Free Essay

The Impact of School-Based Mentoring on Youths with Different Relational Profiles

In:

Submitted By sbrinkley1
Words 12031
Pages 49
Developmental Psychology 2011, Vol. 47, No. 2, 450 – 462

© 2010 American Psychological Association 0012-1649/10/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/a0021379

The Impact of School-Based Mentoring on Youths With Different Relational Profiles
Sarah E. O. Schwartz, Jean E. Rhodes, and Christian S. Chan
University of Massachusetts Boston

Carla Herrera
Public/Private Ventures, Philadelphia, Pennsylvania

Associations between youths’ relationship profiles and mentoring outcomes were explored in the context of a national, randomized study of 1,139 youths (54% female) in geographically diverse Big Brothers Big Sisters school-based mentoring programs. The sample included youths in Grades 4 –9 from diverse racial and ethnic backgrounds, the majority of whom were receiving free or reduced-price lunch. Latent profile analysis, a person-oriented approach, was used to identify 3 distinct relational profiles. Mentoring was found to have differential effects depending on youths’ preintervention approach to relationships. In particular, youths who, at baseline, had satisfactory, but not particularly strong, relationships benefited more from mentoring than did youths with profiles characterized by either strongly positive or negative relationships. Implications for research and practice are discussed. Keywords: youth mentoring, parent relationships, teacher relationships, latent profile analysis

Youth mentoring programs such as Big Brothers Big Sisters (BBBS) pair youths with volunteers who are trained to provide support and guidance. Such programs have experienced tremendous growth in the past 2 decades. Millions of volunteer mentors are involved in youths’ lives, and the numbers are continuing to rise (Mentor/National Mentoring Partnership, 2006). Anecdotal reports of mentors’ protective qualities are corroborated by a growing body of research, providing support for the positive contributions nonparental adults can make in the lives of youths (Rhodes & Lowe, 2008). At the same time, research on the effectiveness of mentoring programs has revealed considerable room for improvement in both the strength and consistency of program impacts (DuBois, Holloway, Valentine, & Cooper, 2002; Eby, Allen, Evans, Ng, & DuBois, 2008). In their meta-analysis of 55 evaluations of youth mentoring programs, DuBois, Holloway, et al. (2002) found evidence of only small benefits, on average, for participating youths on measures of emotional, behavioral, and educational functioning. Importantly, however, effect-size estimates increased systematically in conjunction with individual-, match-, and program-related factors (DuBois, Holloway, et al., 2002). Several investigations have also highlighted a range of factors associated with better outcomes, including match length

This article was published Online First December 20, 2010. Sarah E. O. Schwartz, Jean E. Rhodes, and Christian S. Chan, Department of Psychology, University of Massachusetts Boston; Carla Herrera, Public/Private Ventures, Philadelphia, Pennsylvania. The study was funded by a grant from the Edna McConnell Clark Foundation. We thank Keoki Hansen, Public/Private Ventures, Big Brothers Big Sisters of America, the William T. Grant Foundation, and Atlantic Philanthropies for their support of the study. Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Jean E. Rhodes, Department of Psychology, University of Massachusetts, Boston, MA 02125. E-mail: jean.rhodes@umb.edu 450

(Grossman & Rhodes, 2002), consistency (DuBois, Neville, Parra, & Pugh-Lilly, 2002), and closeness (Spencer, 2006; Thomson & Zand, 2010). Although empirical support for youth mentoring interventions remains uneven, a handful of rigorous evaluations and studies have provided an initial base from which to launch and improve new mentoring initiatives (see Rhodes & DuBois, 2006). Within this context, school-based mentoring (SBM), in which mentors and mentees meet on school grounds, generally during school hours, has been the fastest growing approach, accounting for nearly half of all youth-mentoring programs (DuBois & Karcher, 2005). In SBM programs, mentors and mentees engage in activities together, including talking, playing games, and academic-related tasks such as homework or reading. SBM’s strong appeal stems from its ability to serve youths who may not be reached by other forms of mentoring and its potential to connect a broad array of community members with youths’ daily academic and social experiences in the school setting, potentially improving youths’ experience in and outlook on school. Participation in SBM has been associated with positive outcomes, including improvements in academic, behavioral, and psychosocial adjustment (Cavell & Hughes, 2000; Karcher, 2008; Karcher, Davis, & Powell, 2002; Matzenbacher, 1999; Portwood & Ayers, 2005). Although promising, these findings are mostly based on non- or quasi-experimental evidence. Findings from two recent random assignment impact evaluations of SBM showed few statistically significant impacts on academic, psychosocial, or behavioral outcomes (Bernstein, Rappaport, Olsho, Hunt, & Levin, 2009; Herrera, Grossman, Kauh, Feldman, & McMaken, 2007). Secondary analyses of these data, however, have hinted at variability in effects among different subgroups of youths (Bernstein et al., 2009; Herrera et al., 2007; Herrera, Grossman, Kauh, & McMaken, in press). Such findings underscore the importance of identifying factors that account for variation in the effectiveness of SBM. The present study draws on secondary data analyses from the national evalu-

MENTORING AND YOUTH RELATIONAL PROFILES

451

ation of BBBS SBM programs (Herrera et al., 2007) to examine whether the quality of youths’ preintervention relationships with their parents, teachers, and peers was associated with the benefits they later derived from SBM.

Background
Not all youths are equally suited for mentoring. Although some advocates might argue that every youth would benefit from the compassionate attention of a volunteer adult, most concede that mentoring is neither a substitute for professional treatment for youths with serious emotional, behavioral, or academic problems nor a necessary inoculation for all youths (Rhodes & DuBois, 2006). In addition to youths’ baseline functioning, their relationship histories and access to additional sources of support are likely to affect the degree to which they can benefit from mentoring. For example, youths who enter mentoring programs with strong connections to their parents, teachers, and coaches may have sufficient adult support and hence less of a need for volunteer services (Grossman & Johnson, 1999). Additionally, youths who have had unsatisfying relationships may be less inclined to trust the overtures of caring adults (Kobak & Sceery, 1988; Romero-Canyas, Downey, Berenson, Ayduk, & Kang, 2010). Larose, Bernier, and Soucy (2005), for example, found that young adults with insecure parental attachments felt relatively less security in their relationships with mentors. Furthermore, youths with deeply rooted relational difficulties, such as aggressive and antisocial behaviors, which tend to be more resistant to change (see e.g., Connor, 2004; Vaughn & Howard, 2004), may need more comprehensive interventions than volunteer mentors can provide. In fact, research demonstrates that mentoring programs tend to be less effective for youths who show moderate to severe individual risk factors, such as academic or behavioral difficulties (DuBois, Holloway, et al., 2002; Jolliffe & Farrington, 2007). As suggested earlier, such variability in benefits may stem, in part, from youths’ relational histories. Because mentoring is essentially a relationship-based intervention, it can ignite vulnerabilities and elicit behavioral patterns that were established in earlier bonds. In particular, children are thought to draw on their early experiences with caregivers and others to develop experiencebased expectations, or working models. These models, in turn, influence behavior in interpersonal relationships throughout and beyond childhood (Ainsworth, 1989; Bowlby, 1988). There is considerable evidence for continuity in relationship styles across individuals’ various relationships. Specifically, parental attachment has been shown to predict the quality of children’s subsequent relationships with teachers, outside caregivers, and peers (Allen, Porter, McFarland, McElhaney, & Marsh, 2007; Berlin & Cassidy, 1999; Carlson, Sroufe, & Egeland, 2004; Lynch & Cicchetti, 1992; Rydell, Bohlin, & Thorell, 2005). There is thus good reason to hypothesize that youths’ relationship experiences might affect their approaches to mentoring. Some youths entering mentoring programs have had a history of deeply supportive relationships, whereas others have experienced relationships characterized by negativity or even abuse or neglect. These differences are likely to have implications for mentoring relationship quality, length, and outcomes. Grossman and Rhodes (2002), for example, found that youths who had sustained emotional, sexual, or physical abuse had less enduring volunteer men-

tor relationships. Such youths may hold negative relational expectations and biases, leading them to interpret ambiguous gestures (e.g., canceled or late appointments) more negatively (Downey, Irwin, Ramsay, & Ayduk, 2004; Downey, Lebolt, Rincon, & ´ Freitas, 1998) and respond less positively to mentors’ overtures of support (Rhodes, Grossman, & Resch, 2000). These more negative reactions, in turn, may reduce mentors’ enthusiasm and persistence, undermining both the quality and longevity of the mentoring relationship. By contrast, youths with more positive relationship histories are less inclined to perceive ambiguously intentioned negative behavior in others and better equipped to respond to mentors (Romero-Canyas et al., 2010). Depending on the extant social resources of such youths, however, their relationships may take different courses. Youths with an abundance of adult and peer support may fail to invest fully in the relationship, leaving their mentors feeling unnecessary, whereas youths in need of guidance and support may engage with their mentors in ways that enrich the quality and longevity of the match. Youths’ proclivity for forming connections to nonparent adults may also vary as a function of developmental status. In particular, relative to older adolescents, younger adolescents have reported better friendships and more disclosure with adults than have older adolescents (Thomson & Zand, 2010) and tend to have more enduring ties with mentors (Grossman & Rhodes, 2002). It may be the case that older adolescents’ normative desires for autonomy and independence result in less compliance and emotional accessibility in relations with mentors (Allen & Land, 1999). Similarly, peer and romantic relationships may increasingly compete for adolescents’ attention and commitment (Seiffge-Krenke, Shulman, & Klessinger, 2001), rendering their mentoring relationships less intensive. These developmental differences may, in turn, moderate the relationship processes described earlier.

Current Study
In this study, we drew on the national evaluation of BBBS (Herrera et al., 2007) to explore the possibility that the quality of youths’ preintervention relationships would be associated with their proclivity to forge and benefit from volunteer mentoring relationships. Taking developmental status into account, we sought to determine whether weaker effects would be observed for youths who, at baseline, were less successful in their relationships with parents, teachers, and peers. Likewise, we examined whether effects were less pronounced among youths who, at baseline, already had an abundance of such ties. Specifically, the analyses were designed to test (a) whether youths could be grouped in meaningful ways as a function of their baseline relationships with parents, teachers, and peers and, if so, (b) whether the baseline relational profiles of such youths moderated the effects of mentoring on youth outcomes, as well as whether they were associated with match duration and relationship quality.

Method Participants
Youths in this random assignment impact evaluation were recruited from 10 BBBS agencies across the country (serving a total of 71 participating schools), all of which had been operating SBM

452

SCHWARTZ, RHODES, CHAN, AND HERRERA

programs for at least 4 years, served at least 150 youths, recruited at least two different types of volunteers (e.g., high school students and professionals), and had strong leadership in place (Herrera et al., 2007). All youths who met the following criteria were invited to participate in the study: (a) were in fourth though ninth grades at the start of the study, (b) had parental consent to participate, and (c) had not been referred because of a crisis (e.g., referred by Child Protective Services). Of the total 1,139 youths participating in the study, 54% were female, and the majority of participants were White (37%), Hispanic/Latino (23%), or Black/African American (18%; see Table 1). Although data were not collected on language spoken in the home, 10 percent were identified by teachers as having limited proficiency in English. Sixty percent of participants were in elementary school (fourth or fifth grade), 34% were in middle school (sixth through eighth grade), and 6% were in ninth grade. Sixty-nine percent of youths were receiving free or reducedprice lunch, and 39% lived in a single-parent home. On the basis of teacher reports, 51% of youths were performing below grade level and/or needed improvement in their overall academic performance. Of the 554 mentors who completed baseline surveys, nearly half (48%) were high school students, 18% were college students, and the remaining volunteers were nonstudent adults. BBBS programs had recruited mentors from businesses and schools. Seventy-two

percent were female, and 77% were White. Seventy-one percent of mentors reported receiving training from BBBS, typically covering program rules, match expectations, and how to build a strong relationship with their mentees (Herrera et al., 2007).

Procedure and Intervention
Youths were recruited to participate in the study through BBBS agencies and participating schools. Youth participants completed baseline surveys at their school—surveys administered by on-site researchers in small group settings. All of the 1,139 youth participants completed baseline surveys. Teachers were also given surveys to complete individually. For the majority of youths in middle and high school settings, youths’ science, social studies, English as a second language, or homeroom teacher completed the survey. Teachers of 1,009 youths (of the 1,139) completed baseline surveys. After youths completed the baseline survey, they were randomly assigned to either the treatment group to be assigned to a mentor (n 565) or the control group to be placed on a waiting list (n 574). Follow-up surveys were administered at two subsequent time points: the spring of the first school year (1,067 youths and 959 teachers completed surveys) and the fall of the second school year (968 youths and 920 teachers completed surveys). A survey firm administered follow-up surveys at youths’ schools or by phone if youths had moved or were absent from school. Surveys were available in both Spanish and English. Although mentors committed to meeting with youths for one school year, matches generally began after the start of the school year to allow for volunteer recruitment, screening, and training as well as school scheduling, and some matches terminated prematurely during the first year. In fact, at the time of the first follow-up survey, only 64% of youths in the treatment group were still meeting with the mentor with whom they were originally matched. As a result, youths had received an average of 4.9 months of mentoring by the time of the first follow-up in the spring of the first school year, meeting an average of 3.1 times per month. By the second follow-up survey (during the fall of the second school year), 48% of youths were no longer meeting with a mentor, in many cases because youths had transferred to a new school. Of those youths who were still matched, 41% were meeting with the same mentor they had met with during the previous year, and 11% were meeting with a new mentor. Intent-to-treat analyses were conducted in order to maintain randomization, regardless of whether youths were still meeting with their mentors at the time of the follow-up assessments (Herrera et al., 2007).

Table 1 Demographic Characteristics of Youth Participants (N
Characteristics Gender Male Female Grade level 4 5 6 7 8 9 Race/ethnicitya White Hispanic/Latino Black/African American Native American Asian/Pacific Islander Multiracial Other Economic statusa Free/reduced lunch Not free/reduced lunch Missing data Geographic location Columbus, Ohio Denver, Colorado Ellsworth, Maine St. Louis, Missouri Cleveland, Ohio Oak Harbor, Washington Dallas, Texas Show Low, Arizona Dalton, Georgia Wilkes-Barre, Pennsylvania a 1,139)
Percentage 46 54 36 24 24 9 1 6 37 23 18 6 1 13 3 60 27 14 10 6 4 15 9 6 15 14 16 5

Number 522 617 406 279 275 106 6 67 426 259 202 67 11 142 32 679 304 156 114 69 45 172 100 69 168 154 186 62

Measures
The study’s surveys included questions about youths’ demographic characteristics; measures of parent, teacher, and peer relationship quality; mentoring relationship quality; mentoring relationship duration; and youth outcomes. Consistent with the original evaluation (Herrera et al., 2007), outcome measures fell into three broad categories: school-related performance and attitudes, problem behaviors, and social and personal well-being. Previous studies of SBM have revealed relatively stronger effects on school-related outcomes than on non-school-related outcomes (Bernstein et al., 2009; Herrera et al., 2007). Thus, the majority of

Percentages total more than 100% due to rounding.

MENTORING AND YOUTH RELATIONAL PROFILES

453

the outcomes included in this study were school-related. Moreover, whereas previous reports with these data have treated changes in parent, teacher, and peer relationships over time as dependent variables (Herrera et al., 2007; Herrera et al., in press), on the basis of our hypothesis that relationship history may influence the degree to which youths benefit from mentoring, baseline measures of these variables were instead used to create youths’ relational profiles. In recognition of the significant role mentor relationship quality and match duration play as intermediate outcomes in mentoring (see e.g., DuBois, Holloway, et al., 2002; Grossman & Rhodes, 2002; Rhodes & DuBois, 2008), these variables were also included as dependent variables in the current study. Covariates were included on the basis of patterns of significant group differences between profiles. We did not include data from the second school year, because nearly half of the matches (48%) terminated at the end of the first school year. Therefore, baseline data at Time 1 (T1) and first follow-up data at Time 2 (T2) were used in the current analyses. Zero-order correlations for all variables included in the analyses are presented in Table 2. Baseline relationship quality (T1 only). Parent– child relationship quality. This was assessed with a 16-item youth-reported scale combining the Parent Trust subscale and the Parent Communication subscale of the Inventory of Parent and Peer Attachment (IPPA; Armsden & Greenberg, 1987). The IPPA has been used with youths 12–20 years of age and has demonstrated good psychometric properties, including concurrent validity, reliability, and internal consistency (Armsden & Greenberg, 1987). Respondents indicated the level of support they felt in their relationship with their parent or guardian (e.g., “My parents accept me as I am,” “My parents trust my judgment”). Responses were coded on a 4-point Likert-type scale ranging from 1 (hardly ever) to 4 ( pretty often), and the mean was calculated to form a parent attachment score ( 1 .89). A higher score indicates a more positive parent– child relationship.

This was measured Teacher–student relationship quality. with an 11-item youth-reported scale adapted from a teacher– student relationship scale (Eccles et al., 1993) and a teacher connectedness scale (Karcher, 2003). The scales were combined because they measured similar constructs and, when combined, retained relatively high reliability. The scale includes items such as “I get along well with my teachers this year” and “I care what my teachers think of me.” The items were scored on a 4-point Likerttype scale ranging from 1 (not at all true) to 4 (very true), and a mean score was calculated, with a higher score indicating a more positive teacher–student relationship ( 1 .82). Peer acceptance. This was measured with a six-item youthreported subscale of the Self-Perception Profile for Children (SPPC; Harter, 1985) containing statements assessing how accepted youths feel by their peers (e.g., difficulty making friends, popularity). The SPPC has demonstrated reliability, validity, and stability among cross-cultural samples of youths in elementary and middle school (Schumann et al., 1999), although some evidence has suggested it may be less reliable among African American girls (Winters, Myers, & Proud, 2002). The original version of the instrument was adapted by using a 4-point Likert-type scale ranging from 1 (not at all true) to 4 (very true). Mean scores were calculated, with a higher score indicating a greater level of peer acceptance ( 1 .69). Mentor–youth relationship quality (T2 only). Youth emotional engagement. This was measured with an eight-item youth-reported scale including items such as “When I’m with my mentor, I feel excited” (Rhodes, Reddy, Roffman, & Grossman, 2005). The items were scored on a 4-point Likert-type scale ranging from 1 (not at all true) to 4 (very true), and a mean score was calculated, with higher scores indicating higher levels of emotional engagement ( 2 .84). Youth unhappiness. This was measured with a six-item youth-reported scale that includes items such as “When I am

Table 2 Zero-Order Correlations for Baseline Relationship Variables, Outcomes, and Covariates
Variable 1. 2. 3. 4. 5. 6. 7. 8. 9. 10. 11. 12. 13. 14. 15. 16. 17. PR TR SA AP PB CA CE SPAA GSW UA UH (T2) EE (T2) Gender MS Grade School ML (T2) 1 — .39 .20 .13 .09 .10 .14 .32 .40 .03 .10 .01 .04 .02 .20 .03 .00 2 — .00 .17 .20 .12 .23 .26 .26 .09 .13 .11 .14 .10 .28 .02 .06 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16

— .10 .01 .14 .03 .33 .45 .01 .04 .06 .04 .08 .11 .04 .01

— .32 .31 .56 .29 .18 .13 .06 .03 .15 .01 .01 .11 .03

— .55 .60 .05 .05 .12 .10 .06 .28 .05 .10 .12 .00

— .50 .09 .12 .09 .04 .03 .13 .11 .02 .08 .05

— .23 .12 .14 .03 .01 .29 .07 .04 .08 .03

— .54 .03 .10 .05 .00 .11 .09 .10 .05

— .05 .12 .12 .04 .07 .05 .10 .021

— .03 .05 .02 .03 .09 .08 .01

— .71 .08 .05 .01 .05 .00

— .07 .02 .13 .02 .13

— .02 — .01 .14 .03 .11 .00 .01

— .26 .10

— .07

Note. PR parent relationship; TR teacher relationship; SA social acceptance; AP academic performance; PB classroom affect; CE classroom effort; SPAA self-perceptions of academic abilities; GSW global self-worth; UA unhappiness; T2 Time 2; EE emotional engagement; MS minority status; ML match length. p .05. p .01.

prosocial behavior; CA unexcused absences; UH

454

SCHWARTZ, RHODES, CHAN, AND HERRERA

with my mentor, I feel disappointed” (Rhodes, Reddy, et al., 2005). The items were scored on a 4-point Likert-type scale ranging from 1 (not at all true) to 4 (very true), and a mean score was calculated, with higher scores indicating greater levels of unhappiness ( 2 .68). Match duration (T2 only). Match duration. This was a single-item variable referring to the total number of days that youths had been in an open match as measured at T2. Outcome variables (T1 and T2). Overall academic performance. This was determined on the basis of teachers’ ratings of youths’ academic performance on a single-item 5-point scale ranging from 1 (below grade level) to 5 (excellent; Pierce, Hamm, & Vandell, 1999). Unexcused absences. These were measured with a singleitem measure in which teachers reported the number of times in the previous 4 weeks that youths had been absent from school without an excuse. Classroom effort. This was measured with a six-item subscale of the Research Assessment Package for Schools-Teachers, a tool that has been validated in urban and suburban schools among youths from diverse racial and ethnic backgrounds (Institute for Research and Reform in Education, 1998). Teachers rated how often students demonstrated effort in the classroom (e.g., “Works hard in class,” “Does more than is required of him/her”) on a 4-point Likert-type scale ranging from 1 (never) to 4 (very often). Means were calculated, with a higher score indicating a higher level of effort ( 1 .90, 2 .90). Classroom affect. This was measured with a three-item scale based on teacher reports of whether youths appear happy, angry, or depressed in the classroom (e.g., “In my class, this child appears happy”; Herrera, 2004). Teachers rated youths on a 4-point Likerttype scale ranging from 1 (never) to 4 (very often). The measure was calculated as the mean of the three items, with higher scores reflecting more positive classroom affect ( 1 .77, 2 .77). Prosocial behavior. This was measured with an eight-item subscale from the Child Behavior Scale (Ladd & Profilet, 1996) in which teachers rated how frequently youths demonstrate prosocial behavior toward their peers (e.g., “Offers help or comfort when classmates are upset”) using a 4-point Likert-type scale where 1 (never) and 4 (very often). The scale has been demonstrated to yield reliable and valid information among samples of youths in elementary and middle school from diverse racial and socioeconomic backgrounds (Ladd, Herald-Brown, Andrews, 2009; Ladd & Profilet, 1996). The measure was calculated as a mean of the eight items, with higher scores reflecting more prosocial behavior ( 1 .92, 2 .94). Self-perceptions of academic abilities. This was measured with a six-item youth-reported subscale of the SPPC (Harter, 1985). The items assessed youths’ estimation of their own academic competence (e.g., self-assessments and social comparisons of intelligence and class work). The original version of the instrument was adapted using a 4-point Likert-type scale ranging from 1 (not at all true) to 4 (very true). The measure was calculated as a mean of the six items, with higher scores reflecting higher levels of self-perceived academic abilities ( 1 .70, 2 .72). Global self-worth. This was measured with an eight-item youth-reported subscale of the Self-Esteem Questionnaire (DuBois, Felner, Brand, Phillips, & Lease, 1996) that measures the

level of youths’ self-worth through items such as “I am happy with the way I can do most things” and “I am the kind of person I want to be.” The scale has demonstrated reliability and validity among youths of diverse racial backgrounds in Grades 5– 8 (DuBois et al., 1996). Respondents rated items on a 4-point Likert-type scale ranging from 1 (not at all true) to 4 (very true). Higher mean scores reflect more positive self-evaluations ( 1 .76, 2 .80). Covariates (T1 only). Substance use. This was measured with four youth-reported items adapted from the Self-Reported Behavior Index (Brown, Clasen, & Eicher, 1986). Youths reported whether they had ever used alcohol, tobacco, marijuana, or other drugs and, if so, how frequently during the past 3 months. The response scale and the reference period were modified for the current study (the original measure asks for a report of use in the past month). The items were combined to form a dichotomous variable where 1 indicates any previous substance use and 0 indicates no reported history of substance use. Demographic characteristics. This included youths’ grade in school, gender, and minority status at baseline. These variables were selected on the basis of patterns of significant differences between profiles.

Statistical Methods
To categorize youths on the basis of their preexisting relationships, latent profile analysis (LPA), a person-centered approach, was employed. A person-centered approach is appropriate for identifying patterns within individuals’ responses on the basis of its capacity to consider multiple characteristics simultaneously (Bergman & Trost, 2006; Magnusson, 1998; O’Brien, 2005). LPA is a model-based procedure that reveals categorical latent variables from observed continuous variables and generates probabilities for group membership. This approach provides a statistically rigorous method of detecting patterns of associations among youth relationships across contexts. Furthermore, this approach allows for the examination of relatively homogenous subgroups of youths based on variables of interest, in this case, their relationship networks. LPA was conducted using the mixture model in Mplus, Version 5 (Muthen & Muthen, 2007), on youth-reported measures of baseline ´ ´ parent, teacher, and peer relationships. This allowed for the identification of distinct profiles of youths with similar patterns of responses in reporting on their relationships with parents, teachers, and peers. Models were tested with a single profile, two profiles, and three profiles. Because it is not appropriate to test models with a greater number of profiles than the number of variables used to create those profiles, all possible models were tested, varying model restraints (e.g., correlating variables within the model). Fit indices that included the Bayesian information criteria (BIC) and sample-size-adjusted BIC, entropy statistics, and average probabilities for most likely latent variable memberships were compared. Considering the possible differences in youths’ relationships with adults on the basis of their age, it was important to test the measurement equivalence across different age groups after establishing a latent profile model. To this end, an unconstrained, semiconstrained, and fully constrained multigroup latent profile analysis (MLPA) was conducted with two age groups—Grade 4 and 5 (60% of the sample) versus Grades 6 –9 (40% of the sample)—as the grouping variable. This procedure tests whether the two age groups’ profile structures can be considered to be the

MENTORING AND YOUTH RELATIONAL PROFILES

455

same. Although the number of profiles is kept the same in these models, in the unconstrained model both the size of profiles (the percentage of youths in each profile) and the means and variances of baseline parent, teacher, and peer relationships in each profile are free to vary. On the other hand, in the semiconstrained model, the baseline parent, teacher, and peer relationships in each profile are constrained to be equal across the two age groups, and the profile size is free to vary. If the semiconstrained model fits the data as well as does the unconstrained model, then the structure of the model can be concluded to be the same across age groups. Finally, in the fully constrained model, both the profile size and baseline parent, teacher, and peer relationships are constrained to be equal across both age groups. If the fully constrained model fits the data as well as does the semiconstrained model, then the profile size can be concluded to be equivalent across age groups. We used the BIC and sample-size-adjusted BIC to compare the fit of these different LPA models (see Geiser, Lehmann, & Eid, 2006, for a detailed example of a multigroup mixture model). After grouping youths by the categorical latent variable revealed from the LPA (relational profile), a two-level random-intercept regression model was used to examine whether relational profile moderated the impact of mentoring, while accounting for clustering by school. In addition, analyses of variance were used to examine associations between profiles, match duration, and relationship quality. Finally, race, gender, and age group were also investigated as potential moderators of the impact of mentoring, again using two-level random-intercept regression models.

Figure 1. Standardized means for baseline relationships for the threeprofile latent profile analysis solution.

Results Profile Results
A three-profile model had the best fit to the data, with a BIC of 8,951.10, a sample–size-adjusted BIC of 8,884.39, and an entropy score of .63 (see Table 3). The average probabilities for most likely latent variable membership were high (.90 for Profile 1, .80 for Profile 2, and .80 for Profile 3), indicating a high level of certainty in determining membership in a given profile. Profile 1 (relationally vulnerable) was characterized by youth reports of low-quality parent relationships, low-quality teacher relationships, and average levels of peer acceptance (N 388, 34% of total). Profile 2 (relationally adequate) was characterized by average parent relationship quality, average teacher relationship quality, and average peer acceptance (N 516, 45% of total). Profile 3 (relationally strong) was characterized by high-quality parent relationships, high-quality teacher relationships, and relatively high levels of peer acceptance (N 232, 20% of total; see Figure 1 and Table 4). Notably, there were significant differences between all profiles on

all three relationship variables, with the exception of peer acceptance. For this variable, both relationally adequate and relationally vulnerable youths were significantly lower than relationally strong youths, but they did not differ significantly from each other. The results of the MLPA showed that, regarding the BIC and sample-size-adjusted BIC, respectively, the semiconstrained model had the smallest (6,929.66 and 6,847.08), compared with the unconstrained (7,022.12 and 6,872.83) and fully constrained (6,988.15 and 6,911.92) models. This indicates that although the size of profiles differed across age groups, the structure of the profiles was consistent across the two age groups. On the basis of these results, the same profile model was used for the full sample in the remaining analyses.

Baseline Differences Between Profiles
Results of baseline group differences between youths in each profile are presented in Table 5. As indicated in the MLPA, there were significant differences in school grade level among youths in the three profiles, F(2, 1136) 32.1, p .001, with relationally strong youths having a mean grade of 4.79 (SD 1.09), relationally adequate youths having a mean grade of 5.30 (SD 1.30), and relationally vulnerable youths having a mean grade of 5.67 (SD 1.48). Minority youths were also slightly more likely to be in the relationally vulnerable or the relationally adequate profile than in the relationally strong profile, 2(2, N 1,136) 9.27, p .01,

Table 3 Fit Indices for One-, Two-, and Three-Profile Latent Profile Analysis Solutions
Variable BIC Sample-size-adjusted BIC Entropy Note. BIC One-profile solution 9,672.36 9,650.12 Two-profile solution 9,058.77 9,014.30 .63 Three-profile solution 8,951.10 8,884.39 .63

Bayesian information criteria.

456

SCHWARTZ, RHODES, CHAN, AND HERRERA

Table 4 Unstandardized Means (and Standard Errors) for Youths’ Baseline Relationships for the Three-Profile Latent Profile Analysis Solution
Relationship Parent Teacher Peers Profile 1: RV (n 2.77a (0.03) 2.91a (0.03) 2.55a (0.04) 388) Profile 2: RA (n 3.34b (0.02) 3.43b (0.01) 2.58a (0.03) 516) Profile 3: RS (n 3.69c (0.02) 3.80c (0.02) 2.74b (0.05) 232) Profile differences: F(3, 1136) 335.33 338.58 5.39 relationally vulnerable; RA relationally

Note. Means with different subscripts differ significantly by profile on the basis of Scheffe post hoc tests. RV adequate; RS relationally strong. p .01. p .001.

and there was a trend for girls to be in the relationally strong or relationally adequate profile rather than the relationally vulnerable profile, 2(2, N 1,136) 4.94, p .08. Baseline differences between groups for outcome variables were also examined (see Table 6). Relationally vulnerable youths demonstrated significantly lower academic performance, prosocial behavior, classroom effort ( p .01), and self-perceptions of academic abilities and global self-worth ( p .001) than did relationally adequate or relationally strong youths at baseline, as well as marginally lower classroom affect than did relationally strong youths. Notably, however, relationally adequate and relationally strong youths did not differ significantly from each other on most outcome variables, showing significant differences in only global self-worth and self-perceptions of academic abilities.

Mentoring Impacts by Profile
A two-level, random-intercept regression model was conducted using Stata (Release 11.0) to examine the impact of mentoring on each of the three profiles of youths, while accounting for clustering by school. On the basis of the pattern of significant group differences between profiles, we included grade in school, gender, and minority status as covariates, along with the baseline level of the outcome variable being tested. Youths in the control group reported slightly higher levels of substance use at baseline (15% vs. 11%), 2(2, N 1,133) 4.52, p .05; thus this variable was also included as a covariate in the models. Relationally adequate youths who were in the treatment group showed significantly higher levels of academic performance ( p .01) and prosocial behavior ( p .05) and marginally significantly higher levels of classroom effort ( p .07) and self-perceptions of academic abilities ( p .07) than did relationally adequate youths who were in the control group (see Table 7). In contrast, relation-

ally strong youths did not show significant differences between the treatment and control groups on any of the outcome variables, and relationally vulnerable youths showed only marginally significant improvement ( p .09) on one outcome variable, specifically decreased unexcused absences. Moreover, although not all of the effects reached statistical significance, it is notable that for five of the seven outcome variables, the same pattern was observed, with relationally adequate youths tending to show greater (although nonsignificant in some cases) impacts from mentoring than did relationally strong or relationally vulnerable youths. The two exceptions to this pattern were self-perceptions of academic abilities and unexcused absences. All profiles showed trends toward fewer unexcused absences, although only among relationally vulnerable youths did this effect reach marginal significance ( p .09), and both relationally vulnerable and relationally adequate youths showed trends toward improvements in self-perceptions of academic abilities, although only among relationally adequate youths did this trend reach marginal significance ( p .07). In addition to examining the main effect of mentoring in each relational profile, we were also interested in determining whether youths in the relationally adequate profile derived greater benefits from mentoring than did youths in the relationally vulnerable and relationally strong profiles. A two-level random-intercept regression model, accounting for clustering by school, was conducted to test for interaction effects between treatment group and a dummy variable for youths who were in the relationally adequate profile (vs. youths in either of the other two profiles). Grade in school, gender, minority status, baseline substance use, and baseline level of the outcome variable were included as covariates. Significant interaction effects were observed between treatment group and relational profile for prosocial behavior ( p .05), and marginally significant

Table 5 Descriptive Information for Youth Profiles at Baseline
Variable Girls Minority status Free/reduced lunch Grade level: M (SD) Profile 1: RV (n 192 (50%) 250 (64%) 228 (70%) 5.67a (1.48) 388) Profile 2: RA (n 291 (56%) 335 (65%) 319 (70%) 5.30b (1.30) 516) Profile 3: RS (n 131 (57%) 125 (54%) 132 (64%) 4.79c (1.09) 232)
2 2 2

/F test

(2, N 1,136) 4.94† (2, N 1,136) 9.27 2 (2, N 1,136) 2.80 F(2, 1136) 32.1 relationally

Note. Means with different subscripts differ significantly by profile on the basis of Scheffe post hoc tests. RV adequate; RS relationally strong. † p .10. p .05. p .001.

relationally vulnerable; RA

MENTORING AND YOUTH RELATIONAL PROFILES

457

Table 6 Means (and Standard Deviations) for Baseline Values in Outcome Variables for Youth Profiles
Variable Overall academic performance Prosocial behavior Classroom affect Classroom effort Self-perceptions of academic abilities Global self-worth Unexcused absences Profile 1: RV (n 2.32a (1.03) 3.03a (0.59) 3.17a (0.70) 2.61a (0.76) 2.59a (0.65) 3.01a (0.62) 0.42a (1.93) 388) Profile 2: RA (n 2.60b (1.12) 3.16b (0.56) 3.23a (0.66) 2.81b (0.76) 2.77b (0.60) 3.21b (0.50) 0.25a (0.98) 516) Profile 3: RS (n 2.65b (1.12) 3.19b (0.53) 3.31a (0.64) 2.92b (0.72) 3.10c (0.56) 3.45c (0.44) 0.36a (1.20) 232) F(2, F(2, F(2, F(2, F(2, F(2, F(2, F test 977) 999) 996) 998) 1135) 1136) 954) 8.21 6.77 2.81† 12.15 49.06 50.35 1.34 relationally

Note. Means with different subscripts differ significantly by profile on the basis of Scheffe post hoc tests. RV adequate; RS relationally strong. † p .10. p .01. p .001.

relationally vulnerable; RA

effects were observed for overall academic performance ( p .06) and for classroom affect ( p .05; see Table 8).

Additional Analyses
We also examined intermediate outcomes that included mentor–youth relationship quality and match length. The association between baseline relational profile and mentor–youth relationship quality was tested using two measures of relationship quality: youth emotional engagement (M 3.59, range 1.00 to 4.00, SD 0.54) and youth unhappiness (M 1.34,

range 1.00 to 4.00, SD 0.53). The three profiles did not differ significantly on either of these measures. The association between relational profile and match length (M 134.14 days, range 0 to 251 days, SD 63.92 days) was also explored; it revealed no significant differences in match length among the three profiles. In addition to investigating relational profile as a moderator of impacts, we examined subgroup analyses exploring potential differential impacts from the mentoring intervention by gender, race (minority vs. White), and age group (Grades 4 –5 vs. Grades 6 –9).

Table 7 Effects of Mentoring on Youths With Different Relational Profiles
Variable and effect Overall academic performance B 95% CI Effect size Prosocial behavior B 95% CI Effect size Classroom affect B 95% CI Effect size Classroom effort B 95% CI Effect size Self-perceptions of academic abilities B 95% CI Effect size Global self-worth B 95% CI Effect size Unexcused absences B 95% CI Effect size


Profile 1: RV 0.00 [ 0.17, 0.18] .00 0.04 [ 0.15, 0.06] .06 0.07 [ 0.20, 0.05] .10 0.05 [ 0.07, 0.17] .06 0.09 [ 0.03, 0.20] .15 0.04 [ 0.16, 0.08] .07 0.25† [ 0.53, 0.03] .16 relationally adequate; RS

Profile 2: RA 0.21 [0.06, 0.36] .19 0.09 [0.00, 0.18] .15 0.07 [ 0.03, 0.17] .10 0.10† [0.00, 0.21] .13 0.09† [0.00, 0.18] .15 0.07 [ 0.02, 0.15] .12 0.17 [ 0.39, 0.06] .11 relationally strong; CI

Profile 3: RS 0.05 [ 0.17, 0.27] .04 0.04 [ 0.15, 0.08] .06 0.04 [ 0.18, 0.10] .06 0.11 [ 0.26, 0.05] .14 0.02 [ 0.14, 0.11] .03 0.01 [ 0.13, 0.10] .02 0.17 [ 0.51, 0.17] .11 confidence interval.

Note. RV p .10.

relationally vulnerable; RA p .05. p .01.

458

SCHWARTZ, RHODES, CHAN, AND HERRERA

Table 8 Interaction Effects Between Treatment Group and Relationally Adequate Youths
Variable Overall academic performance Prosocial behavior Classroom affect Classroom effort Self-perceptions of academic abilities Global self-worth Unexcused absences


Interaction effects .19 .12 .14† .10 .03 .08 .13


Effect size .17 .19 .21 .13 .05 .13 .1

95% confidence interval [0.00, 0.39] [0.00, 0.24] [0.00, 0.28] [ 0.05, 0.23] [ 0.10, 0.15] [ 0.05, 0.20] [ 0.18, 0.45]

p

.10.

p

.05.

These analyses failed to reveal significant effects, with the exception of one marginally significant interaction effect between age group and classroom effort ( p .07).

Discussion
The results of this study suggest that there are meaningful differences in the quality of youths’ baseline relationships with their parents, teachers, and peers and that these differences may help to explain why some youths are more likely than others to benefit from the guidance and support of volunteer mentors. In particular, person-centered analysis suggested three distinct relational profiles: (a) youths who were most relationally vulnerable, struggling particularly in their relationships with their parents and teachers; (b) youths who displayed moderately close relationships with most people in their lives, adults and peers alike; and (c) youths who had the strongest relationship networks, showing especially positive relationships with their parents and teachers. These groups, in turn, experienced differential benefits from school-based mentoring. Youths who already had very positive relationships with other key adults and peers in their lives did not benefit significantly from mentoring on any of the outcome variables tested. Youths who were most relationally vulnerable at baseline benefited more than those who had the strongest relationships, yet they too derived relatively few benefits. This group demonstrated only marginally significant improvements on one outcome (unexcused absences). Youths who showed the greatest improvements from mentoring were characterized by moderately strong relationships at baseline. Such youths demonstrated significant improvements in overall academic performance and prosocial behavior and marginally significant improvements in classroom effort and self-perceptions of academic abilities, relative to youths with similar preintervention relational profiles who did not receive mentoring. Moreover, they showed significantly stronger impacts in prosocial behavior from mentoring than did youths with especially strong or especially weak relationships, and they showed marginally stronger impacts in overall academic performance and classroom affect. These findings are consistent with previous studies, which have shown that mentoring relationships can vary considerably in their effectiveness, depending on the characteristics of youths and mentors (Grossman & Rhodes, 2002; Rhodes & DuBois, 2008). The differential patterns might also help to explain the relatively disappointing effects that have emerged from recent meta-analyses (Eby et al., 2008; Jolliffe & Farrington, 2007) and large-scale,

random assignment evaluations of youth mentoring (Bernstein et al., 2009; Herrera et al., 2007). When impacts from all of the matches are combined, positive outcomes are sometimes masked by the neutral and even negative outcomes associated with lesseffective matches. Interventions that, a priori, target the subgroup of youths that is most likely to benefit are likely to yield more promising effect sizes. At the same time, researchers should identify those factors that facilitate or impede the formation of effective mentoring ties among more vulnerable youths. Youths with different baseline relational profiles did not differ significantly in perceptions of the quality of their relationships with their mentors, as measured by youth engagement in the relationship as well as unhappiness with the relationship. In fact, the vast majority of youths had positive perceptions of their relationships with their mentors. That most mentors were able to establish close, positive relationships, regardless of youths’ past experiences in relationships with adults, is encouraging, because the relationship is the foundation for any change that may occur as a result of mentoring. Nonetheless, the current study suggests that these relationships were most strongly associated with positive youth outcomes for youths who had moderately strong relationships with adults before they began mentoring. It is not surprising that those who already had strong relationships with their parents, teachers, and peers were also able to forge close ties with their mentors, even if they did not necessarily benefit from them. By contrast, vulnerable youths may have distanced themselves from their mentors in subtle ways that were not measured. The differential effects of mentoring for youths with different relational profiles may also be partially explained by the fact that SBM is a relatively low dosage intervention. Mentors in this program met with youths approximately three times per month for an average of only 5– 6 months over the course of the academic year. Youths who were most relationally vulnerable may have required a more intensive mentoring intervention or more specifically trained volunteer mentors. In fact, the meta-analysis conducted by DuBois, Holloway, et al. (2002) showed that although mentoring programs failed to demonstrate significant impacts among youths with individual risk factors (but without environmental risk factors), programs that implemented a majority of “best practices” were able to affect such youths. Similarly, a small study of the impact of volunteer mentoring on aggressive children found that youths who were rated by peers as high in narcissism or who had troubled relationships with their parents tend to benefit more from a comprehensive, multicomponent mentoring program

MENTORING AND YOUTH RELATIONAL PROFILES

459

than from standard mentoring (Cavell & Hughes, 2000). Taken together, these studies suggest that more intensive models of mentoring programs may be more effective with youths whose relationship histories bring with them unique challenges. It is also possible that the lack of benefits demonstrated by relationally strong youths may be due in part to a ceiling effect; that is, because such youths reported higher scores than did relationally vulnerable youths on most of the outcome variables at baseline, they may have had less room for improvement. However, it is notable that relationally strong youths differed from the relationally adequate youths on only two of the outcome variables at baseline (global self-worth and self-perceptions of academic abilities), and relationally adequate youths showed the strongest impacts from mentoring. It is therefore unlikely that baseline differences account for relationally strong youths failing to show significant impacts from mentoring. In fact, these data are consistent with other research demonstrating that youths with the highest levels of family support derive relatively fewer benefits from mentoring (DuBois, Holloway, et al., 2002; Grossman & Johnson, 1999; Johnson, 1997). Although it is possible that these youths may benefit on outcomes that were not assessed in the current study, these results, in conjunction with previous research, suggest that mentoring interventions may be less efficacious for youths who already have a surfeit of adult support and that such youths may be better served by other programs and activities that place less emphasis on relationship building and more on developing their specific skills and interests. Referring such youths to other programs would also help to relieve the long wait lists characteristic of many volunteer-mentoring programs. For youths who had only marginally strong relationships— neither particularly difficult nor close—the individual attention from a caring mentor proved to be particularly effective. These youths may have the necessary foundation of trust and the skills to forge and benefit from close ties yet, unlike the relationally strong youths, may not have experienced as many close relationships. In fact, because teachers’ attention is frequently dominated by the most troubled youths, more moderately vulnerable youths are sometimes relatively neglected in the classroom (Pianta, 1999). It appears that SBM has the potential to give such youths the extra attention they need to help them make measurable gains in several key outcomes. In addition to the practical implications for mentoring, this study also has interesting theoretical implications. The clusters resulting from latent profile analysis are consistent with previous research, which has demonstrated considerable continuity between parent, teacher, and peer relationships (Allen et al., 2007; Berlin & Cassidy, 1999; Carlson et al., 2004; Lynch & Cicchetti, 1992; Rydell et al., 2005; Sroufe, 1989). Notably, these profile structures did not differ by youth age. This may be due in part to the fact that the sample included a relatively narrow range of ages, with most of the participants (94%) falling between Grades 4 and 8. However, previous research has also indicated that similar patterns of relationships may exist across age groups. Specifically, a study investigating social support in adolescents’ relationships with their mothers, friends, and romantic partner in Grade 10 and in Grade 12 observed the same structure of relationship networks across youths in both grades (Laursen, Furman, & Mooney, 2006). This study also identified three profiles (high mother, high friend, and high romantic partner; low mother, low friend, and low romantic part-

ner; and low mother, low friend, no romantic partner). Because they used median splits to group youths, they excluded the possibility of a middle group, yet both this study and the current study provide support for continuity across youths’ relationships, as well as indicate similar profile structures across age groups. Nevertheless, in the current study, although profile structure was the same across age groups, youth age differed across profile. Specifically, relationally vulnerable youths tended to be older, and relationally strong youths tended to be younger. This is consistent with previous research documenting youths’ feelings of reduced closeness to teachers and other adults as they transition from elementary to middle school (Cattley, 2004; Lynch & Cicchetti, 1997). Although relational profile was significantly associated with grade level, it is notable that, with the exception of a marginally significant effect on one outcome variable, grade level failed to significantly moderate youth impacts from mentoring (also see Herrera et al., in press). As noted earlier, this could relate to the relatively constricted range of grade levels for youths in the study. In addition, youth age is associated with mentor age in this study. Specifically, older youths were more likely to be matched with adult mentors, whereas younger children were more likely to be matched with high school student mentors, who may be less effective than adult mentors are (Herrera, Kauh, Cooney, Grossman, & McMaken, 2008). As a result, differences in impacts across grade levels might have been masked due to differences in mentor characteristics. Examination of the relational profiles also reveals that peer relationships did not appear to differ by profile to the same extent that teacher and parent relationships did. In particular, relationally vulnerable and relationally adequate youths did not differ significantly in their relationships with peers. Although this may be in part a function of the instrument, which measured peer acceptance as opposed to relationship quality and attachment, it may also be related to the fact that there is a more natural transference from parent to adult teacher relationships (Hartup, 1989). Furthermore, the fact that youths’ relationships with their mentors tended to be positive, regardless of their relationships with their parents or teachers, suggests that mentors may be viewed in some ways as a hybrid between an adult and a peer, particularly because a large percentage of mentors to younger youths were high school students. Mentors may have the potential to serve as a bridge between peer and adult relationships, allowing even those youths with insecure parental relationships to update their working models of attachment through positive relationships with mentors (Rhodes & DuBois, 2006). Although considered to be relatively stable over time, working models are flexible to modification in response to changing life circumstances, such as engaging in emotionally supportive relationships (Belsky & Cassidy, 1994). Indeed, with the increases in perspective taking and interpersonal understanding that often accompany this stage of development, adolescence may lend itself uniquely to the revision of working models (Main, Kaplan, & Cassidy, 1985). Analyses of therapeutic alliances (Goldfried, 1995), home visitors (Olds, Kitzman, Cole, & Robinson, 1997), and mentoring relationships provide additional support for such revision. In fact, although not measured in this study, a common mechanism thought to underlie the effectiveness of mentoring is its potential to influence youths’ relationships with their parents and teachers (Rhodes et al., 2000; Rhodes, Reddy, & Grossman, 2005; Thomson & Zand, 2010). Of course, these pro-

460

SCHWARTZ, RHODES, CHAN, AND HERRERA

cesses take time (Grossman & Rhodes, 2002), and it may be that the dosage provided in the current study was insufficient for complex processes such as the revision of working models of attachment to occur, particularly among the most relationally vulnerable youths. In future research, the critical inputs necessary for more vulnerable subgroups of youths to benefit from mentoring interventions should be specified. Within this context, it will be important to include outcomes representing a range of developmentally salient domains, particularly because youths might display different competencies at different developmental stages (Luthar, 2006; Masten, 2001; Zand et al., 2009). Once the necessary threshold of quality, intensity, and duration is established, it will also be important to explore the underlying processes governing mentors’ influence in different groups. In some cases, for example, mentors may function as alternative or secondary attachment figures, providing a secure base from which youths can achieve crucial social and cognitive competencies. In other cases, the relationship might have a positive impact by providing role modeling and opportunities for mastery (Rhodes & Lowe, 2009). Although this study has several strengths, including longitudinal data from multiple informants and a large, national sample, there also are limitations that should be noted. Youths and parents or guardians who consented to participate in the study may have differed from those who declined to participate. Moreover, all data were drawn from youths in Big Brothers Big Sisters SBM programs, limiting our ability to generalize our findings to other mentoring programs that may differ in structure. For example, the Big Brothers Big Sisters SBM programs include a relatively large percentage of mentors who are of high school age, and they have been found to be somewhat less effective than older volunteer mentors (Herrera et al., 2008). Likewise, because youths in SBM programs tend to spend less time with their mentors than do youths in community-based mentoring programs, results might differ in more intensive programs, perhaps allowing relationally vulnerable youths to derive more benefits from the intervention. It should also be noted that the analyses are based on a relatively short follow-up period in a sample in which a significant number of matches ended even before the follow-up. It may be that stronger effects across a broader range of youths would be observed if youths were matched with a higher proportion of older volunteers in more sustained relationships. In addition, although differences in mentor–youth relationship quality between profiles were not detected, it is possible that more subtle, unmeasured differences may exist. Future studies using more sensitive measures, possibly including qualitative components (e.g., in-depth interviews), are needed to further explore the role mentor–youth relationships may play in mediating the association between preintervention relationships and youth outcomes. This study would have benefited from more extensive measures of attachment, such as the Adult Attachment Interview (George, Kaplan, & Main, 1996) and other scales that have been more directly linked with attachment-relevant constructs (McElhaney, Allen, Stephenson, & Hare, 2009). Likewise, given the importance of the development of autonomy during adolescence and the complex interconnections between attachment and autonomy strivings, future studies might also include indices of autonomy (McElhaney et al., 2009).

It is also worth noting that relationship profiles were based solely on youth self-report, whereas outcomes were based on both teacher report and youth self-report. The inclusion of teacher reports could be problematic, particularly if teachers knew which students were receiving mentoring and biased their responses in favor of them. Herrera et al. (in press) investigated this possibility and found no evidence that teachers systematically inflated their assessments of youths as a function of their group status. Furthermore, although our analyses revealed associations between baseline relational profiles and mentoring impacts, it is possible that there exist underlying factors that may contribute to both relationship difficulties and difficulties in benefiting from a mentoring relationship. For example, highly mobile youths may have more difficulty establishing strong relationships with teachers and peers, as well as establishing lasting relationships with mentors. In addition, our analysis of youths’ support networks focused solely on parents, teachers, and peers, excluding support derived from other caregivers such as natural mentors, older siblings, and extended family members. Despite these limitations, this study represents an important step in considering the key role that children’s and adolescents’ relationship histories play in influencing the benefits that are derived from youth-mentoring interventions. Future research is recommended to better discern the patterns suggested in this study and to further explore the processes through which relationship histories may influence mentoring experiences.

References
Ainsworth, M. D. (1989). Attachment beyond infancy. American Psychologist, 44, 709 –716. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.44.4.709 Allen, J. P., & Land, D. (1999). Attachment in adolescence. In J. Cassidy & P. R. Shaver (Eds.), Handbook of attachment theory and research (pp. 319 –335). New York, NY: Guilford Press. Allen, J. P., Porter, M., McFarland, F. C., McElhaney, K. B., & Marsh, P. (2007). The relation of attachment security to adolescents’ paternal and peer relationships, depression, and externalizing behavior. Child Development, 78, 1222–1239. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2007.01062.x Armsden, G. C., & Greenberg, M. T. (1987). The Inventory of Parent and Peer Attachment: Individual differences and their relationship to psychological well-being in adolescence. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 16, 427– 454. doi:10.1007/BF02202939 Belsky, J., & Cassidy, J. (1994). Attachment: Theory and evidence. In M. L. Rutter, D. F. Hay, & S. Baron-Cohen (Eds.), Development through life: A handbook for clinicians (pp. 373– 402). Oxford, England: Blackwell. Bergman, L. R., & Trost, K. (2006). The person-oriented versus the variable-oriented approach: Are they complementary, opposites, or exploring different worlds? Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 52, 601– 632. doi: 10.1353/mpq.2006.0023 Berlin, L. J., & Cassidy, J. (1999). Relations among relationships: Contributions from attachment theory and research. In J. Cassidy & P. R. Shaver (Eds.), Handbook of attachment theory and research (pp. 688 – 712). New York, NY: Guilford Press. Bernstein, L., Rappaport, C., Olsho, L., Hunt, D., & Levin, M. (2009). Impact evaluation of the U.S. Department of Education’s Student Mentoring Program. Washington, DC: National Center for Education Evaluation and Regional Assistance, Institute of Education Sciences, U.S. Department of Education. Bowlby, J. (1988). A secure base: Parent-child attachment and healthy human development. New York, NY: Basic Books. Brown, B. B., Clasen, D. R., & Eicher, S. A. (1986). Perceptions of peer

MENTORING AND YOUTH RELATIONAL PROFILES pressure, peer conformity dispositions, and self-reported behavior among adolescents. Developmental Psychology, 22, 521–530. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.22.4.521 Carlson, E. A., Sroufe, L. A., & Egeland, B. (2004). The construction of experience: A longitudinal study of representation and behavior. Child Development, 75, 66 – 83. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2004.00654.x Cattley, G. (2004). The impact of teacher-parent-peer support on students’ well-being and adjustment to the middle years of schooling. International Journal of Adolescence and Youth, 11, 269 –282. Cavell, T. A., & Hughes, J. N. (2000). Secondary prevention as context for assessing change processes in aggressive children. Journal of School Psychology, 38, 199 –235. doi:10.1016/S0022-4405(99)00040-0 Connor, D. F. (2004). Aggression and antisocial behavior in children and adolescents: Research and treatment. New York, NY: Guilford Press. Downey, G., Irwin, L., Ramsay, M., & Ayduk, O. (2004). Rejection sensitivity and girls’ aggression. In M. Moretti, C. Odgers, & M. Jackson (Eds.), Girls and aggression: Contributing factors and intervention principles. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association Press. Downey, G., Lebolt, A., Rincon, C., & Freitas, A. L. (1998). Rejection ´ sensitivity and children’s interpersonal difficulties. Child Development, 69, 1074 –1091. DuBois, D. L., Felner, R. D., Brand, S., Phillips, R. S., & Lease, A. M. (1996). Early adolescent self-esteem: A developmental-ecological framework and assessment strategy. Journal of Research on Adolescence, 6, 543–579. DuBois, D. L., Holloway, B. E., Valentine, J. C., & Cooper, H. (2002). Effectiveness of mentoring programs for youth: A meta-analytic review. American Journal of Community Psychology, 30, 157–197. doi:10.1023/ A:1014628810714 DuBois, D. L., & Karcher, M. J. (2005). Handbook of youth mentoring. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. DuBois, D. L., Neville, H. A., Parra, G. R., & Pugh-Lilly, A. O. (2002). Testing a new model of mentoring. New Directions for Youth Development, 93, 21–57. Eby, L. T., Allen, T. D., Evans, S. C., Ng, T., & DuBois, D. L. (2008). Does mentoring matter? A multidisciplinary meta-analysis comparing mentored and non-mentored individuals. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 72, 254 –267. doi:10.1016/j.jvb.2007.04.005 Eccles, J. S., Midgley, C., Wigfield, A., Buchanan, C. M., Reuman, D., Flanagan, C., & Mac Iver, D. (1993). Development during adolescence: The impact of stage-environment fit on young adolescents’ experiences in schools and in families. American Psychologist, 48, 90 –101. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.48.2.90 Geiser, C., Lehmann, W., & Eid, M. (2006). Separating “rotators” from “non-rotators” in the Mental Rotations Test: A multigroup latent class analysis. Multivariate Behavioral Research, 41, 261–293. doi:10.1207/ s15327906mbr4103_2 George, C., Kaplan, N., & Main, M. (1996). The Attachment Interview for Adults (3rd ed.). Unpublished manuscript, University of California, Berkeley. Goldfried, M. R. (1995). From cognitive-behavior therapy to psychotherapy integration: An evolving view. New York, NY: Springer. Grossman, J. B., & Johnson, A. W. (1999). Assessing the effectiveness of mentoring programs. In J. B. Grossman (Ed.), Contemporary issues in mentoring (pp. 28 – 47). Philadelphia, PA: Public/Private Ventures. Grossman, J. B., & Rhodes, J. E. (2002). The test of time: Predictors and effects of duration in youth mentoring programs. American Journal of Community Psychology, 30, 199 –219. doi:10.1023/A:1014680827552 Harter, S. (1985). Manual for the Self-Perception Profile for Children. Denver, CO: University of Denver. Hartup, W. (1989). Social relationships and their developmental significance. American Psychologist, 44, 120 –126. doi:10.1037/0003066X.44.2.120

461

Herrera, C. (2004). School-based mentoring: A closer look. Philadelphia, PA: Public/Private Ventures. Herrera, C., Grossman, J. B., Kauh, T. J., Feldman, A. F., & McMaken, J. (with Jucovy, L. Z.). (2007). Making a difference in schools: The Big Brothers Big Sisters school-based mentoring impact study. Philadelphia, PA: Public/Private Ventures. Herrera, C., Grossman, J. B., Kauh, T. J., & McMaken, J. (in press). Mentoring in schools: An impact study of Big Brothers Big Sisters school-based mentoring. Child Development. Herrera, C., Kauh, T. J., Cooney, S. M., Grossman, J. B., & McMaken, J. (2008). High school students as mentors: Findings from the Big Brothers Big Sisters school-based mentoring impact study. Philadelphia, PA: Public/Private Ventures. Institute for Research and Reform in Education. (1998). Research Assessment Package for Schools (RAPS) manual. Philadelphia, PA: Author. Johnson, A. W. (1997). Mentoring at-risk youth: A research review and evaluation of the impacts of the Sponsor-A-Scholar Program on student performance. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Pennsylvania. Jolliffe, D., & Farrington, D. P. (2007). A rapid evidence assessment of the impact of mentoring on re-offending. London, England: Research Development and Statistics Directorate, Home Office. Karcher, M. J. (2003). The Hemingway: Measure of Adolescent Connectedness. Retrieved April 14, 2009, from http://www.adolescentconnectedness .com Karcher, M. J. (2008). The Study of Mentoring in the Learning Environment (SMILE): A randomized study of the effectiveness of school-based mentoring. Prevention Science, 9, 99 –113. doi:10.1007/s11121-0080083-z Karcher, M. J., Davis, C., & Powell, B. (2002). The effects of developmental mentoring on connectedness and academic achievement. School Community Journal, 12, 35–50. Kobak, R. R., & Sceery, A. (1988). Attachment in late adolescence: Working models, affect regulation, and representations of self and others. Child Development, 59, 135–146. doi:10.2307/1130395 Ladd, G. W., Herald-Brown, S. L., & Andrews, R. K. (2009). The Child Behavior Scale (CBS) revisited: A longitudinal evaluation of CBS subscales with children, preadolescents, and adolescents. Psychological Assessment, 21, 325–339. doi:10.1037/a0016205 Ladd, G. W., & Profilet, S. M. (1996). The Child Behavior Scale: A teacher report measure of young children’s aggressive, withdrawn, and prosocial behaviors. Developmental Psychology, 32, 1008 –1024. doi:10.1037/ 0012-1649.32.6.1008 Larose, S., Bernier, A., & Soucy, N. (2005). Attachment as a moderator of the effect of security in mentoring on subsequent perceptions of mentoring and relationship quality with college teachers. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 22, 399 – 415. doi:10.1177/0265407505052443 Laursen, B., Furman, W., & Mooney, K. S. (2006). Predicting interpersonal competence and self-worth from adolescent relationships and relationship networks: Variable-centered and person-centered perspectives, MerrillPalmer Quarterly, 52, 572– 600. doi:10.1353/mpq.2006.0030 Luthar, S. S. (2006). Resilience in development: A synthesis of research across five decades. In D. Cicchetti & D. J. Cohen (Eds.), Developmental psychopathology: Vol. 3. Risk, disorder, and adaptation (2nd ed.; pp. 739 –795). New Jersey: Wiley. Lynch, M., & Cicchetti, D. (1992). Maltreated children’s reports of relatedness to their teachers. In R. Pianta (Ed.), Beyond the parent: The role of other adults in children’s lives (pp. 81–107). San Francisco, CA: Jossey–Bass. Lynch, M., & Cicchetti, D. (1997). Children’s relationships with adults and peers: An examination of elementary and junior high school students. Journal of School Psychology, 35, 81–99. doi:10.1016/S00224405(96)00031-3 Magnusson, D. (1998). The logic and implications of a person-oriented

462

SCHWARTZ, RHODES, CHAN, AND HERRERA Rhodes, J. E., Reddy, R., & Grossman, J. B. (2005). The protective influence of mentoring on adolescents’ substance use: Direct and indirect pathways. Applied Developmental Science, 9, 31– 47. doi:10.1207/ s1532480xads0901_4 Rhodes, J. E., Reddy, R., Roffman, J., & Grossman, J. B. (2005). Promoting successful youth mentoring relationships: A preliminary screening questionnaire. Journal of Primary Prevention, 26, 147–167. doi: 10.1007/s10935-005-1849-8 Romero-Canyas, R., Downey, G., Berenson, K., Ayduk, O., & Kang, N. J. (2010). Rejection sensitivity and the rejection-hostility link in romantic relationships. Journal of Personality, 78, 119 –148. doi:10.1111/j.14676494.2009.00611.x Rydell, A. M., Bohlin, G., & Thorell, L. B. (2005). Representations of attachment to parents and shyness as predictors of children’s relationships with teachers and peer competence in preschool. Attachment & Human Development, 7, 187–204. doi:10.1080/14616730500134282 Schumann, B. C., Striegel-Moore, R. H., McMahon, R. P., Waclawiw, M. A., Morrison, J. A., & Schreiber, G. B. (1999). Psychometric properties of the Self-Perception Profile for Children in a biracial cohort of adolescent girls: The NHLBI growth and health study. Journal of Personality, 73, 260 –275. Seiffge-Krenke, I., Shulman, S., & Klessinger, N. (2001). Adolescent precursors of romantic relationships in young adulthood. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 18, 327–346. doi:10.1177/0265407501183002 Spencer, R. (2006). Understanding the mentoring process between adolescents and adults. Youth Society, 37, 287–315. doi:10.1177/0743558405278263 Sroufe, L. A. (1989). Pathways to adaptation and maladaptation: Psychopathology as developmental deviation. In D. Cicchetti (Ed.), Emergence of a discipline. Rochester Symposium on Developmental Psychology (pp. 13– 40). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Stata (Release 11.0) [Statistical software]. College Station, TX: StataCorp LP. Thomson, N. R., & Zand, D. H. (2010). Mentees’ perceptions of their interpersonal relationships: The role of the mentor-youth bond. Youth Society, 41, 434 – 445. doi:10.1177/0044118X09334806 Vaughn, M. G., & Howard, M. O. (2004). Adolescent substance abuse treatment: A synthesis of controlled evaluations. Research on Social Work Practice, 14, 325–335. doi:10.1177/1049731504265834 Winters, N. C., Myers, K., & Proud, L. (2002). Ten-year review of rating scales: III. Scales assessing suicidality, cognitive style, and self-esteem. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 41, 1150 –1181. Zand, D. H., Thomson, N., Cervantes, R., Espiritu, R., Klagholz, D., LaBlanc, L., & Taylor, A. (2009). The mentor-youth alliance: The role of mentoring relationships in promoting youth competence. Journal of Adolescence, 32, 1–17.

approach. In R. B. Cairns, L. R. Bergman, & J. Kagan (Eds.), Methods and models for studying the individual (pp. 33– 62). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Main, M., Kaplan, N., & Cassidy, J. (1985). Security in infancy, childhood, and adulthood: A move to the level of representation. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 50, 66 –104. doi:10.2307/ 3333827 Masten, A. S. (2001). Ordinary magic: Resilience processes in development. American Psychologist, 56, 227–238. doi:10.1037/0003066X.56.3.227 Matzenbacher, D. L. (1999). Evaluating the effects of college student mentors on children in an elementary school environment. Dissertation Abstracts International, 60(5-B), 2387. McElhaney, K., Allen, J., Stephenson, C., & Hare, A. (2009). Attachment and autonomy during adolescence. In R. M. Lerner & L. Steinberg (Eds.), Handbook of adolescent psychology (3rd ed.; pp. 358 – 403). New York, NY: Plenum Press. doi:10.1002/9780470479193.adlpsy001012 Mentor/National Mentoring Partnership. (2006). Mentoring in America 2005: A snapshot of the current state of mentoring. Alexandria, VA: Author. Muthen, L. K., & Muthen, B. O. (2006). Mplus Version 5 [Computer ´ ´ software]. Los Angeles, CA: Muthen & Muthen. ´ ´ O’Brien, M. (2005). Studying individual and family development: Linking theory and research. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 67, 880 – 890. doi:10.1111/j.1741-3737.2005.00181.x Olds, D., Kitzman, H., Cole, R., & Robinson, J. (1997). Theoretical formulations of a program of home visitation for pregnant women and parents of young children. Journal of Community Psychology, 25, 9 –25. doi:10.1002/(SICI)1520-6629(199701)25:1 9::AID-JCOP2 3.0 .CO;2-V Pianta, R. C. (1999). Enhancing relationships between children and teachers. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. doi: 10.1037/10314-000 Pierce, K. M., Hamm, J. V., & Vandell, D. L. (1999). Experiences in after-school programs and children’s adjustment in first-grade classrooms. Child Development, 70, 756 –767. doi:10.1111/1467-8624.00054 Portwood, S. G., & Ayers, P. M. (2005). Schools. In D. L. DuBois & M. J. Karcher (Eds.), Handbook of youth mentoring (pp. 336 –347). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Rhodes, J. E., & DuBois, D. L. (2006). Understanding and facilitating the youth mentoring movement. Social Policy Report, 20, 3–19. Rhodes, J. E., & DuBois, D. L. (2008). Mentoring relationships and programs for youth. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 17, 254 –258. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8721.2008.00585.x Rhodes, J. E., Grossman, J. B., & Resch, N. R. (2000). Agents of change: Pathways through which mentoring relationships influence adolescents’ academic adjustment. Child Development, 71, 1662–1671. doi:10.1111/ 1467-8624.00256 Rhodes, J. E., & Lowe, S. (2009). Youth mentoring and adolescent development. In R. M. Lerner & L. Steinberg (Eds.), Handbook of adolescent psychology (3rd ed.; pp. 152–189). New York, NY: Plenum Press.

Received November 13, 2009 Revision received August 24, 2010 Accepted August 25, 2010

Similar Documents

Free Essay

Grant Writing for Dummies

...Grant Writing FOR DUMmIES 3RD ‰ EDITION by Dr. Beverly A. Browning, MPA, DBA Grant Writing For Dummies® 3rd Edition , Published by Wiley Publishing, Inc. 111 River St. Hoboken, NJ 07030-5774 www.wiley.com Copyright © 2009 by Wiley Publishing, Inc., Indianapolis, Indiana Published by Wiley Publishing, Inc., Indianapolis, Indiana Published simultaneously in Canada No part of this publication may be reproduced, stored in a retrieval system, or transmitted in any form or by any means, electronic, mechanical, photocopying, recording, scanning, or otherwise, except as permitted under Sections 107 or 108 of the 1976 United States Copyright Act, without either the prior written permission of the Publisher, or authorization through payment of the appropriate per-copy fee to the Copyright Clearance Center, 222 Rosewood Drive, Danvers, MA 01923, 978-750-8400, fax 978-646-8600. Requests to the Publisher for permission should e addressed to the Permissions Department, John Wiley & Sons, Inc., 111 River Street, Hoboken, NJ 07030, (201) 748-6011, fax (201)748-6008, or online at http:// www.wiley.com/go/permissions. Trademarks: Wiley, the Wiley Publishing logo, For Dummies, the Dummies Man logo, A Reference for the Rest of Us!, The Dummies Way, Dummies Daily, The Fun and Easy Way, Dummies.com, Making Everything Easier, and related trade dress are trademarks or registered trademarks of John Wiley & Sons, Inc. and/ or its affiliates in the United States and other countries, and...

Words: 113853 - Pages: 456

Premium Essay

Leadership in Action Alnap Study

...Leadership in Action: Leading Effectively in Humanitarian Operations A CK N O W L E D G E M E N TS First and foremost, we would like to thank the individuals who allowed their leadership to be placed in the spotlight as one of our case-study examples of effective operational humanitarian leadership. Their generosity and openness in doing so and in answering our many questions are greatly appreciated. We also thank the many other interviewees who spoke to us frankly and shared their insights on what they valued in terms of operational humanitarian leadership. David Peppiatt played an important role in identifying and facilitating a number of the case studies. Isobel McConnan carried out interviews and provided helpful comments on earlier drafts. Georgia Armitage helped with the literature review. The Advisory Group provided invaluable input, words of advice and comments on the draft report, they are: Tim Cross, Wendy Fenton, Randolph Kent, Ky Luu, and Claire Messina. Paul Knox Clarke’s inputs undoubtedly raised the quality of the analysis. Deborah Eade provided excellent editorial services and comments. Sara Swords also commented on an earlier draft. Ben Ramalingam played a key role in getting the study off the ground and in its design. Finally, thanks to John Mitchell for his ongoing and valuable support to this work. The project was undertaken as part of the joint initiative on leadership in the humanitarian sector in partnership with People In Aid, Humanitarian...

Words: 32753 - Pages: 132

Premium Essay

Hokage

...Literature Review PRN 18219 July 2008 Erebus International Australian Catholic University Table of Contents Appendix 1: Literature Review 1 Table of Contents 2 Executive Summary 4 Section 1: Project Overview 4 Section 2: What is Student Wellbeing? 5 Section 3: The Outcomes of Student Wellbeing and its Pathways 6 1. Physical and Emotional Safety 6 2. Pro-social values 7 3. A supportive and caring school community 7 4. Social and Emotional Learning 7 5. A Strengths-based Approach 8 6. A sense of Meaning and Purpose 8 7. A Healthy Lifestyle 9 Section 4: International Focus on Student Wellbeing 9 Section 5: Student Wellbeing in the Australian Educational Context 9 5.1: Australian Government National Frameworks in Education 9 5.2 Report on the of the Responses of State and territory and non-government education authorities to the Concept of a National Student Wellbeing Framework 11 5.3 Student Wellbeing in State and Territory Curriculum and Policy Documents 11 Section 6: Whole School Approaches to Student Wellbeing: Issues of School Leadership, Implementation and Sustainability of Student Wellbeing Initiatives 12 Section 1: Project Overview 13 1.1 Introduction 13 1.2 Project Objectives 13 1.3 Methodology 14 Section 2: Student Wellbeing and its Pathways 16 2.1 What is Student Wellbeing? 16 The Definition of Student Wellbeing Used in this Report 21 Explanations of Key Terms in this Definition 21 The Assumptions Underpinning this Definition...

Words: 33991 - Pages: 136

Free Essay

Social Strigication

...A STUDY ON DRUG ABUSE AMONG YOUTHS AND FAMILY RELATIONSHIP <<青年濫藥與家庭關係的研究 青年濫藥與家庭關係的研究>> 青年濫藥與家庭關係的研究 THE FINAL REPORT (FINAL version) February 2011 1 PREFACE The project on the study on “DRUG ABUSE AMONG YOUTHS AND FAMILY RELATIONSHIP” (“the Study”) is being undertaken by the Centre for Suicide Research and Prevention and the Department of Social Work and Social Administration at the University of Hong Kong (“the Consultant”). The research team comprises the principal investigator (PI), Professor Paul YIP, Director of the Centre for Suicide Research and Prevention and Professor of the Department of Social Work and Social Administration at the University of Hong Kong, and eight co-investigators (Co-Is), Dr. CHEUNG Siu Lan Karen (Demographer), Dr. Sandra Tsang (Social and Family Worker), Dr. Samson Tse (Focus group expert on mental health and drug abuse), Dr. Wong Oi Ling (Family therapist, Family Institute), Prof. Karen Laidler (Sociologist, expertise on assessing drug abuse problem), Dr. Paul Wong (Clinical psychologist), Ms. Frances Law (Social Worker), and Dr. Lilian Wong (Associate Consultant, Department of Pediatrics and Adolescent Medicine, Tseung Kwan O Hospital, Hospital Authority). We would like to thank Mr. Gary Ip, the research assistant of the Department of Social Work and Social Administration, Ms. Garlum Lau, the senior research officer 2 of the Department of Sociology, the participants and NGOs for focus groups and case studies for their kind and valuable...

Words: 80145 - Pages: 321

Premium Essay

Leadership

...Life Cycle Management Pharmaceuticals 360 Degree Feedback Human Development Third Party Audit H R Processes Global H R Mission Performance Management Processes Upward Feedback Clasroom Learning On-the-Job Development Superordination Leadership Pipeline * The contribution of these authors have appeared in Part I of the Colloquium in the July-September, 2011 issue of Vikalpa. The names of authors appear in alphabetical order. Neharika Vohra and Deepti Bhatnagar T he Colloquium on Leadership Development was planned to put together the experiences of various companies and practitioners in companies located in India. In trying to be inclusive and extensive, the final Colloquium turned out to be a diverse and rich collection of 17 different perspectives. Limitations of the journal (in terms of page length and inclusion of other features) did not allow us to carry all the perspectives in one issue. Thus, in the previous volume, we...

Words: 34803 - Pages: 140

Premium Essay

Cross Culture

...Cultural diversity in Britain A toolkit for cross-cultural co-operation Phil Wood, Charles Landry and Jude Bloomfield With the current debate about ‘multiculturalism’, this study sets out a new approach to cultural diversity. It explores ways of unlocking the potential in diversity and identifies strategies to aid greater exchange between different cultural groups. The authors examine the connections between cultural diversity, innovation and thriving, prosperous urban communities, in relation to the economic, social and cultural mix of Britain’s population. They developed tools to harness the potential of diverse communities, and their powers of innovation, for use by policymakers, planners and practitioners. These include a set of indicators of openness to check the readiness of a city to take advantage of diversity, and the intercultural lens through which professionals can examine the familiar in a new light. The study evaluated six aspects of local activity: public consultation and engagement urban planning and development business and entrepreneurship schools the arts and creative industries sport. The project went further by helping participating cities to develop specific economic, social, cultural and planning policies and so to become role models for others. The study draws on local case studies and in-depth interviews with 33 intercultural innovators in seven UK cities, with comparative analysis also conducted in Europe, North America and Australasia. It is aimed...

Words: 31833 - Pages: 128

Premium Essay

Hrm Notes

...Chapter 1: Strategic Role of HRM Strategic Role of Human Resources Management Human Resources Management (HRM): management of people in organizations to drive successful organizational performance and achievement of organization's strategic goals - Attract, retain, engage diverse talent - Hire best, develop talent, create productive work environment ** HR Strategy align with Company Strategy Better HR practices = Better Organizational performance Which practices? ○ Profit Sharing Result? Positive Relationship with turnover, productivity, corporate performance ○ Results Oriented Performance Appraisal ○ Employment Security Human Resource Management Responsibilities 1) Traditional: Operational (Administrative) Category - Hire/maintain employees - Offer advice - Plan future workforce requirements - Appraising performance - Disciplining/counselling - Health and safety - Complaints Handling Outsourcing: using outside vendors to handle specified functions on permanent basis Employee counselling, recruitment, payroll, training, benefits administration 2) Strategic Category - Strategic partner ○ Help achieve strategic objectives Strategy: company's plan on how it will balance internal strengths and weaknesses with external opportunities and threats to maintain competitive advantage • HR Challenge: building leadership pipeline (manage talent shortage) Role in Formulating Strategy - HR professionals + line managers do environmental scanning: identify and analyze...

Words: 21571 - Pages: 87

Premium Essay

Learning Style

...LSRC reference Learning styles and pedagogy in post-16 learning A systematic and critical review This report critically reviews the literature on learning styles and examines in detail 13 of the most influential models. The report concludes that it matters fundamentally which instrument is chosen. The implications for teaching and learning in post-16 learning are serious and should be of concern to learners, teachers and trainers, managers, researchers and inspectors. Learning styles and pedagogy in post-16 learning A systematic and critical review LSRC reference Learning styles and pedagogy in post-16 learning A systematic and critical review LSRC reference LSRC reference Learning styles and pedagogy in post-16 learning A systematic and critical review Frank Coffield Institute of Education University of London David Moseley University of Newcastle Elaine Hall University of Newcastle Kathryn Ecclestone University of Exeter The Learning and Skills Research Centre is supported by the Learning and Skills Council and the Department for Education and Skills The views expressed in this publication are those of the authors and do not necessarily reflect the views of the Learning and Skills Research Centre or the Learning and Skills Development Agency Published by the Learning and Skills Research Centre www.LSRC.ac.uk Feedback should be sent to: Sally Faraday Research Manager Learning and Skills Development Agency Regent Arcade House 19–25 Argyll Street London...

Words: 108874 - Pages: 436

Premium Essay

Business Management

...SECOND 21ST CENTURY ACADEMIC FORUM CONFERENCE AT HARVARD MARCH 8 - 10, 2015 MARTIN CONFERENCE CENTER HARVARD UNIVERSITY BOSTON, MA USA Teaching, Learning, and Research in the “Just Google It” Age CONFERENCE PROCEEDING VOL. 5, NO.1 ISSN: 2330-1236 Table of Contents Authors Paper Title Page Maryam Abdu Investigating Capital Structure Decisions and Its Effect on the Nigerian Capital Market 1 Norsuhaily Abu Bakar Rahimah Embong Ibrahim Mamat Ruzilawati Abu Bakar Idris Abd. Hamid Holistically Integraded Curriculum: Implications for Personality Development 16 Sandra Ajaps Geography Education in the Google age: A Case Study of Nsukka Local Government Area of Nigeria 30 Helen Afang Andow Impact of Banking Reforms on Service Delivery in the Nigerian Banking Sector 45 Billy Batlegang Green IT Curriculum: A Mechanism For Sustainable Development 59 Rozeta Biçaku-Çekrezi Student Perception of Classroom Management and Productive Techniques in Teaching 74 Thomas J.P.Brady Developing Digital Literacy in Teachers and Students 91 Lorenzo Cherubini Ontario (Canada) Education Provincial Policy: Aboriginal Student Learning 101 Jennifer Dahmen Natascha Compes Just Google It?! But at What Price? Teaching Pro-Environmental Behaviour for Smart and Energy-Efficient Use of Information and Communication Technologies 119 Marion Engin Senem Donanci Using iPads in a dialogic classroom: Mutually exclusive or naturally compatible? 132 Nahed Ghazzoul Teaching and Learning in...

Words: 236613 - Pages: 947

Premium Essay

No Paper to Upload

...REGENT UNIVERSITY COLLEGE OF ARTS & SCIENCES UNDERGRADUATE CATALOG 2013-2014 (Fall 2013-Summer 2014) Regent University 1000 Regent University Drive Virginia Beach, VA 23464-9800 800.373.5504 admissions@regent.edu www.regent.edu PREFACE Regional Accreditation Regent University is accredited by the Southern Association of Colleges and Schools Commission on Colleges to award associates, baccalaureate, masters, and doctorate degrees. Contact the Commission on Colleges at 1866 Southern Lane, Decatur, Georgia 30033-4097 or call 404-679-4500 for questions about the accreditation of Regent University. National and State Accreditation Regent University’s undergraduate school is accredited or certified by the following bodies:   Council for Higher Education Accreditation (CHEA) (www.chea.org/) The Teacher Education Accreditation Council (TEAC) The Regent University School of Education's educational leadership and teacher preparation programs and the College of Arts & Sciences interdisciplinary studies program, which are designed to prepare competent, caring, and qualified professional educators are accredited by the Teacher Education Accreditation Council for a period of seven years, from January 9, 2009 to January 9, 2016. This accreditation certifies that the educational leadership, teacher preparation and interdisciplinary studies programs have provided evidence that they adhere to TEAC's quality principles. Teacher Educational Accreditation Council, One Dupont Circle, Suite...

Words: 74326 - Pages: 298

Premium Essay

Edphod8

...# 2011 University of South Africa All rights reserved Printed and published by the University of South Africa Muckleneuk, Pretoria EDPHOD8/1/2012Ð2014 98753223 3B2 Karin-mod Style CONTENTS Learning unit PREFACE SECTION 1 A theoretical framework 1 The pastoral role of the educator in South African public schools: a theoretical framework SECTION 2 Practical examples 2 Understanding cultural diversity in my public school classroom 3 The ABC of building schools for an integrated South African society Ð diverse people unite 4 Education for human rights and inclusivity 5 Child abuse: an educator's guide for the Senior Phase and FET 6 HIV/AIDS education at school 7 Educators' pastoral role in their schools and communities: an opportunity to care SECTION 3 Crisis and trauma in adolescence 8 Crisis: the theory 9 The crisis intervener and the person in crisis: prevention, prejudice and the intervener 10 Crisis intervention: general models 11 The skills for ensuring a positive relationship and interview between the crisis intervener and the adolescent in crisis SECTION 4 The religious world of the learner 12 Understanding religious diversity in my school 186 122 136 144 168 16 24 41 57 81 92 Page (iv) 2 EDPHOD8/1/2012±2014 (iii) PREFACE The study material for this module comprises four sections. Section 1: The theoretical framework for the pastoral role of the educator (see learning unit 1) Section 2: Practical examples to illustrate the applied competence of the community...

Words: 100366 - Pages: 402

Premium Essay

Summer Internship S&M in Hccbpl

...Title (Summer Internship Project Report) Submitted in the Partial fulfillment of the requirement for the award of POST GRADUATION DIPLOMA IN MANAGEMENT (PGDM) Submitted to SIES COLLEGE OF MANAGEMENT STUDIES Nerul, Navi Mumbai Submitted By Sunil Kumar Bose 111 Marketing 2014-16 Declaration I, Sunil Kumar Bose, studying in the second year of POST GRADUATION DIPLOMA IN MANAGEMENT (PGDM) at SIES College of Management Studies, Nerul, Navi Mumbai, hereby declare that I have completed the Summer Internship Project titled “Driving Kinley Water NDs at Medical Channels and at WD outlets” as a part of the course requirements for the POST GRADUATION DIPLOMA IN MANAGEMENT (PGDM) Program. I also declare that the work undertaken by me is original and has not been copied from any sources. I further declare that the information presented in this project is true and original knowledge and has not been submitted to SIESCOMS or any other institute for any other examination. Signature of the Student: Date: 5th July 2015 Name of the Student: Sunil Kumar Bose Roll No.: 111 Acknowledgement I take this opportunity to express my profound gratitude and deep regards to my corporate project guide Mr. Gurjot Singh Bedi, Area Sales Manager (ASM) and Mr. Alok Chand, Sale Manager (SM), HCCBPL Varanasi for his exemplary guidance, monitoring and continuous encouragement throughout...

Words: 17851 - Pages: 72

Premium Essay

Coke Sustainability Report

...2011/2012 GRI Report A companion to the 2011/2012 Sustainability Report This report was released on October 7, 2012 | v1 . 8 About This Report The Global Reporting Initiative (GRI) is “a network-based organization that produces a comprehensive sustainability reporting framework that is widely used around the world.” This year, in 2012, The Coca-Cola Company has set out to report against the Key Performance Indicators (KPIs) that measure economic, environmental and social performance. We have done so within the scope of our Company’s wholly owned operations. Where we have reported information on behalf of the Coca-Cola system (The Coca-Cola Company and our bottling partners), we have flagged this information within the body of the text. For 2012, and the 2011/2012 Sustainability Report specifically, our Company has self-declared a grade B against the GRI G3.1 Guidelines. This year’s Sustainability Report has also received verification by a third-party external verification agency, FIRA Sustainability BV. Their verification is evidenced by a “+” sign next to our grade B, which reflects their verification and approval of our tracking systems. Throughout this report, you will find the KPIs that we have addressed, along with additional information regarding our most critical initiatives and programs. While we strive to continuously increase our transparency, some of the information requested in response to additional KPIs could put at risk our ability to compete and therefore...

Words: 49808 - Pages: 200

Premium Essay

Damsel

...2014-2015 Undergraduate Academic Calendar and Course Catalogue Published June 2014 The information contained within this document was accurate at the time of publication indicated above and is subject to change. Please consult your faculty or the Registrar’s office if you require clarification regarding the contents of this document. Note: Program map information located in the faculty sections of this document are relevant to students beginning their studies in 2014-2015, students commencing their UOIT studies during a different academic year should consult their faculty to ensure they are following the correct program map. i Message from President Tim McTiernan I am delighted to welcome you to the University of Ontario Institute of Technology (UOIT), one of Canada’s most modern and dynamic university communities. We are a university that lives by three words: challenge, innovate and connect. You have chosen a university known for how it helps students meet the challenges of the future. We have created a leading-edge, technology-enriched learning environment. We have invested in state-of-the-art research and teaching facilities. We have developed industry-ready programs that align with the university’s visionary research portfolio. UOIT is known for its innovative approaches to learning. In many cases, our undergraduate and graduate students are working alongside their professors on research projects and gaining valuable hands-on learning, which we believe is integral...

Words: 195394 - Pages: 782

Premium Essay

Environmental Analysis

...VIEW Strategic Human Resource Management Taken from: Strategic Human Resource Management, Second Edition by Charles R. Greer Copyright © 2001, 1995 by Prentice-Hall, Inc. A Pearson Education Company Upper Saddle River, New Jersey 07458 Compilation Copyright © 2003 by Pearson Custom Publishing All rights reserved. This copyright covers material written expressly for this volume by the editor/s as well as the compilation itself. It does not cover the individual selections herein that first appeared elsewhere. ii Permission to reprint these has been obtained by Pearson Custom Publishing for this edition only. Further reproduction by any means, electronic or mechanical, including photocopying and recording, or by any information storage or retrieval system, must be arranged with the individual copyright holders noted. This special edition published in cooperation with Pearson Custom Publishing. Printed in the United States of America 10 9 8 7 6 5 4 3 2 1 Please visit our web site at www.pearsoncustom.com ISBN 0–536–72690–6 BA 996748 PEARSON CUSTOM PUBLISHING 75 Arlington Street, Suite 300 Boston, MA 02116 A Pearson Education Company iii iv Table of Contents SECTION ONE ................................................................. 1 An Investment Perspective and Human Resources .... 2 HUMAN RESOURCE INVESTMENT CONSIDERATIONS ...6 INVESTMENTS IN TRAINING AND DEVELOPMENT ..... 14 INVESTMENT PRACTICES FOR IMPROVED RETENTION ..................

Words: 132738 - Pages: 531