Free Essay

Social

In:

Submitted By yazzyb5
Words 9281
Pages 38
Where Is Utopia in the Brain? DanieL s. Levine
Introduction
The designer of utopian societies, whether fictional or real, often confronts the limits of what is possible for members of our species. But how severe or flexible are those limits? The explosive growth of behavioral neurobiology and experimental psychology in the last decade has produced many results on the biological bases of social interactions. This growth suggests that we can now look to science for some partial answers to the question of limits. Until recently, the social sciences and the biological sciences have mainly developed separate and disconnected accounts of human behavior. In the “nature/nurture controversy,” for example, anthropology has tended to emphasize cultural influences on human nature whereas behavioral biology has tended to emphasize genetic influences. The journalist Matthew Ridley (Nature via Nurture) provides an accessible account of the intellectual history and rhetoric of these two fields. Yet an increasing number of scholars in both areas are now realizing that behavioral biology and anthropology are studying the same human phenomena from different viewpoints. This overlap means there should be an underlying reality that is consistent across the different disciplines regardless of any disagreements in terminology. The behavioral biologist Edward O. Wilson calls this type of interdisciplinary commonality consilience, a term coined earlier by the nineteenth-century philosopher William Whewell from the Latin for “jumping together.” Wilson’s version of consilience, however, is mainly a one-way street. He tends to advocate restructuring the social sciences by breaking social entities down into biological components studied by principles established in the biological sciences: this is an example of what philosophers call reductionism. By contrast, the interdisciplinary social scientist Gerald Cory (The Consilient Brain) has taken steps toward a consilience in which the social and biological sciences are equal partners, each contributing to advances in the other; that is, a theory is developed in which knowledge of economics, political science, and sociology provides insights about brain functions, and vice versa. The
© Utopian Studies 20.2 (2009): 249–274

~UTOPIAN STUDIES 20.2~ approach to consilience taken in this article is closer to Cory’s than to Wilson’s, despite my own grounding in the natural sciences. Further advances are needed in nonreductionistic partnership between the two sets of disciplines in order to better understand the boundaries of cooperative and competitive behavioral tendencies and the social structures tending to encourage cooperative or competitive patterns. Evolutionary accounts of behavior have pervaded popular scientific writing (Pinker, How the Mind Works and Blank Slate) and influenced modern theories of literature (Barash and Barash; Carroll; Gottschall and Wilson; Turner). These accounts have tended to focus on the evolutionary survival or reproduction advantage given to humans by their prevailing behaviours. These accounts thereby provide explanations for dominance hierarchies, wealth inequalities, and gender conflicts both in real life and in literature. Yet evolution is not all survival of the fittest. Many authors (for example Barash and Barash 216–244; Pinker, How the Mind Works 402–406 and 502–509) note that an important aspect of evolutionary adaptation is reciprocal exchange with others of one’s species, an exchange which often leads to cooperation. Hence, each of us carries multiple evolutionary programs, such as cooperation and competition, and sometimes two or more of our evolutionary programs are in conflict. Far from constraining us to act within a narrow genetically determined range of behavior, evolution allows for a range of social arrangements that can selectively enhance or inhibit any one or more of those programs (Wilson, Evolutionary Social Constructivism).

Conflicting Adaptive Behavior Patterns
A biologically oriented social psychologist, Shelley Taylor, has written several articles and a book about the evolutionary advantages of mutual bonding to both female animals and women. Taylor calls this type of behavior tend-andbefriend, and contrasts it with the fight-or-flight (response to danger or stress by aggression or escape) that has been central to evolutionary accounts by and about males (Taylor et al., Biobehavioral Responses). Tend-and-befriend behaviors include mutual support in friendship, help in rearing offspring, and (for non-human animals) grooming. In response to stressful situations, such mutual support can serve as an alternative to fighting or fleeing. Yet tend-and-befriend versus fight-or-flight is not a matter only of gender differences. The benefits of social bonding are as important to the

~250~

Levine Where Is Utopia in the Brain? emotional and physical health of men as they are to women. While there might possibly be statistically significant differences between males and females in their genetic tendencies toward competition or cooperation, both competition and cooperation involve separate biological programs present in all of us (Eisler and Levine). Each of these programs is mediated by a different set of biochemical pathways in the brain: the stress hormone cortisol and the neurotransmitter norepinephrine are particularly important for the competitive programs, and the peptide hormones oxytocin and vasopressin for the cooperative programs. Eisler and Levine mapped out likely brain pathways by which social influences, such as a pattern of caring or abuse both in the family and society at large, can bias people toward one or another type of program. These pathways for social influence involve connection between rational and emotional regions of the brain through a part of the frontal lobes called the orbitofrontal cortex that is required for the efficient processing of social and emotional knowledge. In other words, evolution has selected simultaneously for two conflicting sets of behaviors. Both fight-or-flight and tend-and-befriend have survival value for us in different contexts. A utopian society would be one that increases the bias toward tendand-befriend but does not eliminate fight-or-flight. Fight-or-flight is not only ineradicable but sometimes valuable. It is needed to make us protect against danger (which can be reduced in day-to-day life but not eliminated) and stand up for our rights and interests. Fight-or-flight programs may also be necessary for a sense of adventure, or risk, which is needed for a rewarding life. Some of the earlier utopian communities of fiction (e.g., Bellamy, More, Skinner) and some experimental intentional communities in real life such as the Kaweah Commonwealth in late-nineteenth-century California (Fogarty 148–149), sought to eliminate risk and make the government fully take care of everyone’s needs. What we know about the brain and human nature suggests that people on the whole do not achieve lasting happiness in such a society, which on a cosmic scale has been called the “heated swimming pool of eternal life” (Hendra and Kelly last page). We prefer a different kind of utopia that includes risk, adventure, and humor. (This article returns later to differences among types of utopias in literature.) Because fighting and cooperation are both evolutionarily adaptive programs, an individual’s choice between them at a given time is not made by

~251~

~UTOPIAN STUDIES 20.2~ natural selection. It is a choice over which we, as individuals, families, institutions, and societies, have some control.

Motivation to Live and Not Merely Survive
There is another reason that explanations provided by a survival-of-the-fittest evolutionary orientation are incomplete. It is that not all of our behavior is motivated by survival and reproduction (Turner 226–227). The existence of other motivations in addition to survival and reproduction is sometimes dismissed as non-scientific or non-biological. Yet such a complexity of motives does not go against Darwin’s own beliefs (Loye). In fact, Darwin said in his account of human evolution that he did not believe natural selection or survival of the fittest could account for human altruism or self-sacrificing behavior (Descent of Man 163). Our needs for meaning, bonding, and pleasurable stimulation are just as biologically rooted as our needs for survival. We even partly share these needs with other mammals. The much-studied prairie vole, a Rocky Mountain relative of the mouse, forms stable pair bonds in which both parents nurture offspring (Cho et al.; Insel et al.). Moreover, many cases have been documented of animals seeking, or working for, tickling or other stimulation that gives them pleasure but has no instrumental value for survival or reproduction (Balcombe). A particularly important blow to “survival and reproduction only” theory was the mid-twentieth-century work of Harry and Margaret Harlow on monkeys (Harlow; Harlow and Harlow). Before then, it was believed that juvenile monkeys were interested in their mothers only as sources of food and protection. But the Harlows took some monkeys away from their mothers and gave them a choice of two artificial surrogate mothers, one made of stiff wire and providing milk, the other made of soft cloth and not providing milk. They found the juveniles spent much more time with the cuddly-cloth mothers; they went to the rough-wire mothers only to be fed. This response suggested that the hugging and pleasurable physical sensations that normal monkeys receive from their real mothers is at least as important as the food those mothers provide. In humans, there have been many studies of children raised in orphanages, notably the orphanages constructed in Romania during the Ceausescu dictatorship (Iftene and Roberts). From the pure survival viewpoint,

~252~

Levine Where Is Utopia in the Brain? these Romanian children were well treated, with nutritious food, comfortable shelter, and good medical care. However, they were warehoused in an impersonal manner with no adult caretakers showing them affection or playing with them. To the surprise of many, this lack of love and stimulation led to brain development that lagged several years behind their peers raised in homes. So any utopia that real, biological human beings can live in and enjoy requires social arrangements that optimize fulfillment not only of our survival and reproductive needs but also of our needs for meaning, bonding, personal competence, emotional and physical stimulation, and pleasure. How can we understand the structure and function of our brains in the light of the complexity of our motivations? The understanding requires a set of quantifiable scientific theories that match the complexity of our neural and behavioral functioning. There has been much recent progress in the development of biologically realistic neural network theories of basic processes such as learning, perception, and categorization (Levine, Introduction to Neural and Cognitive Modeling; O’Reilly and Munakata). This type of neural network method has been applied more recently to modeling aspects of human decision making and behavioral rule development (Grossberg; Levine, Angels, Devils, and Censors; Levine and Perlovsky). Such models of brain and behavior are not reductive but include theories for how social interactions influence decision making. Our model building using neural networks is not traditional “valuefree science”: it includes a description of what might be a more or a less optimal state, both for an individual and for society. Optimal behaviors involve all three parts of what the neuroscientist Paul MacLean has called the triune brain: the instinct-bound areas deep in the midbrain; the emotional areas of the limbic system; and the information processing areas of the cerebral cortex. And they involve all major neurotransmitters and several hormones.

Brain Pathways and Rules of Behavior
Because the human brain contains a huge number of partially autonomous subsystems, the brain has been compared to a miniature society (Gazzaniga; Minsky). However, this comparison does not mean our mental life is anarchy. It is more like democracy. Each of the brain’s subsystems has a “voice,” and subsystems often “argue” with each other. Moreover, there is a large brain re-

~253~

~UTOPIAN STUDIES 20.2~ gion functioning more or less as an “arbitrator”: the frontal lobes, sometimes called the executive of the brain (Pribram). Because the frontal lobes can communicate with all three parts of the triune brain (instinctive, emotional, and rational), we can (unfortunately we do not always) make thoughtful decisions that integrate knowledge of both current rewards and preexisting beliefs. The frontal executive operates through feedback connections with other brain regions that are “participants” in decisions, especially emotionrelated regions. It is the part of the brain that is the “most human.” That is, of all brain regions the frontal lobes are the most different between humans and other mammals as well as the slowest to develop in each individual (Thompson et al.). While complex cognitive tasks are not restricted to one brain area, recent neuroimaging results suggest broad functions for certain subregions of the frontal lobes. For example, one subregion is involved in processing emotional and social cues; another is devoted to recording ambiguity or conflict; and a third is dedicated to resolving conflicts and linking memories. The three parts of the frontal lobes devoted to these functions are respectively called the orbital prefrontal cortex (OFC); anterior cingulate cortex (ACC); and dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC). These three areas, along with their interactions with both cognitive and emotional regions of the brain, are regarded as the brain’s executive system (Faw, Pribram). The functions of these three parts are summarized here; Levine (How Does the Brain) describes them in more detail. The OFC’s main functions are mainly social and emotional. A famous nineteenth-century patient, Phineas Gage, changed from a sober foreman to an undependable and irritable person after a railroad accident in which an iron rod went through his skull. Modern reconstruction has determined that his primary area of damage was the OFC, and other patients damaged in that region exhibit similar deficiencies in decision making and in socially appropriate behavior (Damasio). Such clinical observations, along with lesion studies in monkeys, have led neuroscientists to believe that the OFC forms and sustains mental linkages between specific sensory events or behaviors and positive or negative emotional states. By extension, Eisler and Levine (“Nurture, Nature, and Caring”) conjectured that the OFC mediates activation of classes of emotionally significant behaviors, such as tend-andbefriend versus fight-or-flight.

~254~

Levine Where Is Utopia in the Brain?
The ACC is activated when a person must select or switch among different interpretations or aspects of a stimulus, as shown by brain-imaging data (Posner and Peterson). Recent theories of ACC function have emphasized its role in detecting either potential response error or conflict between signals promoting competing responses (Brown and Braver). The DLPFC is the region that manipulates the most complex working memory. It is involved with processing information that may or may not be emotionally loaded, and operates at a higher level of abstraction than does the OFC. The DLPFC creates different possible decision rules whereas OFC emotional circuits filter possible rules based on rewards and punishments received from following these rules (Damasio). All these three regions of the frontal lobes are extensively interconnected with each other as well as with different parts of the brain. These interconnections allow integration of emotional information and task-relevance information within the brain’s overall executive system. The brain interconnections enable us to decide when to continue fulfilling a task and when to allow urgent task-external considerations to override task fulfillment.

Utopias and the Compassionate Revolution
How does all this brain structure relate to utopia? The discussion in the last section suggests that a major role of complex brain-processes is to select and implement rules for what behaviors to perform, in what contexts, and what behaviors to refrain from performing. Societies have rules of behavior just as individuals do; they do so in the form of unofficial social customs as well as laws. The evolution toward a utopian society would be characterized not by abandonment of rules but by a change in the nature of rules: for example, the fictional Ecotopians (Callenbach) had fewer legal restrictions than presentday Americans in some areas (for instance, sex and family) but more restrictions in other areas (for example, economic growth and pollution). How would rules in a utopian society differ from rules in current societies, or in a dystopian society? For clues we can look to some work of the late population-ecologist John Calhoun on the evolution of human behavior rules over history (Space and the Strategy of Life; The Population Crisis). Calhoun explained major revolutions in human thought as adjustments made to cope with the stresses caused by population reaching a certain level of increase. Revolutions lead to changes in the rules for acceptable interpersonal

~255~

~UTOPIAN STUDIES 20.2~ interactions, and these rule changes reduce the percentage of interactions that are frustrating. The peak times of the revolutions Calhoun chronicled correspond to successive doublings of the world’s population, which grows fast enough that each doubling time is approximately half as long as the preceding one. To the current Communications Revolution, centered on computers and electronics, he assigned a peak date of 1988. Yet that revolution has already brought stresses of its own: information explosion, massive detachment, and loss of social connections. With world population now growing at the fastest rate in human history, Calhoun predicted another revolution peaking between 2018 and 2030. If we act unwisely as a species, he added, this future revolution might entail ecological disaster or cultural quiescence. But if we act wisely, it can instead bring about a much more desirable scenario, which he labeled the Compassionate Revolution. The Compassionate Revolution would create societies oriented toward fulfillment of human potential and not merely toward survival. It also means that we would respond to the complexity of society by acting with greater trust of others, including others who differ from us in race, gender, religion, or cultural beliefs. (See also Friedman 320.) Here is Calhoun’s partial description of the transition process from the Communications to the Compassionate Revolution: the present era of radical change will become intensified as the character of roles needed to meet new functions also change[s]. Thus, in the presence of increased exposure to value conflict, there will be required an augmented awareness of the necessity of others to maintain value sets differing from one’s own. Furthermore, realizing one’s own functional role requires expenditure of considerable effort in assisting others to fulfill the objectives of their value sets. It is this awareness, and participation in, the realization of values held by others which characterizes the compassionate perspective. This perspective also includes an awareness that many individuals will experience difficulty in developing and altering their roles and value sets in accordance with the demands of an

~256~

Levine Where Is Utopia in the Brain? overall system which is changing and becoming more complex. Holding to this perspective further requires marked attention to assisting others, whom we ourselves might earlier have been, to recoup from this hopefully temporary derailment. (Space and the Strategy of Life 374)

Calhoun (The Transitional Phase) suggested that achieving this desired state would necessitate an ultimate drop in world population combined with technological prostheses that would add to each of our informationprocessing capacities. Yet he left many details of the revolution’s cultural and social structure to the imagination. Other scientific findings and the work of other social commentators suggest likely implications of the revolution for different areas of life, which we discuss here in the light of published utopian literature. (See also Levine, Explorations in Common Sense and Common Nonsense 255–288.) The Compassionate Revolution would synthesize individualist and communitarian outlooks. People would follow their individual consciences, but would treat society as an interdependent web and consider other people’s welfare in making their decisions. Children would be raised to feel part of a community and to develop a sense of responsibility toward others, but their membership in the community would not be bought at the price of conformity. More generally, the rules for acceptable conduct between people would be based on mutual trust more than on protection of social hierarchies. Increased trust and community without conformity are two of the pillars of any society that embodies the Compassionate Revolution. A third pillar is an orientation toward delight; that is, living responsibly but being driven by pleasure rather than mere obligation. As the late feminist Marilyn French put it: “if we want to create a world that can live together without utter uniformity and totalitarianism, we must gear ourselves to cooperate, must teach our young the pleasures of working/playing together,” and “The question that should provide our standard as we make the choices that will lead us to a new vision is what kinds of human behavior bring us delight” (538). In the next section we look more closely at a few utopian societies in literature that seem to embody the Compassionate Revolution, namely the societies developed by Callenbach, Huxley, and Piercy. We note that those three societies have features in common that support these three pillars of

~257~

~UTOPIAN STUDIES 20.2~ increased trust, community without conformity, and sense of delight. Among the supporting features are child-rearing based on promoting independence; family units that are larger or more fluid than current nuclear families; increased openness of emotional expression; conflict resolution that emphasizes mediation; gender equality, and strong roles for women; and democracy of creativity (artistic production is spread across many people, talented artists and intellectuals also do manual work, and creativity is encouraged in mundane jobs). The final sections of this article return to the brain. Based on the brain processes outlined earlier, we explore scientific arguments for the benefits of Compassionate Revolution social structures for optimal mental functioning. Then we return to the limits question posed at the beginning of this article. In the light of recent knowledge in neuroscience, we examine the arguments of other scientists about the feasibility of successful social engineering.

Decentralized Utopias in Literature
Which of the societies in widely read utopian novels fit the Compassionate Revolution scenario, and the needs of our brains, most closely? Broadly speaking, the closest fits are mid- to late-twentieth-century fictional utopias that are decentralized and evolved spontaneously across large segments of society, such as Ecotopia (Callenbach), Mattapoisett (Piercy), and Pala (Huxley). Utopias created largely by governmental authority or by the charismatic influence of an individual (such as Bellamy, More, Skinner) fit the Compassionate Revolution less well. Charismatic or externally imposed utopias could last over a long period, as indicated by sociological studies showing that intentional communities with strong religious restrictions tend to survive the longest (Sosis and Alcorta). Yet even if such societies have staying power, they would score less well than decentralized societies on the dimensions of meaning and personal freedom that are part of the usual conceptions of “utopia.” The distinction drawn in the last paragraph between “imposed” and “evolved” utopias can be criticized as overly sharp. The categories actually overlap to some degree: much of Pala’s social ethos, for example, arose from the work of two nineteenth-century reformers (the Old Raja and Dr. MacPhail), and the economic arrangement in Bellamy’s 2000 was a spontaneous outgrowth of the progress of corporations. In spite of this overlap, though, there is a difference in emphasis and “flavor” between the two types

~258~

Levine Where Is Utopia in the Brain? of utopias, and the type here labeled “decentralized” is in better harmony with what recent results in psychology and neuroscience tell us about our brains. We now look at several key features of the Compassionate Revolution that can sometimes occur in imposed utopias, but are particularly characteristic of decentralized ones.

Child Rearing That Promotes Independence
Particularly instructive is the difference in child-rearing practices between the imposed utopia of Skinner and the decentralized utopia of Huxley. Skinner brought to his fictional society his behaviorist approach to psychology, which relies on control of behavior by positive and negative reinforcement (104–115, 124). Yet many experimental psychologists since Skinner have discovered that relying on external rewards to reinforce an act can actually reduce intrinsic motivation to perform the act for its own sake (Deci and Ryan). By contrast, Huxley’s society relies on intrinsic motivation and encourages children’s sense of delight in the world—and in the complexity of human relations. Huxley’s Palanese also detect children with antisocial and dictatorial tendencies, and do their best to redirect such tendencies to socially useful ends (208–211). Also, they encourage children as well as adults to air personal feelings, for they trust in the natural desire of their people to form positive social bonds. Palanese children are raised to function as independent adults not to conform to a narrow set of predetermined social expectations. The same is true in Callenbach’s Ecotopia and Piercy’s Mattapoisett, and is particularly noticeable in their schools. Callenbach’s visiting outside narrator, Will Weston, notes with astonishment that the Ecotopians shunned behaviorcontrol drugs, so “unable to make difficult children adapt to the schools, they had to adapt the schools to the children!” (Ecotopia 123). Ecotopian and Mattapoisett school children both spend relatively little time in classrooms and more time applying their classroom-acquired knowledge to actual productive enterprises such as farms, gardens, and even factories.

Fluid Family Units
The independence children develop in decentralized utopias is enhanced by their exposure to the influence of multiple adults, not just a nuclear family or even a biological extended family. Innovations in family structure in these

~259~

~UTOPIAN STUDIES 20.2~ novels take on multiple forms, perhaps the most radical being in Piercy’s society where each child has two mothers in addition to his or her biological mother, and those mothers can be male as well as female. In Huxley’s society, children are free to escape from their parents at any age and migrate into another home belonging to a member of what Palanese call a Mutual Adoption Club. In one passage in Ecotopia, the narrator asks a boy whose mother was gone for a week if this made him lonely, and the child answers, with surprise, “Why should I be lonely? Everybody else is here” (101–102). The Ecotopian fluid-family structure extends into adulthood. Likeminded groups of adults that are not biologically related but share a common enterprise, such as journalism in Will Weston’s case, live together in units that they call “families.”

Emotional Openness
Each of the three utopian societies in the novels of Callenbach, Piercy, and Huxley contrasts sharply with outside societies in how much it accepts open expression of anger, sadness, and joy. In each novel, the outside visitor (Will Weston in Ecotopia, Connie Ramos in Woman on the Edge of Time, or Will Farnaby in Island) experiences culture shock on his or her first encounter with intense emotional expression. Farnaby feels particularly strange because the first member of the society he meets (the child Mary Sarojini) talks intimately to him by asking him to remember and mentally work through his feelings about an encounter with a snake (11–13). In all three of those novels, however, as the visitor becomes more acclimated he or she gradually learns to appreciate the emotional intensity. The visitor finds it refreshing that conflicts come out in the open and can be resolved in ways that are likely to satisfy all people involved.

Mediation of Conflicts
A dramatic example of conflict resolution with emotional openness is the Ecotopian practice of cooperative criticism (Ecotopia 53–55). The narrator witnesses an episode in a restaurant wherein a customer complains that his scrambled eggs are overcooked. The woman who cooked the eggs is a valued employee and appreciated by other customers. So a discussion ensues about her workload and some customers offer to help her in the kitchen, so everyone is made happy including the original complainer.

~260~

Levine Where Is Utopia in the Brain?
In Piercy’s future Cape Cod society, this same manner of listening to everyone’s viewpoint is applied to a political assembly (142–145). At this assembly, a dispute occurs about whether to use scarce iron ore for repair of a pretty bridge or for weapons needed to defend their freedom, and one participant suggests use of a different (biological) material for the bridge so the ore can be available for weapons. Such disputes may end up with winners and losers, but the loser’s face and sense of equality are preserved by having the winner treat the loser. Some conflicts do not involve substantive issues but simply result from individual emotional strains leading to potentially destructive forms of anger and frustration. In utopian societies dangerous emotions are often discharged by means of physical release not directed at the object of the emotion, such as the Palanese Rakshasi Hornpipe (Huxley 228), named after a Hindu demon, and the Ecotopian war games, resulting in injuries that are usually minor and seldom life-threatening (Ecotopia 173–177).

Gender Equality
Imposed and decentralized utopias often differ sharply in their gender roles. More’s utopia, as befits his time period, is clearly male-dominated, and Bellamy’s and Skinner’s include long preachy pronouncements by male leaders. In more recent utopian novels, whether the authors are female (Piercy) or male (Huxley or Callenbach), the role of explaining the utopia’s social mores to the outsiders is taken by both male and female characters who are more concise than Bellamy’s Dr. Leete or Skinner’s Frazier. Fittingly, the nineteenth-century utopia with perhaps the strongest decentralized flavor is Charlotte Perkins Gilman’s Herland, a society composed of women.1 In the novels of Callenbach, Huxley, and Piercy, women exert more power and gender roles are less rigidly defined than in most current societies. Women perform hard physical work, and men openly express emotions, including feelings of weakness. The caring values traditionally associated with females are blended in both sexes with the assertive values traditionally associated with males. In Piercy’s society, this blending leads to some blurring of gender distinctions (for example, men nursing children at their breasts) but not in the other two: in fact, the narrator in Ecotopia remarks about how feminine the women are and how masculine the men in spite of the role changes (42). In all these societies, equality of power also enhances sexual pleasure.

~261~

~UTOPIAN STUDIES 20.2~ Democracy of Creativity
Nowhere is the democracy of decentralized utopian societies more manifested than in the creative aspects of work. This principle has many implications not only for the classic creative pursuits in the arts and sciences, but for increasing fulfillment in the performance of other jobs including those of farm and factory workers and telephone operators. The societies in Callenbach, Huxley’s, and Piercy’s books all contain artists and intellectuals who are exceptionally good and whose work is widely known and respected. Yet people also respect the artistic and intellectual work of amateurs. The narrator in Ecotopia notes that participation of the young in the arts is nearly universal, that every farm, factory, or extended family has a musical group, and that average people successfully design many of the buildings they live in or use (170–171). A nurse in Pala notes wryly that her society is “the place for stupid people,” and that the intelligence of ordinary people (in which she includes herself ) is as important to the society as the intelligence of exceptionally bright people (Huxley 190). In future-Mattapoisett, Connie from the present is astonished by the profusion of art appearing all over the translucent panels in a room she visits, and even more astonished to learn the art was created by average people and not professional artists (Piercy 67). The lack of a sharp boundary in these three societies between “creative” people and “ordinary” people also means that the best artists (such as Jackrabbit in Woman on the Edge of Time) and scientists (such as Ranga in Island) are not, and do not expect to be, exempted from such tasks as farm work and cooking. In Looking Backward, by contrast, “brain workers” and “hand workers” are largely separate categories (Bellamy 137–138) and book authors get furloughs from the industrial army (Bellamy 200). (Walden Two is closer in this regard to the decentralized utopias, expecting physical labor from creative scientists [Skinner 56–57]). Conversely, the decentralized societies ascribe dignity and creative potential to paid jobs that current Western societies regard as menial. For example, the Ecotopian wire service clerk Jerry at first annoys the American reporter Will by reading and commenting on his dispatches instead of simply “doing his job” by sending them out quickly (Ecotopia 27). But later Will’s attitude changes: he has a dream about trying unsuccessfully to telephone his Ecotopian lover from New York, and wakes up appreciating Jerry who would

~262~

Levine Where Is Utopia in the Brain? make the effort to complete the call “even if it had to go via Timbuktu, because he could tell it mattered” (Ibid. 208).

Brain Pathways for Utopian Functioning
What can be said about the impact on brain function of human interactions shaped by the Compassionate Revolution? In particular, how are the features of that revolution and of decentralized utopias—independence-based child-rearing, fluid families, emotional openness, mediation-based conflict resolution, gender equality, and democracy of creativity—manifested in brain processes? This section explores evidence from cognitive neuroscience suggesting that those features of interpersonal social interaction foster optimal brain functioning. The available knowledge from brain-behavior interactions is sketchy enough not to constitute definitive proof but extensive enough to generate plausible arguments. The arguments will be strengthened when future results emerge from the novel but growing field of social neuroscience, which describes interactions between the brain and social systems (Cacioppo and Berntson).

Child Rearing and Fluid Family Units
Childhood patterns of good or ill treatment have effects on the brain that last through adulthood (Eisler and Levine; Perry et al.). The wiring in the frontal lobes, the key part of the brain for our character and moral development, is not fully developed until a person is in her or his mid-20s (Thompson et al.). Social influences on brain development partly explain why children raised in a caring manner tend to become caring adults (Eisenberg 64–131), and therefore able to encode behavior rules appropriate for living in a utopia. Also, critical thinking skills are most likely to last through adult life if those skills are taught early in childhood. Exposure to influence from multiple adults should enhance the development of both cognitive and moral skills because cognitive psychologists have shown that a skill is typically learned best when it is learned from more than one source (Schmidt and Bjork).

Emotional Openness
Recent findings in neuroscience have increasingly pointed to the important role of emotions in human decision-processes. Antonio Damasio ran tests on several patients who were damaged in the orbitofrontal cortex, the area of

~263~

~UTOPIAN STUDIES 20.2~ the frontal lobes that processes emotional and social stimuli. He found those patients were terrible decision makers even if their cognitive abilities were largely intact (34–51). The decision-making deficit occurs because lacking emotional involvement in the possible outcome of a decision, the patients have no basis for deciding on one reasonable course of action over another. Damasio concluded that effective decisions require strong connections between emotional and rational parts of the brain. This finding suggests that social mores that encourage people to be honest in showing their emotions to others indirectly enhance the accuracy of communication of all types of information.

Mediation of Conflicts
Brain functioning is likely to be enhanced, on the cognitive as well as the emotional level, if conflicts are regarded as situations where all parties are likely to gain something. Mediated conflicts are less likely than adversarial conflicts to generate high levels of brain-stress hormones such as cortisol that are involved in fight-or-flight responses. While cortisol is useful in responding to emergency situations, chronic high cortisol levels are harmful to both cognitive and emotional functioning by putting strains on the frontal-lobe executive system. In extreme cases, this stress accounts for enduring brain deficiencies in adults who were chronically abused as children (Perry et al.). In less extreme cases, chronic or severe stress tends to reduce the flexibility of neural connections in the hippocampus, the brain area involved in consolidating memories, and thereby impairs cognitive functioning for a short time after the stressful event (Sapolsky). By contrast, successful conflict resolution often generates positive affect, and positive affect can enhance cognitive functioning by a mechanism involving dopamine, the neurotransmitter associated with rewards (Ashby, Turken, and Isen).

Gender Equality
Female influences enhance the likelihood of tend-and-befriend responses to social conflicts (Taylor et al.). Taylor and her colleagues note that such responses, including mutual grooming in female animals and supportive social bonding in women, are mediated by the hormone oxytocin, which was first observed in connection with lactation. Bonding has been studied in two species of rodents that are closely related but have radically different social or-

~264~

Levine Where Is Utopia in the Brain? ganization: the prairie vole, which is monogamous with strong male-female pair bonding and both parents involved in care of young, and the montane vole, which is promiscuous with fathers uninvolved with young (Insel et al.). Oxytocin attaches to reward-related brain areas in the pair-bonding prairie vole but not in the promiscuous montane vole. In male prairie voles, a different hormone (vasopressin, associated with protective behavior) plays a stronger role than oxytocin in promoting pair bonding. Yet this does not mean that oxytocin and its stress-reducing effects are for females only. In prairie voles, partner preference development in either sex requires intact brain-receptor molecules for both oxytocin and vasopressin (Cho et al.), and in humans, oxytocin administered through the nose induces trust in both sexes (Kosfeld et al.). All this evidence is still circumstantial, but suggests that an overall decrease across society in levels of stress hormones and situational increases in levels of oxytocin (a hormone which only lasts a short time in the body) are part of the neurological basis for healthy social functioning. Such an outcome is likely to involve empowerment of women and reduction of social stress for both women and men (Eisler and Levine 41–46).

Democracy of Creativity
Future developments in social neuroscience are needed to provide coherent theories of differences between societies where everyone is encouraged to be creative but also asked to do menial tasks and societies where a few are creative while the majority do mostly menial tasks. The knowledge we have now, though, suggests that expressing creativity is closely tied to the universal human need for exploration to make sense of one’s environment (Deci and Ryan; Harlow and Harlow). Behavioral studies of group creativity on mundane problems show that encouraging the capacity for exploration enhances the ability to arrive at solutions that are both novel and effective (Rietzschel, Nijstad, and Stroebe).

How Far Can Social Engineering Go?
Are our brains powerful and flexible enough to work together in creating and maintaining a society incorporating principles of the Compassionate Revolution? Or are there inherent limitations in human nature that doom us to fall short of the possibilities embodied in decentralized utopias such as those of

~265~

~UTOPIAN STUDIES 20.2~
Callenbach, Huxley, and Piercy? We cannot answer this question with complete confidence. Yet the more we learn about our brains, the richer becomes the range of imaginable possibilities for the types of behaviors and cognitions that our brains can encode—specifically, for the types of rules we can incorporate about what actions to perform, and what actions to refrain from performing, in what contexts (Bunge and Wallis). Again, growth of knowledge in the area of social neuroscience will provide more answers in the near future. Recall the three large regions of the frontal lobes involved in rule setting: one region processes emotional and social cues; another detects errors or conflicts; and a third resolves conflicts and links working memories. A fanciful analogy has been made between these three brain regions and the three branches (legislative, judicial, and executive) of the United States government (Levine, How Does the Brain 171). The emotional/social processor (orbitofrontal cortex) is legislative: it judges appropriateness of actions using the “pulse of the people” (the “people” being the brain’s emotional regions). The error detector (anterior cingulate) is judicial: it reins in neural patterns of the other two areas that stray too far from principles of effective behavior. The working memory region (dorsolateral prefrontal cortex) is executive: it has the specialized expertise required to create and try out significant behavioral “policies.” Connections of all these regions with each other and with the brain’s sensory, motor, and emotional regions seem to be heavily influenced by learning, particularly from childhood development but also from adult experience. Continuing the governmental analogy, the plasticity of brain connections enables a range of possible outlooks from “ultra-liberal” to “ultra-conservative.” Levine (Angels, Devils, and Censors) reviewed brain interactions involving all three regions of the frontal lobes discussed here and several other regions, which could mediate an individual’s gradual transition from more restrictive to more fulfilling rules of behavior. The research psychiatrist Robert Cloninger defined such growth in terms of development along three character dimensions: self-directedness (relating to acceptance of the individual self ); cooperativeness (relating to acceptance of other people); and self-transcendence (relating to acceptance of nature in general). He emphasized that levels of these dimensions were largely determined by development not by genetic differences between individuals. This finding suggests that most normal in-

~266~

Levine Where Is Utopia in the Brain? dividuals are open to developing a considerable range of possible rules of behavior. In particular, most normal people probably have the capacity for a range of social interactions from utopian to dystopian. Interactions between brains and society are not understood well enough for us to draw firm conclusions about the range of social arrangements possible with the brains we have. Yet the large body of data we already have about brain connections suggests no inherent limitations that make the Compassionate Revolution or decentralized utopian societies impossible. Sociologists sometimes argue that societies have dynamics of their own that cannot be reduced to the dynamics of their individual members (Durkheim). Yet the current mainstream in sociology believes there is constant mutual influence between individuals and societies. Societal norms for behavior constrain the possible actions of each individual whereas individual needs constrain the possible structures and customs of society (Giddens; Habermas). A belief in strong societal influences on character does not imply that each human being is a “blank slate” at birth (Pinker, Blank Slate). While Cloninger noted learned aspects of character, many other important aspects of human personality are believed to have a strong genetic component. Yet genetically influenced personality traits can also be strengthened or weakened by social interactions and customs. One such trait is need for cognition (Cacioppo and Petty), defined as the motivation to enjoy effortful cognitive activity and to analyze arguments deeply. Another is need for closure (Webster and Kruglanski), defined as the motivation to come to decisions and opinions quickly, often without having thought through the relevant issues. The decentralized utopias of Callenbach, Huxley, and Piercy tend to enhance and fulfill their citizens’ needs for cognition. By contrast, the imposed utopias of Bellamy, More, and Skinner tend to provide their citizens conceptual closure at the sacrifice of optimal cognition. Yet some neuroscientists remain skeptical about how much human behavior can be changed by social arrangements. One of them is Michael Gazzaniga, a pioneer in “left and right brain” research who was a member of President George W. Bush’s ethics panel. Gazzaniga contrasted two common views of human nature and society. One view, which he called externalist, is that society should set up structures, agencies, institutions to help manage the individual’s affairs (Gazzaniga 191). The other view, which he called internal-

~267~

~UTOPIAN STUDIES 20.2~ ist, is that “personal beliefs are the proper guides for personal action and that by placing responsibility on the person, social groups function better” (191). Gazzaniga argued in favor of the internalist view, based on results from psychological experiments showing that external rewards have little effect on people’s beliefs. In one experiment, Yale students who disapproved of the police’s handling of a riot were asked to go against their beliefs by writing an essay in support of the police, and were offered varying amounts of money to write this essay (Cohen). After writing, these students were again asked their opinions about the riot. Those students offered the most money changed their beliefs the least after writing the essay. Gazzaniga explained the Yale results using the brain’s organization into distinct subsystems. If one subsystem generates behaviors in response to rewards or punishments, it can remain largely separate from another subsystem that generates beliefs. The greater the rewards for an action, the more the person can attribute the action to the reward and not to his or her own beliefs. He used the separateness of behavior and beliefs to explain why externalist social programs often reduce individual initiative and fail to reach their goals. Yet government social programs can facilitate responsible actions by individuals who are already motivated toward those actions. In the 1970s, for example, my mother’s father was over eighty years old and could no longer care for himself. He lived in Toronto but wanted to move to California to be with my mother. She wanted to move him into a managed apartment or retirement home in her area, but he stayed in Toronto because the Ontario provincial health insurance system paid for ninety percent of his care at a high-quality retirement center. If the United States or the state of California had also provided a comprehensive government health insurance system, my grandfather would have been able to get the same care in California, and my mother could then have been more actively involved in seeing to his day-today welfare. In the decentralized utopias of Callenbach, Huxley, and Piercy, governments enact enough laws and programs to ensure fairness, prevent poverty, and facilitate personal creativity—including entrepreneurship in the case of Ecotopia. However, those governments do not run the economy or make most major decisions, as they do in the societies of More, Bellamy, and Skinner. The internalist outlook Gazzaniga favors sounds hostile to utopian

~268~

Levine Where Is Utopia in the Brain? social visions. However, the data he cited can be interpreted as favoring decentralized but not imposed utopias. They suggest that individual initiative would be reduced in an imposed utopia, but not in a decentralized utopia with a less intrusive form of activist government.

Conclusions
The biological processes we have discussed here are manifestations of the brain’s overarching function, which is to mediate between the rest of the body and the environment. In order to perform its function, it must be open to virtually any type of powerful influence from the environment, including influences from cultural customs and official institutions. That means that the brain regions discussed in this article have the potential to be involved in creating either a utopia or dystopia, or anything in between. So the answer to “where is utopia in the brain?” is “everywhere, and so is its opposite.”

Acknowledgments
The author is indebted to Kenneth Roemer and to two anonymous reviewers for valuable comments and suggestions on earlier versions of this manuscript. The manuscript is adapted from the author’s presentation at the 2006 annual meeting of the Society for Utopian Studies, on which Rebecca Totaro and Cade Jameson provided helpful feedback.

Endnote
Feminist influence is also notable in Ursula Le Guin’s cooperative society of Anarres, which had arisen out of a female-led rebellion against another society. However, Anarres is rule-bound and hostile to the individual freedom of the male protagonist and his female partner, both of them creative scientists.
1

Works Cited
Ashby, Gregory, Ann Turken, and Alice Isen. “A Neuropsychological Theory of Positive Affect and Its Influences on Cognition.” Psychological Review 106 (1999): 526–550.

~269~

~UTOPIAN STUDIES 20.2~
Balcombe, Jonathan. The Pleasurable Kingdom: Animals and the Nature of Feeling Good. New York: Macmillan, 2006. Barash, David, and Nanelle Barash. Madame Bovary’s Ovaries: A Darwinian Look at Literature. New York: Bantam Dell, 2005. Bellamy, Edward. Looking Backward, 2000–1887. 1888. Ed. John L. Thomas. Cambridge, MA: Belknap-Harvard UP, 1967. Brown, Joshua, and Todd Braver. “Learned Predictions of Error Likelihood in the Anterior Cingulate Cortex.” Science 307 (2005): 1118–1121. Bunge, Silvia, and Jonathan Wallis, ed. Neuroscience of Rule-Guided Behavior. Oxford: Oxford UP, 2008. Cacioppo, John, and Gary Berntson, ed. Social Neuroscience: Key Readings. New York: Psychology P, 2005. Cacioppo, John, and Richard Petty. “The Need for Cognition.” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 42 (1982): 116–131. Calhoun, John B. “The Population Crisis Leading to the Compassionate Revolution and Environmental Design.” World Journal of Psychosynthesis 4 (1972). ___. “Space and the Strategy of Life. Behavior and Environment.” The Use of Space by Animals and Men. Ed. Aristide Esser. New York: Plenum, 1971. 329–387. ___. “The Transitional Phase in Knowledge Evolution.” Man-Environment Systems 14 (1984): 131–142. Callenbach, Ernest. Ecotopia. Berkeley, CA: Banyan Tree Books, 1975. ___. Ecotopia Emerging. Toronto: Bantam, 1982. Carroll, Joseph. Literary Darwinism. New York: Routledge, 2004. Cho, Mary, Courtney DeVries, Jessie Williams, and Sue Carter. “The Effects of Oxytocin and Vasopressin on Partner Preferences in Male and Female Prairie Voles (Microtus Ochrogaster).” Behavioral Neuroscience 113 (1999): 1071–1079. Cloninger, Robert. “A New Conceptual Paradigm from Genetics and Psychobiology for the Science of Mental Health.” Australia and New Zealand

~270~

Levine Where Is Utopia in the Brain?
Journal of Psychiatry 33 (1999): 174–186. Cohen, Arthur. “An Experiment on Small Rewards for Discrepant Compliance and Attitude Change.” Explorations in Cognitive Dissonance. Ed. Jack Brehm and Arthur Cohen. New York: Wiley, 1962. 73–78. Cory, Gerald. The Consilient Brain: The Bioneurological Basis of Economics, Society, and Politics. New York: Kluwer Academic, 2004. Damasio, Antonio. Descartes’ Error: Emotion, Reason, and the Human Brain. New York: Grosset/Putnam, 1994. Darwin, Charles. Descent of Man, and Selection in Relation to Sex. Princeton, NJ: Princeton UP, 1871. Deci, Edward, and Richard Ryan. Intrinsic Motivation and Self-Determination in Human Behavior. New York: Plenum, 1985. Durkheim, Émile. 1895 (in French as Les Règles de la Methode Sociologique). The Rules of the Sociological Method, New York: Free P, 1938. Eisenberg, Nancy. The Caring Child. Cambridge, MA: Harvard UP, 1992. Eisler, Riane, and Daniel S. Levine. “Nurture, Nature and Caring: We Are Not Prisoners of Our Genes.” Brain and Mind 3 (2002): 9–52. Faw, William. “Pre-frontal Executive Committee for Perception, Working Memory, Attention, Long-Term Memory, Motor Control, and Thinking: A Tutorial Review.” Consciousness and Cognition 12 (2003): 83–139. Fogarty, Robert S. Dictionary of American Communal and Utopian History. Westport, CT: Greenwood, 1980. French, Marilyn. Beyond Power: On Women, Men, and Morals. New York: Ballantine, 1986. Friedman, Thomas. The World is Flat: A Brief History of the Twenty-first Century. 2nd ed. New York: Farrar, Straus, and Giroux, 2006. Gazzaniga, Michael. The Social Brain: Discovering the Networks of the Mind. New York: Basic Books, 1985. Giddens, Anthony. The Constitution of Society: Outline of the Theory of Structuration. Oxford: Polity P, 1984. Gilman, Charlotte Perkins. Herland. 1915. Ed. Ann Lane. New York: Pan-

~271~

~UTOPIAN STUDIES 20.2~ theon Books, 1979. Grossberg, Stephen. “The Complementary Brain.” Trends in Cognitive Science 4 (2000): 233–246. Habermas, Jurgen. The Theory of Communicative Action. Boston: Beacon P, 1984. Harlow, Harry. “The Nature of Love.” American Psychologist 13 (1958): 673– 685. Harlow, Harry, and Margaret Harlow. “Social Deprivation in Monkeys.” Scientific American 207.5 (1962): 137–146. Hendra, Tony, and Sean Kelly. Not the Bible. New York: Ballantine, 1983. Huxley, Aldous. Island. New York: Harper & Row, 1962. Iftene, Felicia, and Nasreen Roberts. “Romanian Adolescents: Literature Review and Psychiatric Presentation of Romanian Adolescents Adopted in Romania and in Canada.” Canadian Child and Adolescent Psychology Review 13 (2004): 110–113. Insel, Thomas, James Winslow, Zuoxin Wang, and Larry Young. “Oxytocin, Vasopressin, and the Neuroendocrine Basis of Pair Bond Formation.” Vasopressin and Oxytocin: Molecular, Cellular, and Clinical Advances. Ed. Hans Zingg, Charles Bourque, and Daniel Bichet. New York: Plenum, 1998. 215–230.Kosfeld, Michael, Markus Heinrichs, Paul Zak, Urs Fischbacher, and Ernst Fehr. “Oxytocin Increases Trust in Humans.” Nature 435 (2005): 673–676. Le Guin, Ursula. The Dispossessed. New York: Harper & Row, 1974. Levine, Daniel S. “Angels, Devils, and Censors in the Brain.” ComPlexus 2 (2005): 35–59. ___. Explorations in Common Sense and Common Nonsense. 1998. http:// www.uta.edu/psychology/faculty/levine/EBOOK/index.htm. ___. “How Does the Brain Create, Change, and Selectively Override Its Rules of Conduct?” Neurodynamics of Higher-level Cognition and Consciousness. Ed. Robert Kozma, and Leonid Perlovsky. Heidelberg: Springer-Verlag, 2007. 163–181. ___. Introduction to Neural and Cognitive Modeling. 2nd ed. Mahwah, NJ:

~272~

Levine Where Is Utopia in the Brain?
Erlbaum, 2000. Loye, David. “Charles Darwin, Paul MacLean, and the Lost Origins of ‘the Moral Sense’: Some Implications for General Evolution Theory.” World Futures 40 (1994): 187–196. MacLean, Paul. The Triune Brain in Evolution: Role in Paleocerebral Functions. New York: Plenum, 1990. Minsky, Marvin, Society of Mind. New York: Simon and Schuster. 1986. More, Sir Thomas. 1516 (in Latin). Utopia. Second ed. trans. and ed. Robert M. Adams. New York: Norton, 1972. O’Reilly, Randall, and Yuko Munakata. Computational Explorations in Cognitive Neuroscience: Understanding the Mind by Simulating the Brain. Cambridge, MA: MIT P, 2000. Perry, Bruce, Ronnie Pollard, Toi Blakley, William Baker, and Domenico Vigilante. “Childhood Trauma, the Neurobiology of Adaptation, and ‘UseDependent’ Development of the Brain: How ‘States’ Become ‘Traits.’” Infant Mental Health Journal 16 (1995): 271–291. Piercy, Marge. Woman on the Edge of Time. New York: Fawcett Crest, 1976. Pinker, Steven. Blank Slate: The Modern Denial of Human Nature. New York: Viking, 2002. ___. How the Mind Works. New York: Norton, 1997. Posner, Michael, and Steven Petersen. “The Attention System of the Human Brain.” Annual Review of Neuroscience 13 (1990): 25–42. Pribram, Karl. “The Primate Frontal Cortex—Executive of the Brain.” Psychophysiology of the Frontal Lobes. Ed. Karl Pribram and Aleksandr Luria. New York: Academic P, 1973. 293–314. Ridley, Matt. Nature via Nurture. New York: HarperCollins, 2003.

Rietzschel, Eric, Bernard Nijstad, and Wolfgang Stroebe. “Relative AcJournal of Experimental Social Psychology 43 (2007): 933–946.

cessibility of Domain Knowledge and Creativity: The Effects of Knowledge Activation on the Quantity and Originality of Generated Ideas.”

Sapolsky, Robert. “Stress and Plasticity in the Limbic System.” Neurochemical

~273~

~UTOPIAN STUDIES 20.2~
Research 28 (2003): 1735–1742. Schmidt, Richard, and Robert Bjork. “New Conceptualizations of Practice: Common Principles in Three Paradigms Suggest New Concepts for Training.” Psychological Science 3 (1992): 207–217. Skinner, B. F. Walden Two. 1948. With a new pref. by the author. New York: Macmillan, 1969. Sosis, Richard, and Candace Alcorta. “Signalling, Solidarity, and the Sacred: The Evolution of Religious Behavior.” Evolutionary Anthropology 12 (2003): 264–274. Taylor, Shelley. The Tending Instinct: How Nurturing Is Essential to Who We Are and How We Live. New York: Henry Holt, 2002. Taylor, Shelley, Laura Klein, Brian Lewis, Tara Gruenewald, Regan Gurung, and John Updegraff. “Biobehavioral Responses to Stress in Females: Tend-and-Befriend, Not Fight-or-Flight.” Psychological Review 107 (2000): 411–429. Thompson, Paul, Jay Giedd, Roger Woods, David MacDonald, Alan Evans, and Arthur Toga. “Growth Patterns in the Developing Brain Detected by Using Continuum Mechanical Tensor Maps.” Nature 404 (2000): 190–193. Turner, Frederick. Natural Classicism. New York: Paragon House, 1985. Webster, Donna, and Arie Kruglanski. “Individual Differences in the Need for Cognitive Closure.” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 67 (1994): 1049–1062. Wilson, David Sloan. “Evolutionary Social Constructivism.” The Literary Animal. Ed. Jonathan Gottschall and David Sloan Wilson. 20–37. Wilson, Edward. Consilience: The Unity of Knowledge. New York: Alfred A. Knopf, 1998.

~274~

Copyright of Utopian Studies is the property of Society for Utopian Studies and its content may not be copied or emailed to multiple sites or posted to a listserv without the copyright holder's express written permission. However, users may print, download, or email articles for individual use.

Similar Documents

Premium Essay

The Social

...Abelson et al.), 1968 Voices of Modern Psychology, 1969 The Social Animal, 1972, 1976, 1980, 1984, 1988, 1992, 1995, 1999, 2004; (with J. Aronson), 2008 Readings About the Social Animal, 1973, 1977, 1981, 1984, 1988, 1992, 1995, 1999, 2004; (with J. Aronson), 2008 Social Psychology (with R. Helmreich), 1973 Research Methods in Social Psychology (with J. M. Carlsmith & P. Ellsworth), 1976 The Jigsaw Classroom (with C. Stephan et al.), 1978 Burnout: From Tedium to Personal Growth (with A. Pines & D. Kafry), 1981 Energy Use: The Human Dimension (with P. C. Stern), 1984 The Handbook of Social Psychology (with G. Lindzey), 3rd ed., 1985 Career Burnout (with A. Pines), 1988 Methods of Research in Social Psychology (with P. Ellsworth, J. M. Carlsmith, & M. H. Gonzales), 1990 Age of Propaganda (with A. R. Pratkanis), 1992, 2000 Social Psychology, Vols. 1–3 (with A. R. Pratkanis), 1992 Social Psychology: The Heart and the Mind (with T. D. Wilson & R. M. Akert), 1994 Cooperation in the Classroom: The Jigsaw Method (with S. Patnoe), 1997 Nobody Left to Hate: Teaching Compassion After Columbine, 2000 Social Psychology: An Introduction (with T. D. Wilson & R. M. Akert), 2002, 2005, 2007 The Adventures of Ruthie and a Little Boy Named Grandpa (with R. Aronson), 2006 Mistakes Were Made (But Not By Me) (with C. Tavris), 2007 Books by Joshua Aronson Improving Academic Achievement, 2002 The Social Animal To Vera, of course The Social Animal, Tenth Edition Sponsoring Editor: Erik Gilg...

Words: 208005 - Pages: 833

Free Essay

Social

...SOCIAL POLICY ANALYSIS: WELFARE TO WORK Research is key to the continued development of the theory and knowledge base of social work practice. The AASW supports the undertaking of research as a key activity to build and maintain the mission of the social work profession. Research is an essential area of social work practice and is included in the AASW Practice Standards for Social Workers, (section 4.3), highlighting its importance to social work practice. Social workers are expected to “understand the role of research and evaluation in obtaining and generating new knowledge for practice.” (AASW 2013) Social work research informs professional practice. Through social work research, the profession can: * Assess the needs and resources of people in their environments * Evaluate the effectiveness of social work services in meeting people needs * Demonstrate relative costs and benefits of social work services * Advance professional education in light of changing contexts for practice * Understand the impact of legislation and social policy on the clients and communities we serve Australia’s population is ageing. At the same time, spending on income support payments as a whole (and not just age pensions) is anticipated to increase. Without on-going high rates of economic growth, the nation will struggle to support its ageing population and those who rely on government income support (ABS, 2006). Australian governments have long recognized the...

Words: 3103 - Pages: 13

Premium Essay

Social Construction of Reality

...CheckPoint Social Construction of Reality Aldonia Bailey SOC/120 11/29/2013 Carol Parker Currently I work in a chiropractor office where, I have been for a little over a year. My coworkers at first knew me as the quite one but as time went by they got to know me. So now they know me as the feisty one in the office, because I rarely put up with clients ignorance. My coworkers also think I’m antisocial because I’m not fond of going out after work or going to many office functions. I mostly agree with them, because I feel like I should treat work as a professional space. In the beginning I felt different, I socialized with my coworkers by going out on weekends and after work, but as time went on I felt like all my coworkers did was go to clubs and get drunk. However this may work for some people its just not the lifestyle I’m comfortable with. Even though my interaction now is limited with my coworkers I think I’m more open and sociable with my family (mainly my aunt). My personable social status is determined by my family dynamic, mostly because I feel comfortable where I can let my guard down. When considering how my social status influences my view of the world, I instantaneously think of my childhood and the way my mother treated me. My childhood was stressful mainly because my mother wasn’t the most financial or emotionally stable individual. With that being said she was also very physically and mentally abusive towards me and my brother so the cops and DPSS were always...

Words: 333 - Pages: 2

Free Essay

Social

...are at least three different social media networks that I have used or still use. MySpace is one of the ones that really got social media networking to where it is now. I used to use MySpace to talk to friends, share pictures, promote my music, and share my blogs. These were the main functions that I used MySpace for. MySpace was one of the first big steps that lead other social media networking where it is. Being able to interact with your friends and family in a virtual world is a big success to being able to so be many places at once. Facebook is another social media networking tool that took the world by surprise. With Facebook you are able to share photos, express what is on your mind with your friends and family, write messages between you and others, and stay connected with people all over the world. Facebook came and conquered most of, if not all, social media networking web sites. The main thing that separated Facebook from any other social media web site is that it was made for college students. Little by little it became so popular that everyone wanted to be a part of this network. Businesses really took advantage of the social networking site for advertising. With so many advertising competitors on the network, it is hard for users not to take advantage of the products advertised. Businesses even have commercials that tell to “like” them on Facebook to ensure the attention of the social media users. The latest installment to the social media networking web sites that...

Words: 341 - Pages: 2

Premium Essay

Social

...Speech Thesis: Since the social network websites offer social, educational, and economical benefits to their users, everyone who is seeking opportunities in those dimensions should participate in the social network websites. Introduction Social network sites such as Facebook, Myspace, and CyWorld allow individuals to present themselves, articulate their social networks, and establish or maintain connections with others. (Ellision, Steinfield, and Lampe) Some SNSs, Facebook is a very good example, allow their members to join virtual groups based on common interests; to see what classes they have in common; to learn each others’ hobbies, interests, musical tastes, and romantic relationship status through the profiles (Ellision, Steinfield, and Lampe). Social network sites help people keep in touch with their friends and family members, and they connect people around the world into one site. Most people are using the networks to stay in touch with people they already know, and there are 49 percent of social network users say they use the networks to make new friends (Lenhart and Madden). Many social networks provide an online environment for people to communicate and exchange personal information for dating purposes. Intentions can vary from looking for a one time date, short-term relationships, and long-term relationships (Cade). There are 94 percent of the sample saw their 'e-partner' again after first meeting them (University Of Bath). The social network sites can be...

Words: 471 - Pages: 2

Premium Essay

Social

...Types and advantages of social networking sites Nowadays social networking sites are very important in everyday life. Everything is from modern communication through the internet which connects people together anyway. We get to know about the people, their interests, their work etc. through these sites. Social networking sites have become common among individuals of all ages. There are three main types and advantages of social network: finding friends, entertainment and follow the news. First, nowadays we use social network to connect with friends or other people. The most commonly used to connect people are: Facebook, Google plus, Skype, Twitter etc. They are the most popular social network people used. For example, facebook can easily connect with friends using wall updates, private message, poke or text video call chat. Similarly, if you not to want to video call chat by facebook you can video chat call this site is skype. Basically, social network type find friends used to chat but can be share the file of movie, music video etc. for entertainment with friends. Entertainment type of social networking is file media for entertainment in sites. The most commonly used to entertainment is Youtube. It can share the video, see the music video, see the movie etc. Moreover, Youtube can be upload from user account in web sites to share another people. Sometimes youtube can be following the news and see the series can watch in youtube. Finally, following the news...

Words: 360 - Pages: 2

Premium Essay

Abramowitz Social Work and Social

...Social Work and Social Reform: An Arena of Struggle Mimi Abramovitz The profession of social work has the potential both to meet individual needs and to engage in social change. However, the profession’s position between the individual and society often forces practitioners to choose between adjusting people and programs to circumstances or challenging the status quo. The twin pressures of containment and change have made social work an arena of struggle since its origins in the late 19th century. In honor of social work’s centennial, this article examines the sources of the profession’s prochange mandate and the structural factors that limit social work’s ability to pledge itself to this stance permanently and recommends some steps social workers can take to recommit the profession to greater activism. Special attention is given to documenting the long but largely ignored history of social work activism. Key words: activism; history; professionalism; social reform; social work T he twin pressures of containment and change have plagued social work since its origins in the late 19th century. The profession can boast of a long history of progressive activism directed to individual and social change. At the same time, observers within and outside social work have often accused the profession of serving as a handmaiden of the status quo. This contradiction has made the social work profession a site of ongoing struggle. Although often difficult, the battles...

Words: 9381 - Pages: 38

Free Essay

Social Business

...Building Social Business: An Alternative Mode of Ensuring ESC Rights. Chapter one: 1.1 .Introduction 1.2 Background of study 1.3 Concept of social Business 1.4 Concept of ESC Rights 1.5 Rational and significance of the study 1.6 Objective of this research 1.7 Hypothesis 1.8 Reasons for selecting topic 1.9 Research Methodology Chapter two: historical background: 2.1 Introduction 2.2History social Business 2.3 Origin of social Business and its kinds 2.4. Economical situation after liberation war . 2.5Our society and business and commonplace business Chapter four: 4.1 Definition of social business. 4.1 Why social Business? 4.2 Which is not social Business? 4.4 Types of social Business. 4.5 Social Business a business model 4.6 Social business model framework/ Legal framework for social Business 4.7 Features of social Business to be considered 4.8 Seven principle of social Business 4.9 Four rules of social Business 4.10 Advantages and disadvantages of social Business \Chapter five 5.1 profit and social Business 5.2 How Bangladesh can profit from social Business 5.3 Social Business in Bangladesh compared with abroad 5.3 What Social Business offers? 5.1How to build social Business/ How social Business Can be started? 5.2 Social business as a new form of capitalism 5.5 Social investment fund …social stock market. 5.3 creating a glabal insfrustracture for Social business...

Words: 685 - Pages: 3

Premium Essay

Social Skills

...Özdemir, Baykara Acar, Acar ve Duyan Araştırma SOCIAL SKILL LEVELS OF STUDENTS OF SOCIAL WORK: DO OUTSIDE ACTIVITIES MAKE A DIFFERENCE? The study tries to compare the social skill levels of 97 students in the Department of Social Work Faculty of Economic and Administrative Sciences, Hacettepe University, who participate and those who do not participate in activities outside classroom. The data obtained through the research shows that there is a meaningful relationship, statistically, between certain demographic characteristics of the students and their scores on social skill inventory sub-scales; and proving that the total social skill scores of those who participate in artistic, sports and any other social activities are considerably higher than those who do not participate in such activities. Key Words: Social work students, social skill, outside activities Sosyal Hizmet Öğrencilerinin Sosyal Beceri Düzeyleri: Ders Dışı Etkinlikler Bir Fark Yaratıyor mu? Uğur ÖzDEMİR* Yüksel BAYKARA ACAR** Hakan ACAR** Veli DUYAN*** * Dr., Hacettepe Üniversitesi İktisadi ve İdari Bilimler Fakültesi Sosyal Hizmet Bölümü ** Doç. Dr., Hacettepe Üniversitesi İktisadi ve İdari Bilimler Fakültesi Sosyal Hizmet Bölümü *** Prof. Dr., Ankara Üniversitesi, Sağlık Bilimleri Fakültesi, Sosyal Hizmet Bölümü ÖZET Bu çalışmanın amacı ders dışı etkinliklere katılımın sosyal beceri düzeyi üzerinde bir etkisi olup olmadığının anlaşılmasıdır. Araştırmada, Türkiye’de Hacettepe Üniversitesi...

Words: 3162 - Pages: 13

Free Essay

Social Justice

...Social and economic justice occurs when "all members of a society share equally in the social order, secure an equitable consideration of resources and opportunities, and enjoy their full benefit of civil liberties". An easier way to understand the concept of social and economic justice may be to consider what a lack of justice is: "when societies do not accord citizens equity and equality and when they violate their citizens' human and civil rights". Social injustice could include any social problem-domestic violence, inadequate healthcare, or substance abuse. Social justice ideally addresses these problems, but also addresses social issues-the increasing aging population, education expenses, and environmental concerns. Social workers' responsibility regarding social and economic justice is formally defined in the NASW Code of Ethics. The last section of the Code of Ethics, "the social worker's ethical responsibility to society," states that "the social worker should promote the general welfare of society." More specifically, it states "the social worker should advocate changes in policy and legislation to improve social conditions and to promote social justice." It is clear then that social workers are obligated to promote social justice - to empower people individually and collectively. This stipulation is congruent with my own personal beliefs about social justice. Influenced by my values based in Christianity and humanism, I feel action to promote social justice is a personal...

Words: 263 - Pages: 2

Free Essay

Social Media

...Social Media Instagram, Facebook, Twitter, these and many more are examples of social media sites. These sites have been an aid to society and also a thorn in its side. Social media Social media creates new social connections and promote athletes. Social media is a good way to meet new people and to reconnect with old friends and or family. A study showed that 70% of adults found some of their family members online (pros and cons of social media). Also 57% of teenagers have made new friends on some type of social media network. Social media has also blown up the careers of some sports stars. Jeremy Lin was a big sensation back in 2012, everywhere you went we would seem to hear his name. Social media affected his life after one good game he had. It was because of social media that everyone heard of him, because he was a nobody before that. Another example of social media blowing up sport stars is, in 2012 Notre Dame all-star linebacker mante teo and his “girlfriend” death. That had met though some social media site and never met, they dated for about two years and found out she died of cancer (Clemmitt, 83). Later it was reported that she was never real and it was just a big hoax (Burke and Dickey.). This could also be related as a thing that will be talking about later. Many people believe that social media has gotten rid of face to face communication. The way that technology is now, it has change the way we talk to people, and engages with others. Some believe that something...

Words: 586 - Pages: 3

Premium Essay

Social Entrepreneurship

...What is social entrepreneurship? The animation is made to explain the concept of social entrepreneurship to the general public and raise awareness of the importance of this type of business.... Social entrepreneurship is the attempt to draw upon business techniques to find solutions to social problems.This concept may be applied to a variety of organizations with different sizes, aims, and beliefs. Conventional entrepreneurs typically measure performance in profit and return, but social entrepreneurs also take into account a positive return to society. Social entrepreneurship typically attempts to further broad social, cultural, and environmental goals is often associated with the voluntary sector. At times, profit also may be a consideration for certain companies or other social enterprises. There are continuing arguments over precisely who counts as a social entrepreneur. Thus far, there has been no consensus on the definition of social entrepreneurship, so many different sorts of fields and disciplines are associated with social entrepreneurship. Philanthropists, social activists, environmentalists, and other socially oriented practitioners are referred to as social entrepreneurs. For a clearer definition of what social entrepreneurship entails, it is necessary to set the function of social entrepreneurship apart from other socially oriented activities and identify the boundaries within which social entrepreneurs operate.[4] Some have advocated restricting the term...

Words: 2381 - Pages: 10

Premium Essay

Social Media

...Social media marketing refers to the process of gaining website traffic or attention through social media sites. Social media is a platform that is easily accessible to anyone with internet access. Increased communication for organizations fosters brand awareness and often, improved customer service. Additionally, social media serves as a relatively inexpensive platform for organizations to implement marketing campaigns. Social media marketing (SMM) is a form of Internet marketing that utilizes social networking websites as a marketing tool. The goal of SMM is to produce content that users will share with their social network to help a company increase brand exposure and broaden customer reach.  Is social media marketing right for your business? * This is the question few companies ask themselves before launching into social media marketing. And, as a result, their forays into social media marketing often result in frustration and, sometimes, failure. Not because social media marketing doesn’t work – just because they didn’t do it correctly. Social media marketing is like any other campaign, it must have an objective and a strategy to achieve desired results. * There are many companies that think it's the quick fix to a struggling business or that it will generate revenue that will help them in a difficult economic time. Truth is that social media can, in fact, affect your bottom line – but, it takes time. * Social media has a sequence of events that transpire...

Words: 3364 - Pages: 14

Premium Essay

Social Change

...relationship between religion and social change (33 marks) In relation to social change, some theories have argued that religion is a conservative force and acts to prevent change and keep society stable. This is the view held by earlier theories that stressed the role of institutions in shaping human behaviour and maintaining the stability of society, such as Functionalism and Marxism. Religion is also seen as conservative because it is traditional; defending traditional customs and moral views, for example. Functionalists view religion as a conservative force, preventing social change. Durkheim believed that religion performs an important function for society, binding people together like ‘social cement’ preventing anomie. Religion provides a set of moral values that form the collective conscience, ensuring social stability. This is where religion unifies people which leads to conformity which then makes behaviour predictable. Religion also answers eternal questions such as ‘why the good die young’ and ‘why do people suffer’ in Christian for example, helping to prevent social change. Durkheim studied totemism among Australian Aboriginal tribes. Totems are a symbol of a set of beliefs. This can be anything from an object to an animal and is treated with the highest respect by those that follow the set of beliefs or religion. For example, it is similar to the crucifix for Christians which explains why they uphold traditional beliefs in society, preventing social change. However, Durkheim’s...

Words: 1488 - Pages: 6

Free Essay

Social Advertising

...Social Advertising Social media exists in the context of communities that are built of people who develop and nurture relationships by creating, co-creating, sharing, commenting, and engaging in content (Tuten, 2008). Advertising on such social platform is what accounts to social advertisement. Most of these communities are made up by our sponsors, believers, assistants, coaches, cheerleaders, and friends. Advertising has always been a means of mass communication with a great persuasive force. Thus with the current up rise in social networking in the current world, many advertisers are now turning their heads towards social media and networking. There are various social benefits from advertising that are explained as follows (Kaptan, 2003). * Increased social awareness: Social advertising has created much awareness for certain problems faced in the society today such as virtual eco-rallies and such protests instead of blocking down the streets and such. * Faster informational reviews on newer products: The information reviews and critics have reached the consumers faster than the products itself, along with their availability in the market, creating decision options for consumers. * Advertising is Entertaining: Social advertising is also considered as entertainment for some but its goal is not to entertain but actually to move communities through the hierarchy of response so they purchase, subscribe and become loyal to the brand. Along with these benefits, social...

Words: 840 - Pages: 4