Free Essay

None

In:

Submitted By kaydy
Words 5225
Pages 21
Evaluating Commercial Spoken Language Translation Software
Harold SOMERS and Yuri SUGITA1 Centre for Computational Linguistics UMIST, PO Box 88 Manchester M60 1QD, England Harold.Somers@umist.ac.uk, sugita_yuri@yahoo.co.jp
Abstract1 While spoken language translation remains a research goal, a crude form of it is widely available commercially for Japanese–English as a pipeline concatenation of speech-to-text recognition (SR), text-to-text translation (MT) and text-to-speech synthesis (SS). This paper proposes and illustrates an evaluation methodology for this noisy channel which tries to quantify the relative amount of degradation in translation quality due to each of the contributing modules. A small pilot experiment involving word-accuracy rate for the SR, and a fidelity evaluation for the MT and SS modules is proposed in which subjects are asked to paraphrase translated and/or synthesised sentences from a tourist’s phrasebook. Results show (as expected) that MT is the “noisiest” channel, with SS contributing least noise. The concatenation of the three channels is worse than could be predicted from the performance of each as individual tasks.

1. Introduction Evaluation is without doubt a major aspect of language engineering, including Machine Translation (MT). Although it is still true that no consensus exists regarding the best way to evaluate software, there is general agreement about some of the factors that must be taken into account when deciding what form an evaluation should take. MT evaluation has been much studied in recent years, so much so that it has been light-heartedly claimed that MT evaluation “is a better founded subject than machine translation” (Wilks, 1994:1). If this is no longer strictly true, it is because MT is arguably in pretty good shape, at least text-to-text MT of restricted texts or for restricted purposes. This paper however concerns a much less mature application, namely spoken language translation (SLT).
1

Until recently thought to be simply too difficult a task (cf. Krauwer, 2000:1), SLT has now established itself as a growing area for research and development. The apparently attractive option of hooking up a text MT system to speech recognition (SR) at one end and speech synthesis (SS) at the other – so-called linear pipeline architecture (cf. Seligman, 2000:156) – is rejected in all experimental SLT systems in recognition of the fact that spoken language is fundamentally different from written language. In the commercial world, however, things are different. In Japan especially, where text MT systems are widely used and have become a familiar application included as a free add-on with most computers, and where both speech recognition and speech synthesis have reached high levels of quality, the SR–MT–SS chain has proven irresistible. The present paper proposes a methodology for evaluating SLT of this type: considering that concatenating three potentially error-prone processes is bound to provide a triple noisy channel, the aim is to establish how noisy each channel is, or, to put it another way, the relative negative impact of each of the processes on the overall SLT task. The methodology is illustrated via a small-scale evaluation of a commercial Japanese–English SLT system. 2. Background As stated in the Introduction, many Japanese computers now come with SR, MT and SS already installed, or else cheaply available; in many cases, users are encouraged to attempt SLT in that the MT system offers the possibility of both input and output in either spoken or written form. Language pairs are almost inevitably Japanese and English, and one or other of the speech elements may be restricted to Japanese, but it is not unusual to find speech input and output as options in the MT window.

Currently in Tsushi, Mieken, Japan.

One such system is Sourcenext’s Honkaku Hon’yaku, currently one of the best-selling Japanese MT systems,2 at the very modest price of ¥9800 (less than $100). The MT system itself was developed by NEC, and is described in the accompanying literature as a combination of rulebased and example-based methods. Its dictionary contains 526,000 words for basic use, with the option to purchase additional technical dictionaries available in 31 domains, increasing the vocabulary by 1.2m words. The Japanese SR function is SmartVoice, also developed by NEC. This system performs best when it has been trained to recognize an individual user, which is achieved with a 150sentence training set. The English SS engine is the TTS system developed by Lernout & Hauspie. The three functions can be combined to give four translation modes, namely 1. SR–MT–SS speech-to-speech 2. SR–MT speech-to-text 3. MT–SS text-to-speech 4. MT text-to-text In the following sections, we shall briefly discuss the three functions, and some issues in separately evaluating them. 2.1 Speech Recognition In SR an important distinction is made between speaker-independent and speaker-dependent systems, with the latter generally performing considerably better. Training permits the system to accustom itself to idiosyncrasies of both voice quality and allophonic realisation. However, even if individual phonemes are recognized with a high level of accuracy, there is still be the problem of homophone disambiguation. For Japanese, this problem is particularly acute. The simple phonological system (five vowels, 15 consonants, mostly open syllables with almost no consonant clusters) makes phoneme recognition fairly robust, but the high incidence of homophones makes the word-selection task extremely difficult. The same problem arises in text input (in word-processing for example), and Japanese speakers are accustomed to having to select the correct written form from a pop-up menu often showing 10 or more alternatives (see Fig. 1). SmartVoice is typical in requiring the user to confirm the word
2

Figure 1. Pop-up menu offering the user a choice amongst competing orthographic representations of the given input, in this case kyōkai.

selection at the time of input. As we shall see, this feature has an important effect on our evaluation methodology. Where the output from SR is text, there are well-established evaluation measures, notably word-error rate (WER) and sentence-error rate (SER), in either case given as an average percentage over a set of samples. As its name suggests, WER is the number of incorrect, omitted or inserted words divided by the number of words in the target phrase. The SER is a simpler percentage reflecting right/wrong decisions and tends to be pessimistic (Tillmann et al., 2000:55), since a single error in a long sentence will cause the whole sentence to be rejected. Since for most SR tasks, overall comprehension can usually survive a few small errors, WER is to be preferred, sometimes with refinements to take account of the importance or otherwise of the misrecognised word(s). One problem for the WER in our case however is that written Japanese does not indicate word boundaries. Instead, we split the text stream into morphemes, using the Japanese morphological analyser ChaSen (Matsumoto et al., 2000) available free online. Morpheme error rate (MER) is slightly more punitive than WER, but this is more than compensated by the fact that the SR task is massively aided by the human user choosing amongst alternatives. In particular, we wish to

www.computernews.com/award/2003_s.htm (in Japanese).

assess the degree to which SR errors degrade the overall translation result. 2.2 Machine Translation Many distinctions are made in discussions of MT evaluation. Our evaluation is a “declarative” evaluation (Arnold et al., 1993:7) aimed at endusers in that we aim to evaluate the output rather than the process. Such MT evaluations focus on grammaticality and style, intelligibility, fidelity and so on. In choosing the form of our MT evaluation, we took into account the most likely use for an SLT system. Almost without exception, cutting-edge SLT research systems focus on taskoriented cooperative dialogues, usually between monolingual participants. Accordingly, our MT evaluation aims at testing the fidelity in translation of short utterances taken from a tourist’s phrasebook. This is of course similar to the “Traveller Task” defined in the EUTRANS project (Amengual et al., 1997). “Fidelity” is measured in terms of a subject’s ability to infer correctly the intended meaning of the utterance. We are not interested in the grammar or style of the output. As above, we want to know to what extent the MT system is responsible for the overall quality of the SLT. Taking phrases from a tourist’s phrase book helps us avoid some of the well-documented difficulties in translating real-time dialogues, such as hesitations, false starts, repetitions, fragmental phrases, complex topicalization, metonymical phrases, inconsistent expressions, and so on. Other features, such as ellipses, anaphora, idiomatic expressions for etiquette, may still be present to some extent, as will problems common to both SLT and text MT – lexical and syntactic ambiguity above all else. 2.3 Speech synthesis Although SS is considerably more advanced than either SR or MT, there are still pitfalls, especially in text-to-speech synthesis. For many languages the mapping of orthography to speech is relatively straightforward, inasmuch as a word’s pronunciation can be given more or less unequivocally in a dictionary. Exceptions to this general rule are heteronyms such as tear, read, and so on. However, generally this process does not present a major obstacle, in particular for a

language like English which has been studied substantially. Other aspects less well studied include prosodic features such as pitch, loudness and duration, all of which can affect the perceived meaning, as well as contributing to the naturalness of the synthetic speech. In some respects, however, SS is considered “a solved problem” (Kay et al., 1994:140). In fact, for a wide range of applications, for a number of languages imperfect but nonetheless acceptable synthesis is currently available (idem.). Like MT, SS can be evaluated for its accuracy, intelligibility or style (cf. van Heuven and van Bezooijen, 1995). As with our MT evaluation, our aim is to assess the extent to which the synthesised speech permits the hearer to infer correctly the intended meaning of the utterance. Once again, our aim is to quantify the contribution of the SR component to the SLT. 3. Method Our goal is to take a complex process consisting of three elements and to compare the contribution of each of the three to the overall process. It is therefore logical to try to evaluate each of the processes in isolation, as well as all (logical) combinations of them. We therefore conducted six different but closely related evaluations, as follows: 1. speech recognition (SR) 2. speech synthesis (SS) 3. text-to-text translation (MT) 4. speech-to-text translation (SR+MT) 5. text-to-speech translation (MT+SS) 6. speech-to-speech translation (SR+MT+SS) Figure 2 shows more clearly the relationship between these six “modes”, in particular the idea that mode 6 is in some sense a combination of 1+2+3, or 4+2, or 5+1. Another way to look at it is

SR
1 6 4 3 5

SS
2

MT
Figure 2. Three modes – six tests

that mode 4 assumes “perfect” SS and so on. It will be of interest to see to what extent the results bear out this notion of the whole as the sum of its parts.3 3.1 The translation task As mentioned above, all the evaluations have in common the specific SLT task based on the scenario of a tourist using the SLT system as a speaking phrase-book. We selected 30 phrases from the “tourist” side of a Japanese–English phrase-book English: Wagamama Aruki Travel Conversation Series 1 (i.e. phrases the tourist would use, not replies from the informant). The 30 phrases were grouped in six sets of five, each related to a specific scene, e.g. reporting a theft, getting directions, at the bank. One can also group the questions on syntactic criteria: nine were whquestions, nine yes–no questions, seven were requests and the remaining five declarative statements. In the fidelity evaluations, judges are asked to paraphrase what they understand to be the speaker’s intention (more about this below). The choice of phrases was not entirely random. Care was taken to choose different types of phrases (e.g. How do I get to the theatre? and Where is the police station? are functionally almost identical), and to avoid culturally loaded phrases which our subjects might not understand (e.g. Is there a major league game tonight? Can I take the subway?). However, no consideration was made of the likely ease or difficulty for SR, translation or SS each phrase might involve. The selection is of course crucial, especially for the phase of judging the results, which we discuss below. 3.2 Evaluating SR The evaluation of SR on its own (Test 1) differs from the other evaluations in several respects. The most important one is that it is not necessary to make a subjective judgment, since the output of the speech-to-text conversion can easily be compared with the “gold standard” of the target text. As mentioned above, because written Japanese does not indicate word boundaries, we adapted the standard WER measure to count morpheme errors. We used the Japanese morphological analyser ChaSen to segment the phrases, and checked the resulting segmentation for errors along the method
3

adopted in Verbmobil (cf. Waibel, et al., 2000). We also applied the more punitive SER evaluation. The SR system used in this evaluation depends on prior training (“Speaker registration”) of the system which the experimenter (co-author YS) had previously done. In the case of SmartVoice, this is achieved by reading out 150 test sentences, a relatively quick procedure. It is well known that voice quality is subject to change due to fatigue, excitement, health and other factors. We decided to perform multiple evaluations of the SR under differing conditions, and running through the 30 phrases in a randomised order. Nine tests in all were made (three each at three different times of day). The results are presented below. The multiple tests served a second purpose: in the normal use of the SR system either for dictation purposes, or together with the MT system, the user is invited to confirm the accuracy of the text output, to edit it, or to repeat the input. This feature is obviously significant in our evaluation of an SLT chain involving SR, since in real use a misrecognised input will be corrected before it is passed on to the next stage in the chain. For our experiments involving SR+MT+… we always used the “best” SR result obtained for any of the inputs: thus if any of the nine trials had resulted in error-free recognition, that result was subsequently used. Otherwise, the result with the lowest MER was used. In any case, for the sake of consistency and convenience, in the experiments involving speech input, the SR element was simulated, and a pre-stored text fed into the MT stage. 3.3 Evaluating SS In the evaluations involving SS (Tests 2, 5 and 6), the subjects were told that they were going to hear synthetic speech, and the tourist scenario was explained. The subjects were in a relatively quiet room, and the sound was played through extension speakers. The test was run at the subject’s pace: each new item was presented only when the subject said they were ready. However, requests to hear an item again were refused. Subjects were provided with a test script which included general instructions followed by a header for each of the six scenarios (e.g. You are working behind the counter in a bank) with space for each

Actually, as we will see, product rather than a sum.

it

is

a

of the five utterances in each section. They were instructed to write in the space an indication of what they thought the tourist was asking. They were explicitly asked not to write down exactly what they heard, but to paraphrase it, and above all to write down something that made sense, even if they had not heard easily. One way they could do this was by using reported speech (e.g. She’s asking about the exchange rate). In Test 2, the target English phrases were put through the SS system. In Tests 5 and 6, the English text resulting from translation by the MT system was synthesized. 3.4 Tests 3 and 4: Evaluating text output In Tests 3 and 4, text output from the MT system was evaluated. Since the subjects were not required to listen to anything, the evaluations could be done by the whole group in one sitting, with minimal supervision. The test scripts were similar to those for the test involving SS, but instead of being asked to listen and write down a paraphrase, the translated texts were presented on the page, and subjects were asked to write underneath what they thought was meant. 3.5 Judging the answers Apart from Test 1, where the evaluation is mechanical, the tests require a judgment of the match between the subjects’ responses and the expected answers. All the scripts were independently marked by two judges: the experimenter (YS) and the co-author (HS). The answers were rated on a seven-point scale as follows:
Useful A (6) Clearly useful to communicate the intention of the utterance: the response matches what is intended in the original utterance. It contains the same concepts and all the necessary arguments. A– (5) Generally useful: the response nearly matches what is intended in the original utterance; may misrepresent or omit some detail that is not fatal. Borderline B (4) Useful but less informative compared with the above: basic match with what is intended, but some accompanying arguments are incomplete or inadequate. B– (3) Useful but not wholly adequate: as B but some arguments are missing. Useless C (2) Almost useless but still informative and useful: the response doesn’t match what is intended but nevertheless contains some partially useful information. C– (1) Clearly useless: the response doesn’t match what is

intended in the original utterance at all. No response D (0) Blank or garbage.

The judges worked through the 30 test items to agree beforehand which elements were essential or additional information in each. This process was aided by the fact that a small pilot of the experimental design had been run with four subjects, which identified some potential pitfalls.4 For example, in one item, I’d like an automatic sports car, it was agreed that both automatic and sports must be mentioned for an A. After the initial judging, the results were compared, and cases of discrepancy reconciled by discussion. In this way, we tried to make our marking procedure more like an assessment of “precision” in an information retrieval task (cf. Carter et al.’s (2000) evaluation of their SLT system, also discussed below). In fact, scores were never out by more than one point, and these cases were concentrated on six of the test items, affecting less than one fifth of the results. Probably, the judges’ raw scores could have been left intact, with little difference to the overall results. 4. Results It is appreciated that the small number of subjects for each evaluation diminishes the value of the results. We are more interested in presenting the methodology here, though the results such as they are, reveal some interesting issues. The scores for all the tests are summarized in Table 1. For Tests 2 to 6, there were 30 phrases, and five subjects, giving a total of 150 test items. The “rate” shown in the bottom row is the “success rate” of the process, calculated by awarding points for each response as shown above (6 for A, 5 for A–, and so on). A maximum score is thus 900 (=150×6). Scores for Test 1 are calculated differently, but included here for completeness. Let us look at the results in more detail.

Some aspects of the experimental design were changed after the pilot, which also helped to identify one or two misleading phrases, e.g. the American term bill used in translation where note is preferred in British English.

4

4.1 Test 1: SR
Test 1 SR A A– B B– C C– D Rate (%) Test 2 SS No. % 138 92.0 2 1.3 3 2.0 2 1.3 3 2.0 1 0.7 1 0.7 95.6 Test 3 MT No. % 93 62.0 13 8.7 13 8.7 5 3.3 9 6.0 13 8.7 4 2.7 80.1

When we combine the processes, the performance deteriorates further, more or less
Test 4 SR+MT No. % 94 62.7 7 4.7 14 9.3 9 6.0 5 3.3 8 5.3 13 8.7 77.8 Test 5 MT+SS No. % 91 60.7 14 9.3 14 9.3 5 3.3 9 6.0 15 10.0 2 1.3 79.4 Test 6 SR+MT+SS No. % 76 50.7 13 8.7 14 9.3 3 2.0 12 8.0 30 20.0 2 1.3 71.1

97.0

Table 1. Summary of scores in all tests

SR was evaluated over nine occasions, with randomised presentation order of the 30 phrases. Using the MER measure, expressed as a measure of accuracy (AR = 100 – ER) the results were remarkably consistent, ranging from 94.8% to 91.4% with an average of 93.2%, indicating that SR is not sensitive to time of day or fatigue. The SAR as expected shows lower scores over a broader range: 53.3% to 70.0%, average 61.9%. The total number of morphemes in the 30 phrases was 233. The MAR indicates on average a single error every other sentence. Where the results of SR are used as input to MT, we took the best-scoring result for each phrase. The MAR and SAR for this optimised set of phrases are a comfortable 97% and 80% respectively. 4.2 Tests 2 and 3: SS and MT For Test 2, the seven-point rating scale described above was used. 138 (92%) of the items were rated A, the remaining 12 (8%) spread evenly over the other categories (including one D). The scores for Test 3 show that just over 70% of the output of the text translation system is “useful”, itself an encouraging result. The bottom row of Table 1 indicates that the three processes when considered in isolation are ranked SR>SS>MT. The fact that SR outperforms SS might be surprising, until one considers that 97.0% is an optimised score (the average was 93.2%, slightly worse than SS), justified by the fact that SR is aided by the user in this set-up.

proportionately. 4.3 Pipelining the processes Tests 4 to 6 show the results of pipelining or concatenating the processes. As we might have expected, the scores are lower, confirming the prediction that when we take the output of a noisy channel as input to another noisy channel, the result is worse than the lesser of the two channels. The results of this set of experiments allow us to attempt to quantify the multiplication effects of this chaining process. We can illustrate this by looking at Test 4 (SR+MT) compared to Tests 1 (SR) and 3 (MT). MT on its own is rated at 80.1% which goes down to 77.8% when it is combined with SR. One can say that SR “degrades” the MT by a factor of 0.971; that is to say, SR+MT is 0.971 of the quality of MT on its own. Curiously, this is almost exactly the same as the reliability score achieved by SR in Test 1. A similar calculation can be made for SS. Comparing Tests 3 and 5, we can say that SS degrades MT by 0.991, which compares favourably to the Test 2 score for SS of 95.6%. Finally, we can say that the combined degradation factor of SR and SS on MT is 0.888. These derived scores are summarized in Table 2. This table confirms the more intuitive result that SS, which is on the whole very robust, hardly degrades MT output at all (by 0.009 in fact), even though the individual success rate for this module is slightly worse than that for SR. The same data are shown graphically in Figure 3 which indicates

that the success rates and the degradation factors are proportional. SR SS SR+SS Rate 97.0 95.6 Degradation 0.971 0.991 0.888

Table 2. Success rates and degradation factors
1

0.9

0.8

0.7

0.6
SR T+ M l al T M SS T+ M

Figure 3. Success rates (the darker shorter bars) and degradation factors shown graphically.

5. Previous Studies and Discussion For SLT much more than text translation it is the content rather than the form of the message that is important: an ungrammatical but (correctly) understood translation is perfectly acceptable in the communicative scenario for which this type of SLT system is envisaged. As Carter et al. (2000) state:
“Our goal … is to measure objectively the ability of subjects to understand the content of speech output. This must be the key criterion …: if apparent deficiencies in syntax or word choice fail to affect subject’s ability to understand content, then it is hard to say that they represent real loss of quality.” (p. 300)

Their system handles dialogues in the ATIS domain, so it is reasonable for them to evaluate its precision and recall on information retrieval tasks. Jain et al. (1993) performed a glass-box evaluation of an early version of the JANUS system. Gates et al. (1997) evaluated a later version in a fairly traditional subjective manner using bilingual judges. Levin et al. (2000) report an evaluation of the same system similar to ours in that they attempt to evaluate separately the effect of the SR and MT functions. Interestingly, Jain et al. found “errors in

speech recognition [to be] the primary cause of incorrect translation” (op. cit.:159). It is open to speculation whether our contrary finding is due to better SR or inferior MT. The purpose of this paper is to report our methodology for evaluating this form of SLT system much more than the results we happened to get with our small number of subjects. With only five subjects in each mode, it is obvious that one wayward score could completely derail our results. Let us concentrate therefore in this final section on the evaluation methodology itself. We were relatively happy with the choice of task for our evaluation. The “tourist abroad” scenario seems quite a natural use for this software (cf. Ward, 2002), and subjects for the most part quickly understand their part in the role play. It was felt to be important in both the SS tests, and even more so in the text-output tests, to require the subjects to indicate what they understood in their own words: “communicative intent” is the key notion here, which is why we label our evaluation as one of “fidelity” rather than, say, intelligibility. A bad translation could be clearly synthesised and transcribed verbatim by the subject, but this would give no indication that the communicative intent had been translated. We can be self-critical about some of the finer points of our experimental design. As we discovered, choice of test items could be quite crucial. Although we filtered out culturally sensitive items, some of the phrases were difficult to understand out of context, even under the most favourable conditions (hence, perhaps, the ten responses scoring B, C or D in Test 2). Interestingly, it could be argued that the gold standard (100% understanding) is an unreasonable target: even two humans face to face might be expected to misunderstand one another from time to time. In this case one strategy is to ask your dialogue partner to repeat themselves, an option that we denied to subjects participating in Tests 2, 5 and 6 (all involving SS output), where a scraping chair or lack of concentration could lead the subject to underperform – another reason to have a much larger subject population. On the whole however, we would feel confident that our methodology is suitable for replication with a much larger population, for example in a

comparative evaluation of several systems, and look forward to an opportunity to do so.

References
Amengual, J.C., J.M. Benedí, F. Casacuberta, A. Castaño, A. Castellanos, D. Llorens, A. Marzal, F. Prat, E. Vidal and J.M. Vilar (1997) “Using Categories in the EUTRANS System”, Spoken Language Translation: Proceedings of a Workshop Sponsored by the Association for Computational Linguistics and the by the European Network in Language and Speech (ELSNET), Madrid, pp. 44– 53. Arnold, Doug, Louisa Sadler and R. Lee Humphreys (1993) “Evaluation: An Assessment”, Machine Translation 8, 1–24. Carter, David, Manny Rayner, Robert Eklund, Catriona MacDermid and Mats Wirén (2000) “Evaluation”, in Manny Rayner, David Carter, Pierrette Bouillon, Vassilis Digalakis and Mats Wirén (eds) The Spoken Language Translator, Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, pp. 297–312. Gates, Donna, Alon Lavie, Lori Levin, Marsal Gavaldà, Monika Woszczyna and Puminhg Zhan (1997) “End-to-End Evaluation in JANUS: A Speech-toSpeech Translation System”, in E. Maier, M. Mast and S. Luperfoy (eds) Dialogue Processing in Spoken Language Systems, Berlin: Springer, pp. 195–206. Jain, A.N., A.E. McNair, A. Waibel, H. Saito, A.G. Hauptmann and J. Tebelskis (1993) “Connectionist and Symbolic Processing in Speech-to-Speech Translation: The JANUS System”, in Sergei Nirenburg (ed.) Progress in Machine Translation, Amsterdam: IOS Press and Tokyo: Ohmsha, pp.153–160. Kay, Martin, Jean Mark Gawron and Peter Norvig (1994) Verbmobil: A Translation System for Faceto-Face Dialog, Stanford, CA: CSLI. Krauwer, Steven (2000) “Introduction: Special Issue on Spoken Language Translation”, Machine Translation 15, 1–2. Levin, Lori, Alon Lavie, Monika Woszczyna, Donna Gates, Marsal Gavaldà, Detlef Koll and Alex Waibel (2000) “The JANUS-III Translation System: Speechto-Speech Translation in Multiple Domains”, Machine Translation 15, 3–25.

Matsumoto, Yuji, Akira Kitauchi, Tatsuo Yamashita, Yoshitaka Hirano, Hiroshi Matsuda, Kazuma Takaoka and Masayuki Asahara (2000) Morphological Analysis System ChaSen version 2.2.1, Technical Report, NAIST, Nara, Japan; available at http://chasen.aist-nara.ac.jp/. Seligman, Mark (2000) “Nine Issues in Speech Translation”, Machine Translation 15, 149–185. Tillmann, Christoph, Stephan Vogel, Hermann Ney and Hassan Sawaf (2000) “Statistical Translation of Text and Speech: First Results with the RWTH System”, Machine Translation 15, 43–74. van Heuven, Vincent and Renie van Bezooijen (1995) “Quality Evaluation of Synthesized Speech”, in K.K. Paliwal (ed.) Speech Coding and Synthesis, Amsterdam: Elsevier Science, pp. 707–738. Waibel, Alex, Hagen Soltau, Tanja Schultz, Thomas Schaaf and Florian Metze (2000) “Multilingual Speech recognition”, in Wolfgang Wahlster (ed.) Verbmobil: Foundations of Speech-to-Speech Translation, Berlin: Springer, pp. 33–45. Ward, Nigel (2002) “Machine Translation in the Mobile and Wearable Age”, Proceedings of the MT Roadmap Workshop at TMI-2002, Keihanna, Japan, pages not numbered. Wilks, Yorick (1994) “Keynote: Traditions in the Evaluation of MT”, in Muriel Vasconcellos, (ed.) MT Evaluation: Basis for Future Directions. Proceedings of a workshop sponsored by the National Science Foundation, San Diego, California, pp. 1–3.

Similar Documents

Premium Essay

Foreshadowing In Agatha Christie's And Then There Were None

..."It is perfectly clear. Mr. Owen is one of us….” (Christie 150). These famous words from Agatha Christie’s novel, And Then There Were None, would define the murder mystery genre for generations to come. Her use of many literary devices, such as foreshadowing, symbolism, and similes, allowed her to truly express herself. Agatha Christie utilized the gramophone as foreshadowing for the identity of the killer, the 10 solider boys as a motif for the characters’ deaths, and the seaweed as a symbol of guilt and regret all to add suspense to her novel, And Then There Were None. The gramophone in Agatha Christie’s novel, And Then There Were None, foreshadowed who among them was Mr. Owen, thus adding suspense to the story. Near the beginning of the book, all of the characters gather to eat dinner, when suddenly a...

Words: 850 - Pages: 4

Premium Essay

And Then There Were Nonr Summary

...And Then There Were None Agatha Christie ← Plot Overview → Eight people, all strangers to each other, are invited to Indian Island, off the English coast. Vera Claythorne, a former governess, thinks she has been hired as a secretary; Philip Lombard, an adventurer, and William Blore, an ex-detective, think they have been hired to look out for trouble over the weekend; Dr. Armstrong thinks he has been hired to look after the wife of the island’s owner. Emily Brent, General Macarthur, Tony Marston, and Judge Wargrave think they are going to visit old friends. When they arrive on the island, the guests are greeted by Mr. and Mrs. Rogers, the butler and housekeeper, who report that the host, someone they call Mr. Owen, will not arrive until the next day. That evening, as all the guests gather in the drawing room after an excellent dinner, they hear a recorded voice accusing each of them of a specific murder committed in the past and never uncovered. They compare notes and realize that none of them, including the servants, knows “Mr. Owen,” which suggests that they were brought here according to someone’s strange plan. As they discuss what to do, Tony Marston chokes on poisoned whiskey and dies. Frightened, the party retreats to bed, where almost everyone is plagued by guilt and memories of their crimes. Vera Claythorne notices the similarity between the death of Marston and the first verse of a nursery rhyme, “Ten Little Indians,” that hangs in each bedroom. The next morning...

Words: 4161 - Pages: 17

Premium Essay

Novel

...And Then There Were None by AGATHA CHRISTIE CHAPTER 1 IN THE CORNER of a first-class smoking carriage, Mr. Justice Wargrave, lately retired from the bench, puffed at a cigar and ran an interested eye through the political news in the Times. He laid the paper down and glanced out of the window. They were running now through Somerset. He glanced at his watch-another two hours to go. He went over in his mind all that had appeared in the papers about Indian Island. There had been its original purchase by an American millionaire who was crazy about yachting-and an account of the luxurious modern house he had built on this little island off the Devon coast. The unfortunate fact that the new third wife of the American millionaire was a bad sailor had led to the subsequent putting up of the house and island for sale. Various glowing advertisements of it had appeared in the papers. Then came the first bald statement that it had been bought-by a Mr. Owen. After that the rurnours of the gossip writers had started. Indian Island had really been bought by Miss Gabrielle Turl, the Hollywood film star! She wanted to spend some months there free from all publicity! Busy Bee had hinted delicately that it was to be an abode for Royalty??! Mr. Merryweather had had it whispered to him that it had been bought for a honeymoon-Young Lord L-- had surrendered to Cupid at last! Jonas knew for a fact that it had been purchased by the Admiralty with a view to carrying out some very hush hush experiments...

Words: 53558 - Pages: 215

Free Essay

None

...None I don’t want to give you rights to any work but this. None I don’t want to give you rights to any work but this. None I don’t want to give you rights to any work but this. None I don’t want to give you rights to any work but this. None I don’t want to give you rights to any work but this. None I don’t want to give you rights to any work but this. None I don’t want to give you rights to any work but this. None I don’t want to give you rights to any work but this. None I don’t want to give you rights to any work but this. None I don’t want to give you rights to any work but this. None I don’t want to give you rights to any work but this. None I don’t want to give you rights to any work but this. None I don’t want to give you rights to any work but this. None I don’t want to give you rights to any work but this. None I don’t want to give you rights to any work but this. None I don’t want to give you rights to any work but this. None I don’t want to give you rights to any work but this. None I don’t want to give you rights to any work but this. None I don’t want to give you rights to any work but this. None I don’t want to give you rights to any work but this. None I don’t want to give you rights to any work but this. None I don’t want to give you rights to any work but this. None I don’t want to give you rights to any work but this. None I don’t want to give you rights to any work but this. None I don’t want to give you rights to...

Words: 1132 - Pages: 5

Premium Essay

None

...Memory Systems Exam PSYCH 640 October 6, 2014 Gaston Weisz   Student Name: Class: Cognitive Psychology 640 [Memory Systems Test] Achieved Score: Possible High Score: 100 MULTIPLE CHOICE QUESTIONS 1. What type of memory stores information for about 30 seconds? A. Working Memory B. Long Term Memory C. Short Term Memory D. None of the Above E. All of the Above 2. What is the estimated amount of neurons in the human brain? F. 1 Trillion G. 450 Billion H. 100 billion I. 895 million J. 1,000 trillion 3. What is the correct explanation for encoding memory? A. Encoding in psychology is taking information into the mind and coding it with brain code and storing the information for later retrieval B. Encoding memory is when memory is recalled to working memory for use and access, then returned to long term memory when the information is no longer required C. Encoding in psychology is the transformation, as well as the transfer of information into a memory system that requires selective attention which is the focusing of awareness on a particular set of stimuli or events. D. Encoding memory is when your brain applies “1’s and 0’s” to information that is collected and placed in long term memory or discarded depending on if the memory is rehearsed or discarded • True or False questions: True False 1. Can a false memory seem real and be perceived as a genuine memory? True False 2. Is long term memory controlled by the hippocampus portion...

Words: 316 - Pages: 2

Free Essay

None

...KEEP THIS BOOKLET FOR YOUR RECORDS Assistance Application Information Booklet Welcome to the State of Michigan Department of Human Services (DHS) We have programs to help you and/or your household (everyone living in your home) with food, medical care, child care, cash and emergencies. We can also tell you about other programs and resources that may help meet your needs. We look forward to helping you and your household. If you need help with reading, writing, hearing, etc., please tell us. If you need an interpreter, we will provide one or you may bring your own. Steps to Assistance - Apply online for assistance programs at www.michigan.gov/mibridges. You may bring, mail or fax your assistance application to the DHS office in your area. You can find the address and phone number to the office in your area in your phone book under the state government section, or online at www.michigan.gov/dhs-countyoffices. 2 - Read this booklet and keep it. It tells you about our programs and has important information. When you sign the assistance application, you agree to the rules in this booklet. 3 - Answer the questions on the assistance application. We need your answers to decide what help you may receive. You can apply for all or some of our programs. 4 - For some programs we may need to ask for more information (proof). We will let you know what we need. 5 - We will send you a letter in the mail telling you if you are approved or denied. Keep this letter. It has important information...

Words: 19332 - Pages: 78

Free Essay

None

...None of these things can ever work anymore, ever again. None of these things can ever work anymore, ever again. None of these things can ever work anymore, ever again. None of these things can ever work anymore, ever again. None of these things can ever work anymore, ever again. None of these things can ever work anymore, ever again. None of these things can ever work anymore, ever again. None of these things can ever work anymore, ever again. None of these things can ever work anymore, ever again. None of these things can ever work anymore, ever again. None of these things can ever work anymore, ever again. None of these things can ever work anymore, ever again. None of these things can ever work anymore, ever again. None of these things can ever work anymore, ever again. None of these things can ever work anymore, ever again. None of these things can ever work anymore, ever again. None of these things can ever work anymore, ever again. None of these things can ever work anymore, ever again. None of these things can ever work anymore, ever again. None of these things can ever work anymore, ever again. None of these things can ever work anymore, ever again. None of these things can ever work anymore, ever again. None of these things can ever work anymore, ever again. None of these things can ever work anymore, ever again. None of these things can ever work anymore, ever again. None of these things can ever work anymore, ever again. None of these things...

Words: 321 - Pages: 2

Premium Essay

None

...distribution. The timeframe for this strategy is the end of the week to the vice president. With such a short time to complete this complex tasking would require a formal communication channel, in which to explain the direction to the team with the requirement and with the tasking deadline. Therefore, the final presentation is due Thursday, for the meeting with the vice president on Friday. The presentation to the vice president will occur on Friday will dictate a formal communication channel. Scenario number two, is a role of a travel agency manager, in which first thing in the morning, the username and password to company application system does not allow anyone to access the application. This immediately caused a work stoppage, to where none of the travel agents could perform assigned responsibilities, affecting existing, and new customers. An informal communication channel, of a telephone call to the IT Department, determined the cause to the username and password problem. Because there was no formal communication put in place by the travel agency, this dictates an informal communication channel. The IT Department determined the...

Words: 614 - Pages: 3

Premium Essay

None

...Carl robins works as a recruiter at a new campus for ABC, Inc. However, even though his only worked there for six months his faces serious problems and has found himself in quite a predicament. After hiring fifteen new trainees for his operational supervisor, and scheduling an orientation for new hires to take place in mid-June. After assuring his supervisor Monica Carrolls that the work she ask would be done by the time she wanted, he stated noticing everything was going wrong and time was running out. Soon he realized that the paper work his supervisor asked for was not completed and files were missing. The missing files consisted of several missing applications that weren’t completed on the new trainee’s transcripts. Also he found out none of them had been sent to the clinic for a mandatory drug screen. At this point his frustration was through the roof but, the bad news did not end there only continued. Soon he found out, after reviewing the scheduling log for the training room he notice that there was yet another problem. The training room where he is to hold the orientation for the new trainees is booked up and reserved for the entire month. Alternatives Fighting procrastination is an...

Words: 682 - Pages: 3

Free Essay

None

...with a reality that isn’t changeable. This world; which is contingent; is imperfect in an aesthetic moral way. This goes after reality, because it’s awesome and unique. This phase refers back to Plato, where’s the concept, that the world is “real” and more “factual”; plus, the fantasy world we live in, as of our embodied frame of mind. Our universe has many correct forms. With relations to this, it’s hard to explain correctly; so how they’re not both in common, be kin in any other way. How can you tell from the “really real”, and the “want to be real”? You can examine the perception, which will show the lines of metaphysics realness, and not the outcome of regular skills. With skills, we find objects and forces, that are perceptional, and none perceptional; that we can keep intake. We find a universe that’s always changing. Idols are conceived, breathed, and ended. The solar system is after an agenda course as everything in it. In life, we’re models of constellations, and goes after our fate. You know everything is different because it’s in a current. Within our sense skills, this is the knowledge of oue universe. B. Is The Physical World Real, More or Less Than The Spiritual or Psychological The physical universe is more realer than either, because the physical is concerned on seeing and observing things. With the spirit you doesn’t see it, it’s what embeds the soul, and believing by faith. The psychological is within your mentality, because it causes you to imagine and...

Words: 977 - Pages: 4

Free Essay

None

...Elemental Geosystems, 5e (Christopherson) Chapter 1 Foundations of Geography 1) Geography is described as A) an Earth science. B) a human science. C) a physical science. D) a spatial science. Answer: D 2) The word spatial refers to A) the nature and character of physical space. B) items that relate specifically to society. C) things that are unique and special. D) eras of time. Answer: A 3) A principal methodology governing geographic inquiry A) is behavioral analysis. B) involves spatial analysis. C) uses chronological organization. D) is field work. Answer: B 4) Which of the following best describes the current emphasis in the field of physical geography? A) understanding soil development B) modeling economic interrelationships among countries C) studying weather D) understanding how Earth's systems interact to produce natural phenomena Answer: D 5) Which of the following most accurately characterizes the goal of geography? A) the production of maps B) memorization of the names of places on world and regional maps C) memorization of the imports and exports of a country D) understanding why a place has the characteristics that it does Answer: D 6) Which of the following terms...

Words: 5034 - Pages: 21

Premium Essay

None

...WEAKNESSES OF THE BIG BANG THEORY According to Marmet (2005) , the big bang theory believes that the universe originated from an extremely dense concentration of material. The original expansion of this material is called the big bang theory. Moskowits (2010) describes the big bang theory as an assertion that the universe began extremely hot and dense. Around 14 billion years ago, space itself expanded and cooled down eventually allowing atoms to form and clump together to build the stars and galaxies we see today. Taylor (2012) says According to the Big Bang theory, all matter and all space was originally part of an infinitesimally small point called the Singularity. The theory says nothing about where that singularity came from. It is assumed to have come about by a random quantum event. The theory was first proposed in the 1930s, based on Edwin Hubble's discovery that distant galaxies are receding. Hubble measured the distances to a large number of galaxies which was based on the observed brightness of certain stars within them, he went on to collate these distances with their electromagnetic spectra. As it turned out, more distant galaxies had the features in their spectral lines shifted to lower frequencies in a linear manner: that is, more distant galaxies exhibit greater redshifts. The only known mechanism for generating a spectral shift is the Doppler effect, which means that distant galaxies are receding from us. Another dominant idea connects the dots between the...

Words: 1683 - Pages: 7

Premium Essay

None

...can we look at? 1. Purpose: to predict what’s going to happen in the future 2. Look at recent performance, outlook, changes in the company, changes in the market(s) the company is in, and other indicators. b. Working on Bank of America, what was challenging about, for example, finding Weighted Cost of Capital? 1. It was difficult to find because of the many different markets and submarkets that each have their own cost of capital. 2. The percent of each of these that BoA has was difficult to find. III. Homework Problems a. 9-5 1. Part A: Find Total Debt i. Assets – Equity (Common Stock + Retained Earnings) – Accts. Payable = Total Debt Side note: Equity also includes preferred stock, but this company has none ii. $1,200,000 – $720,000 – $375,000 = $105,000 2. Part B: AFN = (A*/S0) ΔS – (L*/S0) ΔS -MS1 (RR) i. A* = Assets = $1,200,000 ii....

Words: 667 - Pages: 3

Premium Essay

None

...Title Name SCI 207: Dependence of man on the environment Instructor Date Title Abstract In these experiments that we have conducted, we used oil, vinegar, laundry soap, and soil to simulate contaminated groundwater. We then constructed a variety of filters to attempt to clean the ground water and make it drinkable. We also tested various bottled and tap water for certain chemicals. Introduction Many areas have water containing impurities from natural or artificial sources. These impurities may cause health problems, damage equipment or plumbing, or make the water undesirable due to taste, odor, appearance, or staining. Those impurities which cause health problems should be attended to immediately; other problems caused by water impurities can be corrected if they are a nuisance. Before beginning any treatment plan, have water tested by an independent laboratory to determine the specific impurities and level of contamination. This will help you select the most effective and economical treatment method. (Ross, Parrott, Woods, 2009) The reason why we conducted this experiment is to test the filtration to remove oil, vinegar, and laundry detergent has on soil before it reaches groundwater. These chemicals go to our local water supply, but first it goes through the soil. Materials and Methods The materials and methods section should provide a brief description of the specialized materials used in your experiment and...

Words: 687 - Pages: 3

Free Essay

None

...Psychoanalytic Psychology 2004, Vol. 21, No. 3, 353–370 Copyright 2004 by the Educational Publishing Foundation 0736-9735/04/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0736-9735.21.3.353 THE UNEXPECTED LEGACY OF DIVORCE Report of a 25-Year Study Judith S. Wallerstein, PhD Judith Wallerstein Center for the Family in Transition and University of California, Berkeley Julia M. Lewis, PhD San Francisco State University This follow-up study of 131 children, who were 3–18 years old when their parents divorced in the early 1970s, marks the culmination of 25 years of research. The use of extensive clinical interviews allowed for exploration in great depth of their thoughts, feelings, and behaviors as they negotiated childhood, adolescence, young adulthood, and adulthood. At the 25-year follow-up, a comparison group of their peers from the same community was added. Described in rich clinical detail, the findings highlight the unexpected gulf between growing up in intact versus divorced families, and the difficulties children of divorce encounter in achieving love, sexual intimacy, and commitment to marriage and parenthood. These findings have significant implications for new clinical and educational interventions. The study we report here begins with the first no-fault divorce legislation in the nation and tracks a group of 131 California children whose parents divorced in the early 1970s. They were seen at regular intervals over the 25-year span that followed. When we first met our ...

Words: 10773 - Pages: 44