrusted to us for the mon good, with special concern for those...

Words: 299 - Pages: 2

Premium Essay

Life

...Date: 09-12-2015 LIFE SEASON OF LIFE WHAT IS LIFE? Life can be defined as a characteristic of living. It is the state of being alive or active in lively activities. The opposite of life is death, once one has stopped to exhibit or show the characteristics of life, he or she is said to be dead. Life is also made of many activities, but we will see how to perform this activities. WHAT DO WE REQUIRE IN LIFE TO SUCCEED? For us to be able to succeed in life, we must know the times and seasons which are required of us to do everything we are to achieve in life. This leads us to “the seasons of life”. SEASONS OF LIFE: Just like the day which is made up of 24 hours is classified into morning, afternoon and night; for example, we take breakfast in the morning, lunch in the afternoon and dinner at night, we can’t say that we take breakfast at night and dinner in the morning. So also, life is classified into morning, afternoon and night, and these times have specific activities it wants us to perform. Let us consider each of them below. MORNING: The morning at every person’s life is the time frame from when he/she was born to 20 years of age. At this age, one is required to learn how to walk, read, write, show respect, learn the basics of education, respect for parents, respect for teachers and other elders in the society. One should also learn how to behave himself or herself in the public, how to speak with good manners in the public. At the latter part...

Words: 576 - Pages: 3

Premium Essay

Life

...THE ABSURDITY OF LIFE Steven Luper, Trinity University In "The Absurd"[i] Nagel claims that self-conscious human beings are necessarily absurd, so that to escape absurdity while remaining human we would have to cease being self-conscious. Fifteen years later, in The View From Nowhere,[ii] he defends the same thesis, supplementing some of his old arguments with a battery of new ones. I want to suggest that Nagel has misdiagnosed, and exaggerated the inescapability of, our absurdity. He does so partly because the grounds on which he bases his conclusion are spurious, and partly because he does not acknowledge the extent to which we can eliminate absurdity by suitably redesigning our plans and modes of justification. Nonetheless, I do not mean to imply that we can easily eliminate absurdity from our lives. Life is not necessarily absurd, but unfortunately, in a world like ours, there are limits to what we can and should do to reduce the absurd elements of our affairs. The View of the Nowhere Man "In ordinary life a situation is absurd," Nagel says, "when it includes a conspicuous discrepancy between pretension or aspiration and reality: someone gives a complicated speech in support of a motion that has already been passed. . . ; as you are being knighted, your pants fall down."[iii] In this passage from "The Absurd" Nagel claims that absurdity is a particularly striking sort of incongruity, and the conception of absurdity he discusses in his book is the same. What...

Words: 8565 - Pages: 35

Premium Essay

Student Life

...Student life is the best part of life. A student learns many things from books. But he has to enter the real world after his student life is over. So, he needs practical knowledge of things. Student life prepares a man to lead a successful life. In fact, student life is a life of learning. A student learns morality and good manners. He understand the value of discipline in life. So student life is a period of making and preparation. Student life is a care-free life. It is a life of joy. A student is free from almost all cares of the world. He gets a different atmosphere at college from that of home. He takes part in games and sports. He goes sometimes on a picnic and sometimes on educational trips. A student lives in a small world of his school or college. The life of a student, therefore, is a golden period of life. Student life is a life of discipline. At home a student may behave well or badly. But the atmosphere of a school college is completely different. Good boys are loved and praised. Wicked boys are punished. All students have to follow some rules and obey their teachers. Thus, they learn discipline. Discipline makes them self less and teachers them ‘live and let live’. Student life is a life of character building. Character has its importance in life. A student must be bold and fearless. He must think society. He should try to solve the problems of the nation. He must be loving and truthful. Student life is a life of character formation. Students must have a sense...

Words: 316 - Pages: 2

Free Essay

Journey of Life

...Running header: Journey of Life 1 Journey of Life Victoria Schoepf ENG125 October 21, 2013 Jennifer Thompson Journey of Life 2 Life starts out as an undetermined journey until the elements of choices are laid in our way. Everyone in life or spirit, will often reflect back on the path or road one has choose to contemplate, with never truly knowing if it we did choose the right or wrong direction. I will compare and contrast the literary works of “The Road Not Taken”, by Robert Frost and “I Used to Live Here Once”, by Jean Rhys. The two literary works attention is on the journey that an individual has decided to take through life. One of the narratives focuses on symbolizing choice, while the other is death, but in the same way are similar, because life itself is a journey that can lead into a life or death situation. “A symbol is an object, person, or action that conveys two meaning: its own literal meaning and something it stands for as well,” (Clugston, 2010). “The Road Not Taken” uses symbolism by starting with “Two roads diverged in a yellow wood,” (Frost, 1916). Two roads diverged is symbolizing the looking back, (a reflection point one has on life altering choices) on life’s lessons, (events) that have impacted one’s journey...

Words: 2272 - Pages: 10

Free Essay

Extraterrestrial Life

...Astronomy Research and the Search for Extraterrestrial Life The objective of this paper is to discuss life here on Earth and the possibilities of extraterrestrial life in one’s solar system. The team will describe the properties of life on Earth and explain the theories for the genesis of life, including the theory of natural selection. Members will provide a brief description of the evolution of life and include a geological timescale that describes specific eras throughout the previous three billion plus years. The team will assess the possibilities of extraterrestrial life, and results will be presented. Describe the properties of life on Earth Properties of life on Earth are what define the word life. The properties of life consist of cells and reproduction, responses to the environment, growth and development, and evolution. Every living thing is collected through cells which make tissues and organs that make a living organism. All living organisms can produce such as humans, animals, bacteria, and plants called asexual reproduction. The responses of the living in the environment are through any changes occurring in “light, sound, heat, and chemical contact” through “eyes, ears, and taste buds” (Cliff’s notes, 2011, para. 3). Behavior is one way that all living organisms changes through the environment such as the food chain for one’s survival. Growth and development of an organism takes in all substantial amount of information through energy of building...

Words: 1783 - Pages: 8

Free Essay

Life History

...1/18/2014 SimUText :: Printable Chapter :: Life History PRINT ER-FRIENDLY PAGE: T his page contains the com plete tex t of this Sim UT ex t chapter. Y ou can use y our browser's print function to print a copy . Life History This chapter explores life cycles, life histories and life tables, and explores the trade-offs that different species make in their reproductive strategy. file:///C:/Users/Hossein/SimUText/labs/LifeHistory_20700/instructions/print_chapter.html 1/156 1/18/2014 SimUText :: Printable Chapter :: Life History Contents Se ction 1 : Life Cycle s a nd Life Historie s Chapter Credits This Sim UText chapter was dev eloped by a team including: Lead Author: Simon Bird Authors: W. John Roach, Ellie Steinberg, Eli Meir Reviewer: Susan Maruca Graphics: Brad Beesley, Jennifer Wallner Simulations: Susan Maruca Programming: Derek Stal, Steve Allison-Bunnell, Jen Jacaruso Outside Reviewer: James Danoff-Burg (Columbia University) Thanks to all the students and instructors who helped test prototy pes of this chapter. For m ore inform ation, please v isit www.sim bio.com . Suggested citation: Sim on Bird, Susan Maruca, W. John Roach, Ellie Steinberg, Eli Meir. 2 009 . Life History . In Sim UText Ecology . Sim bio.com . Sim UText is a registered tradem ark of Sim Biotic Software for Teaching and Research, Inc. © 2 009 -2 01 2 Sim Bio. All Rights Reserv ed. This and other Sim bio Interactiv e Chapters® are accessible through the Sim UText Sy stem ®. ...

Words: 16377 - Pages: 66

Premium Essay

Life Forms

...Life forms adapted to the environmental changes that have taken place over the years to evolve to diverse life forms that are present in planet earth today. The various processes that ensure adaptation of the life forms include evolutions and mutations. There are various environmental factors that have contributed to the evolution of the organisms. All the life forms depend on the environment to obtain the nutrients required for survival. Therefore all life forms have to match up with the environmental changes and this is referred to as adaptation. There are various environmental changes which have occurred over the years such as changes in climate and seasons, competition, occurrence of infectious diseases and predation. In order to survive with these changes in the environment, the life forms have to change over time in a process referred to as evolution. Since nature is more of cooperative rather than competitive, all the life forms tend to evolve together in a process known as co-evolution in ordre to fit in and not become extinct. The various life forms cooperate in that some provide nutrients and shelter to others and competition only comes in when the organisms obtain their nutrients from the same source. Mutation is also another factor that has led to the adaptation of the life forms to the environmental changes over time. The process of mutation takes place in reproduction whereby the genetic makeup of the parent is passed on to the offspring. This process is inevitable...

Words: 384 - Pages: 2