Free Essay

Bases of Power

In:

Submitted By ssils01
Words 10452
Pages 42
Journal of Social Issues, Vol. 49, No. 4 , 1993. pp. 227-251

The Bases of Power: Origins and Recent Developments
Bertram H. Raven
University of California, Los Angeles

The history and background of the analysis of the basis of power is examined, beginning with its origins in the works of Kurt Lewin and his followers at the Research Center for Group Dynamics, particularly the early research by John R. P. French. The original French and Raven (1959) bases of power model posited six bases of power: reward, coercion, legitimate, expert, referent, and informational (or persuasion; Raven, 1965). Since then, as the result of considerable research, the model has gone through signiJicant developments. A more comprehensive model is presented here that reviews the following: various motivations of the influencing agent; an assessment of available power bases in terms of potential effectiveness, time perspective, personal preferences, values and norms; consideration of other strategies such as manipulation; utilization of various preparatory and stage-setting devices to strengthen olte’s power resources; implementation of the power strategies; assessment of effectiveness of influence attempt and its positive andl or negative aftereffects; use of various ameliorative devices; and review, reconsideration, and another round of influence strategies. The overall model is examined in terms of its applicability to various settings including hospital infection control, patient compliance with physicians’ recommendations, confrontations between political jgures, children’s influence on their peers, conflict resolution and negotiation, as well as supervisorlsubordinate relationships. More than 30 years have now elapsed since John R. P. French and I presented a typological analysis of the bases of power in interpersonal inThis article is based on a presentation made at the Centennial Convention of the American Psychological Association, Washington, DC, August 17, 1992, as part of a Symposium in Honor of John R. P. French on his receiving of the Kurt Lewin Award from the Society for the Psychological Study of Social Issues. Correspondence regarding this article should be addressed to Bertram H. Raven, Department of Psychology, UCLA, Los Angeles, CA 90024-1563.
221
022453719311200-0227$07.00 0 1993 The Society for the Psychological Study of Social Issues

228

Raven

fluence (French & Raven, 1959; Raven, 1965, 1983). In this article, I shall discuss the origins of this analysis and point to subsequent and future developments.

The Origins and Initial Formulation of the Bases of Power Model
Kurt Lewin’s Conception of Power

Those who are only familiar with recent works on the bases of power may not be aware of the extent to which our work was stimulated by the many insights of Kurt Lewin. Lewin’s name is not ordinarily associated with social power, but even in his early writings the concept of power and power fields was in evidence. Lewin defined “power” as “‘the possibility of inducing forces’ of a certain magnitude on another person.” Such power could extend over a broad span of that other person’s potential activities, what Lewin would call the “power field” (Lewin, 1944/1951). In discussing the power of the adult over the child, he illustrated the adult’s power field as a series of concentric circles, emanating from the adult and encompassing the life space of the child (Lewin, 1935, p. 131ff.), as can be seen in Fig. 1. In such a situation, he said, “The child may . . . carry out unpleasant tasks unhesitatingly because at every point within his sphere of action he is internally controlled by the wishes of the adult . . .” (p. 132).

Fig. 1. K r Lewin’s conception of the power field of the adult (A) with respect to the child (C). ut (Reprinted by permission from Lewin, 1935, p. 131.)

The Bases of Power

229

Fig. 2. Kurt Lewin’s conceptualization of the child (C) within the power fields of his father (F), grandmother ( G ) , and his tutor, St. Jerome (J), from a story by Tolstoy. (Reprinted by permission from Lewin, 1935, p. 146.)

Of course, a person may be simultaneously affected by the power of several persons, placing him or her under conflicting pressures. From a work of Leo Tolstoy, Lewin illustrated the pressures on a child from the power fields of his father, his grandmother, and his tutor, with concommitant conflicts and frustrations (Lewin, 1935, p. 148). (See Fig. 2.) As Lewin and his colleagues extended their interest to group dynamics, they paid increasing attention to the power of the group over the individual. Often, it seemed, an influencing agent, attempting to change the behavior of persons, would find that hidher influence attempts were countered by group norms in an opposing direction. It thus appeared that, in such circumstances, it would be more effective to influence the behavior of the members of a group collectively, rather than individually. This would be accomplished by presenting the reasons for change to the group collectively, encouraging the members to discuss the need for change, and then to arrive at a group decision to change-assuming, of course, that, on review and discussion of the reasons, the group would arrive at a decision advocated by the change agent. Such an approach proved effective in getting college students to eat more whole wheat rather than white bread; in getting mothers to serve more whole milk, orange juice, and cod liver oil to their

230

Raven

children; in encouraging the consumption of less desirable cuts of meat during a wartime meat shortage, and in encouraging workers to change their method and rate of operation (Lewin, 1947, 1952). Power and Resistance to Change Jack French, and his colleague Lester Coch found clear support for this approach to overcoming resistance to change by workers at the Harwood Manufacturing Corporation (see Fig. 3). The resistance to change came, they said,

8s
Do
75

rd
65

BbQ a 3

:

55

50

45

40

35

0
0

1 . . . . I . . ~ . I . . . . I . . . . l . . . . l . , . , ~ , . , . l , ,

5
M o r e Transfer 1°1

5

10

15

20

25

30

After Transfer

0.P

Ffg. 3. Workers at the Hanvood Manufacturing Corporation resisting change advocated by management and overcoming resistance to change following participation in group decision making and standard setting. (Reprinted by permission from Coch and French, Jr., 1948, p. 523.)

The Bases of Power

231

from a tendency to adhere to group standards that were opposed to the requests from management. Thus, change in production rate could be accomplished through full participation and group decision by the workers involved, with concomitant changes in group standards. Such an approach, they felt, would be more effective than the more common method of management simply telling the workers what changes should be implemented, even if these instructions were accompanied by full explanations and reasons for the requested changes in work behavior, reinforced by the expertise of time-study specialists. “The noparticipation procedure had the effect for the members of setting up management as a hostile power field” (Coch & French, 1948, p. 528). The conflicting forces from management and grohp norms would contribute to frustration in the workers, similar to what Lewin had described in his analysis of the boy in Tolstoy’s story-except that the workers had an additional option-to “leave the field.” Indeed, Coch and French did observe higher turnover rate among workers under these conditions. Mutual participation with management in decision making would operate like influence from a friend, such that the induced force would act more like an own force. That is, the change would be internalized, with obvious advantages for management and the organization (Coch & French, 1948). In rereading this article after many years, I am struck by the extent to which what we later called the “bases of power” are reflected in the rich discussion of Hanvood factory: Management with its coercive, reward, legitimate, informational, and expert power (and hampered by its negative expert and negative referent power) was opposed by the referent and personal coercive power of the work group.
The Focus on Power at the Research Center for Group Dynamics

In the 1950s, the emphasis on group influence continued at the Research Center for Group Dynamics, with Leon Festinger and his co-workers examining pressures toward uniformity in groups (Festinger, 1950, 1954). Meanwhile, Ronald Lippitt and his team focused on implications of his earlier classic work with Lewin and Ralph White on leadership styles (Lewin, Lippitt, & White, 1939). In studies of what they called “contagion” of behavior in youths in summer camps, they had explored the factors that led to some campers being seen as more influential, more powerful, than others (Lippitt, Polansky, Redl, & Rosen, 1952). Alvin Zander and his group examined how people in lower power positions tend to behave defensively toward the powerful (Hurwitz, Zander, & Hymovitch, 1953; Zander & Cohen, 1955). Such effects became particularly apparent in mental health teams consisting of psychiatrists, psychologists, and psychiatric social workers (Zander, Cohen, & Stotland, 1959). Meanwhile, Frank Harary (Harary, 1959) joined Dorwin Cartwright in applying graph and matrix theory to the analysis of group structure, an approach that Jack

232

Raven

French utilized quite effectively in his first analysis of power structure (French, 1956). In the Tuesday evening seminars at the Research Center for Group Dynamics, social power in interpersonal and group influence became a central theme. Dorwin Cartwright recognized our collective efforts and offered a unifying direction in his SPSSI Presidential Address, entitled “Power: A Neglected Variable in Social Psychology” (Cartwright, 1959). Conceptualizing the Bases o Power f It was within this context that Jack French and I began to meet regularly to discuss the development of a general theory of social power. We defined social power as “potential influence,” which we should note was very similar to Lewin’s-“the possibility of inducing forces.” Our approach, then, was to examine what sorts of resources a person might have to draw upon to exercise influence. We began with a paper by Leon Festinger entitled “An analysis of compliant behavior” (Festinger, 1953). Festinger pointed out that in certain occasions social influence in behavior will occur even though the influenced person would not privately accept that change. Such, he said, would be particularly likely when he or she was threatened with punishment for noncompliance and could not leave the situation. In that case, of course, it would be especially important that hidher compliance or noncompliance could be observed by those exerting influence. On the other hand, a person who desires to remain in that social relationship will not only comply but privately accept the change as well. French would note that the latter situation was similar to those of the Harwood factory workers, where the “induced force” from the group became an “own force.” The idea was not entirely new, but it stimulated French and me to examine numerous examples of influence situations and their underlying dynamics. We then discerned that there were two important dimensions that determine the form of influence or compliance: (a) social dependence and (b) the importance of surveillance. We could readily think of examples in which one person, an influencing agent, could convince another with clear logic, argument, or information-where, let us say, a supervisor would give hidher subordinates good reasons why a change in behavior might lead to greater productivity. Even though it was initially the communication from the agent that would produce the change, that change would become socially independent-the target of influence would completely accept (i.e., internalize) the change, and the agent would become inconsequential. We called that informational influence. I also felt that the person who had the means to exert such influence could be said to have informational power-though I was not able to convince French that the term “power” was appropriate. (Apparently, my own informational power was insufficient in this case.)

The Bases of Power

233

The threat of punishment, as described by Festinger, would lead to change that continued to be socially dependent upon the agent, but, as Festinger had pointed out, surveillance was important for such influence to be effective. For example, the supervisor says, “Do the job this way or you will be demoted.” In its potential, then, we referred to such a resource as coercive power. But wouldn’t there be a similar situation if the agent promised some sort of reward, an increase in pay or privileges in exchange for compliance? Reward power, then, would also be socially dependent, with surveillance important in order for its effects to continue. So now we had two bases of power, coercive and reward, in which the change was socially dependent and where surveillance was important, and one, information, which was socially independent and where surveillance was unimportant. Could we conceive of bases of power in which surveillance was unimportant, but where resulting changes were still socially dependent? We soon thought of three additional bases of power which would have such consequences: legitimate power (what others have called “position power”), expert power, and referent power. The concept of “legitimate power” came from Max Weber (1947): “After all, I am your supervisor, and you should feel some obligation to do what I ask.” Expert power has a long and distinguished history in research on opinion and attitude change: “Even if I may not explain the reasons to you, you must know that, after all, I know what is the best thing to do in such circumstances.” The concept of referent power came from Sherif (1936), Newcomb (1958), and Merton (1957), though we could also see it in operation in many of the studies of group norms described earlier. The target of influence in such a case would comply because of a sense of identification with the influencing agent, or a desire for such an identification. The influencing agent then serves as a model by which the target evaluates hidher behavior and beliefs. The original six bases of power, then, are presented in Table 1. Though we originally drew our examples from relationships between supervisors and subordinates, this typology has since been applied to a number of other areas of social interaction: parents influencing children (Rollins & Thomas,
Table 1. The Six Bases of Social Power
~~ ~~

_

_

_

~

~

Basis of Dower Coercion Reward

Social dependence of change Socially dependent Socially dependent

Importance of surveillance Important Important

..............................................................................................
Socially dependent Socially dependent Socially dependent

Legitimacy Expert Reference

..............................................................................................
Socially independent

Unimportant Unimportant Unimportant Unimportant

Informational

234

Raven

1975; Tashakkori, Thompson, & Simonian, 1989), husbands and wives influencing another (Raven, Centers, & Rodrigues, 1975), children influencing one another (Schmidt & Raven, 1985a, 1985b; Schmidt, Raven, Pastorelli, & Caprara, 1993; Schmidt, Yanagihura, & Smith, 1987), teachers influencing students (Jamieson & Thomas, 1974), doctors influencing patients (Rodin & Janis, 1982; Raven, 1988; Raven & Litman-Adizes, 1986), salesmen influencing customers (Busch & Wilson, 1976; Gaski, 1986), franchisors influencing franchisees (Hunt & Nevin, 1974), couples in sexual encounters (McCormick, 1979), political figures influencing one another (Gold & Raven, 1992; Raven, 1990; Rasinski, Tyler, & Fridkin, 1985), and, in quite a few studies, influence of supervisors in organizational settings (e.g., Abdalla, 1987; Ansari & Kapoor, 1987; Cobb, 1980; Cope, 1972; Frost & Stahelski, 1988; Hinkin & Schreischeim, 1990; Kabanoff, 1985; Melii-Navarro & Peir6-Silla, 1984; Podsakoff & Schriescheim, 1985; Shaw & Condelli, 1986; Sinha & Singh-Sengupta, 1991; Stahelski, Frost, & Patch, 1989; Student, 1968).

Subsequent Development of the Conceptual Framework of the Bases of Power
Further Differentiation and Elaboration In the years that followed, the bases of social power model has developed substantially, benefiting from research and theoretical developments in various related fields-cognitive social psychology, attitude and attitude change, and organizational psychology. Though we still believe that most social influence can be understood in terms of the six bases of power, some of these bases have been elaborated and further differentiated (see Table 2). Coercive power and reward power: Personal forms. Going beyond tangible rewards and real physical threats, we have had to recognize that personal approval from someone whom we like can result in quite powerful reward power; and rejection or disapproval from someone whom we really like can serve as a basis for powerful coercive power (Raven & Kruglanski, 1970). There is some indication that the more personal forms, which have sometimes been called “attraction power,” are more likely to be associated with women than with men (Broverman et al., 1972; Johnson, 1976a, 1976b). Legitimate power: Reciprocity, equity, and dependence. We have had to go beyond the legitimate power that comes from one’s formal position and recognize other forms of legitimate power that may be more subtle, which draw on social norms such as (a) the legitimate power of reciprocity (“I did that for you, so you should feel obliged to do to this for me”; Gouldner, 1960), (b) equity (“I have worked hard and suffered, so I have a right to ask you to do something to

The Bases of Power
Table 2. Further Differentiating the Bases of Social Power
Basis of Dower Coercion Reward Legitimacy Further differentiation Impersonal coercion Personal coercion Impersonal reward Personal reward Formal legitimacy Legitimacy of reciprocity Legitimacy of equity Legitimacy of dependence (powerless) Positive expert Negative expert Positive referent Negative referent Direct information Indirect information

235

Expert Reference Informational

make up for it” (Walster, Walster, & Berscheid, 1978), and (c) responsibility or dependence, a norm saying that we have some obligation to help others who cannot help themselves and who are dependent upon us (Berkowitz & Daniels, 1963). (This form of legitimate power has sometimes been referred to as the “power of the powerless.”) Expert power and referent power: Positive and negative forms. Both of these bases of power were originally examined only in their positive forms: A subordinate may do what hidher supervisor asks because he/she feels that the supervisor knows best, or because the supervisor is someone admirable and desirable-who knows, the subordinate may aspire to be a supervisor some day. But it had been observed that sometimes we may do exactly the opposite of what the influencing agent does or desires that we do. [What Hovland, Janis, and Kelley (1953) called the “boomerang effect.”] Why? Perhaps we recognize the expertise of the influencers, but distrust them and assume they are using their superior knowledge for their own best interests, not ours. Or perhaps we see the agent as someone whom we dislike, someone from whom we would prefer to disidentify ourselves. Thus we incorporated into our system the concept of negative expert power and negative referent power. Informational power: Direct and indirect. Informational power, or persuasion, is based on the information, or logical argument, that the influencing agent could present to the target in order to implement change. However, information can sometimes be more effective if it is presented indirectly. The early research on the effectiveness of “overheard” communications, as compared to direct communications, would seem to bear this out (Walster & Festinger, 1962). Falbo and

236

Raven

Peplau (1980) found the direct/indirect dimension particularly important in their classification of power strategies. There is quite a difference between an influencing agent directly telling a target what s/he wants and why, vs. doing so through hints and suggestions. Johnson (1976b) found that women were especially likely to use the indirect forms of information, men more likely to use direct information. Indirect information seems especially useful when a person in what is considered a low-power position attempts to influence someone in a superior position. Thus Stein (1971) notes that nurses who may feel that they have a useful suggestion in the treatment of a patient will tend to avoid direct informational influence, and will instead use an indirect approach such as “This medication seemed helpful, doctor, for the patient down the hall who had a similar problem.” The more direct form of information (e.g., “Doctor, this other medication, you must know, will be less likely to affect the patient’s blood pressure, and would be much better, since he has a heart problem”) might not only be less effective, but could result in disastrous interpersonal problems.
Nonvolitional Methods o Influence: Force and Manipulation f

While it appears that most influence strategies can be analyzed in terms of various combinations of the six bases of power and their variants, we must recognize that there are other means of influence that would not fit so directly. Two such devices are force and manipulation. All of the other bases of power involve volition on the part of the target-the target can decide whether to comply or accept punishment, to refuse and forgo rewards, to go against the advice of an expert or against the logic of the information, etc. Force is defined as a form of induced change in a target that occurs without the target’s volition. It includes the use of physical restraints (as in a psychiatric ward) or compulsion without choice-what is often referred to as “brute force.” The agent of change may treat the target as if the target is no more than a physical object (Wrong, 1980). We see it in uncooperative psychiatric patient, who, not responding to various other influence attempts, is physically strapped to hislher bed. Or in the parent who, after nicely asking the child to stop playing in the street, forcibly picks up and locks the child in a room. The term is perhaps most obviously applied in “forcible rape.” It is, again, the lack of volition that distinguishes force from coercion (as well as other forms of influence), though the threat of force may be used to establish coercive power. Manipulation is a form of influence in which the influencing agent succeeds in changing a target by first changing some other aspect of the target or hidher environment. In ecological or environmental manipulation the environment itself is changed so as to either facilitate or inhibit some form of behavior (Cartwright, 1965). One can influence a driver to use a seat belt by designing the car so that it cannot be started unless the seat belt is fastened. Or one can keep uncooperative

The Bases of Power

237

neighbors from trespassing over one’s property by erecting a high barbed wire fence. There are other forms of manipulation as well, such as influencing the target’s behavior in such a way that helshe is unable to engage in another form of behavior that the agent wishes to prevent. You can manipulate a traveling companion to stop whistling by engaging him/her in active and continuous conversation. Force and manipulation do not fit easily into our power taxonomy since they are characterized by a lack of volition on the part of the target. However, we include them here for completeness.
Some Further Aspects o Interpersonal Influence f Invoking or reducing the power of third parties. Sometimes an influencing agent can bring about change in a target by invoking the power of third parties. Perhaps the supervisor could gain the assistance of a popular co-worker to help persuade the recalcitrant worker. A mother may invoke the potential coercive power of the father-“Daddy is going to hear about this when he gets home!” Or a fire-and-brimstone minister may invoke the power of an omniscient, omnipotent, omnipresent God, who will punish transgressors. In a sense, some of the earlier methods utilized by Lewin (1952) and Coch and French (1948), in which group discussion and group decision were utilized to implement change, can be seen as utilizing the power of groups as third parties. By the same token, the agent can also attempt to reduce the power of third parties who might oppose the recommended changes-undermining their expertise or legitimacy, suggesting that they are not desirable models, questioning the logic in their persuasive appeal, etc. The mode of influence. We have become increasingly aware that the effectiveness of an influence attempt, and the aftermath of influence, is a function not only of the basis of power but the mode, or manner, in which it is exerted. An influence attempt can be presented in a loud, forceful, threatening, or sarcastic mode, or in a softer, friendlier, light-humored mode. The effect of informational influence can be either enhanced or reduced, if the information is presented in a threatening or fear-invoking manner, as, for example, the use of a fear appeal by a physician who attempts to convince a patient to stop smoking (Janis & Feshbach, 1953; Sutton, 1982). Preparatory devices: Setting the stage for social influence. It appears to me that at least some of the inconsistency in the findings of various studies of social power results from the confusion between use of various bases of power or indirect methods of influence, and the various stage-setting or preparatory devices that will allow for such bases of power to be effective. As the influencing agent assesses hislher bases of power, slhe may decide that, yes, such a power

238
Table 3. Examples of Preparatory Devices for the Use of Social Power

Raven

Preparing the stage or scene Displaying diplomas, library, photos with celebrities [Expert] Wearing laboratory coat, stethoscope, etc. [Expert] Arranging of podium, or desk, chairs . . . [Legitimate] Enhancing or emphasizing power bases Refering to agent’s ability to control rewards; punishments; formal role as supervisor, teacher, doctor, etc. [Legitimate] Intimidation, presenting fearful image [Coercion] Ingratiation, via compliments, etc., to increase the target’s attraction to the agent [Personal Reward, Personal Coercion, Referent Power] Self-promotion, emphasizing superior knowledge [Expert] Emphasizing communality of background, identification, goals [Referent] Doing a favor for target [Reciprocity], emphasizing one’s dependence upon target [Responsibility], telling of one’s selfless dedication and sacrifice, reference to some harm that target had imposed on agent [Equity] Making a request that the target would not be likely to accept, to induce guilt, in preparation for other request [Legitimacy of equity] Presenting background information, which can subsequently serve to enhance informational influence Minimizing target Subtle “put-downs” that decrease the target’s self-esteem, or confidence, so as to increase the agent’s informational, expert, or legitimate power Minimizing opposing influencing agents Reducing expertise, reference, legitimacy, etc., of others who may support the target’s current position or mode of behavior

resource might work, but not just yet. Some preparation may be necessary. (See Table 3.) Jones and Pittman (1982) describe a number of “self-presentational strategies” and Schlenker (1980) discusses “impression management.” Interpersonal tactics such as these may be used to set the stage for the use of a particular power strategy. Is it coercion? Then it may be important to demonstrate to the target that not only are the means available for coercion, but that the agent is ready and willing to pay the costs that coercion implies. The agent then might resort to “intimidation” as a preparatory strategy. Is it reward power? Then, again, the availability of rewards must be stressed. If it is personal reward or coercionoffering approval or threatening rejection-then the agent may first attempt to ingratiate her/himself with the target, with well-placed compliments, flattery, etc. Similarly, for referent power, the communality with the target must be demonstrated. For expert power, a few choice demonstrations of one’s superior knowledge would be useful; the supervisor might tell the worker of the amount of training and experience he has had on this and similar jobs. Physicians, attorneys, professors, and other professionals go through elaborate stage-setting devices for expertise-display of diplomas, imposing libraries, etc. (This is what Jones and

The Bases of Power

239

Pittman call “self-promotion.”) Is it legitimate position power? The supervisor might subtly mention that slhe is, after all, the supervisor who is responsible for this job, or mention that s/he has helped the worker in the past and has a right to expect reciprocation. The supervisor may, indeed, do a favor for the worker, planning to later invoke the legitimate reciprocity norm. Or, for legitimacy based on the equity norm, the supervisor might first want to instill a sense of guilt, coupled with a sense of self-sacrifice by the supervisor (“Look at the problems you caused me when you were not able to come to work last week-but I just did some extra work and got the job done”). (This is akin to Jones and Pittman’s, 1982, strategy of “exemplification.”)
Nongoal-oriented motives. In our original formulation, French and I did not examine the motivation of the influencing agent in exerting influence-it seemed obvious that the motivation was the furtherance of some extrinsic goal, e.g., to increase productivity. However, other, less obvious motives might also operate: Perhaps the satisfaction of some personal needs-a need for power? a need to enhance self-esteem? a need to demonstrate hidher own independence (Winter, 1973)? Or perhaps the major concern was to demonstrate to other workers or supervisors who was really the boss. Such motivations may well affect the choice of power strategy utilized by the influencing agent. [There is indeed some evidence that influencing agents who are lacking in self-confidence will be more likely to use “harder” forms of influence, such as coercion, even when information might be effective (e.g., Kipnis, 1976; Raven, Freeman, & Haley, 1982).] A Powerllnteraction Model of Interpersonal Influence

The various choices and stages in the implementation of social power can be illustrated in a powerlinteraction model as presented in Fig. 4 (Raven, 1992). Let us follow the process of influence from motivation to implementation and subsequent readjustment of the agent.
The motivation to influence. In the upper left corner of the chart, we now see the motivational factors that we have just considered. These then lead the agent to an assessment of the various bases of power and other forms of influence which might be available. In the upper middle, we then see the bases of power and other influence devices which might be in the hidher repertoire. Assessment of available power bases. Having determined what bases of power might be available, the target must assess these alternative courses to action in terms of whether they would be effective in implementing changewhat is the likelihood that s/he would be successful or unsuccessful?

240

Raven

+

t

Fig. 4. Model of powerlinteraction from the perspective of the influencing agent.

Assessment o the costs of differing influence strategies. The agent must also f examine the costs of the influence attempt-it may be effective, but at what costs. Informational influence may require more time and effort than is available. Coercion, as we said, carries the costs of having to maintain surveillance, the hostility of an unhappy subordinate, and sometimes the violation of one’s personal value system or generally accepted social norms. The legitimacy of dependence (“I need your help”) may lead to loss of respect and perhaps may imply an obligation to return the favor. Preparing for the influence attempt. It is at this point (see upper right of chart) that we bring in the various preparatory or stage-setting devicesingratiation, setting the scene, emphasizing or enhancing the agent’s power resources, diminishing the target or opposing influencing agents, etc. Choice of mode of influence. The agent may not only choose the power base, but also the power mode, the manner or tone in which influence is exerted. As we have noted, the mode of influence may sometimes be even more important that the basis of power. Assessing the effects of influence. Following the influence attempt, the agent will want to assess the effects. Was it successful? Is there evidence that the target

The Bases of Power

241

has actually accepted the influence, has actually altered his/her behavior in accordance with the outcome desired by the influencing agent. Does the target really accept the change personally, or is the change socially dependent? Is surveillance important for the change to continue-will the target revert to earlier behavior patterns as soon as the agent cannot continue to check on the degree of compliance? Will the target subsequently internalize the changes in hidher behavior? If the influence attempt was unsuccessful, then it is likely that the agent will review his power resources, evaluate the possibility of future success, and try again. But this time the agent’s motivations may change: Whereas previously s/he had merely wanted to achieve the extrinsic goal, s/he now may have developed some hostility toward the target, which in turn will affect his choice of influence strategy the second time around. The agent’s success or failure will also lead to a reassessment of the available bases of power and the development of a quite different strategy. The effects of feedback. The effects of the influence attempt will then feedback to the agent, quite likely changing his/her self-perceptions and perceptions of the target, thereby changing the agent’s perception of the effectiveness, costs, and desirability of various social power and influence strategies. The agent would consider not only whether the influence attempt was successful or not, but also the presumed attitudes of the target following the attempt. Unsuccessful influence attempts may change the perceptions of what power strategies will be effective. They may also change the agent’s attitudes toward the target (Shaw & Condelli, 1986). Thus the next influence attempt will likely be quite different and may also depend on how the agent attributes causality for noncompliance. When noncompliance is interpreted as due to lack of motivation, the agent will be more likely to resort to coercive power on the next influence attempt (Kipnis, 1976). If it is attributed to lack of ability, then informational or expert power may be more likely.
Metamorphic Effects of Power

In an article on “Conflict and Power,” Kruglanski and I (Raven & Kruglanski, 1970) pointed out that when an influencing agent has coercive power and uses it then the agent will tend to diminish and distrust the target. This results, in part, from the fact that coercive power requires surveillance, compliance is attributed to the use of surveillance, and thus the target is judged as untrustworthy (Kruglanski, 1970; Strickland, 1958). Kipnis (1976) has presented an elaborate discussion of the “metamorphic effects of power” on the power holder, which may help explain Lord Acton’s observation that “power corrupts and absolute power corrupts absolutely” (Acton, 1887/ 1948). Kipnis provides evidence that a power holder who has access to strong bases of power, such as

242

Raven

coercion, will tend to use such resources, particularly if s/he is insecure in his or her position. Having used coercive power, along with surveillance, the power holder attributes any successful influence to the power holder, rather than the target, tending thereby to further devalue and distrust the target. Further influence attempts will be even more coercive, more distrusting, and will tend to further devalue the target, while assuming greater power, and greater privileged status for the power holder. The Kipnis analysis of metamorphic effects of power would seem to apply where the influencing agent has access to strong bases of power, particularly coercive power. However, Sinha and Singh-Sengupta (1991), in a study of Indian managers in large nationalized banks, report quite different results when the managers’ power is restricted and where nonmanagers, in a nationalized system, are able to resist influence. While “high-power’’ managers in that system did indeed use their stronger bases of power, such as coercion, they tended to supplement it with ingratiation, as preparatory for personal reward and personal coercion. “Low-power” managers apparently became resigned to their lack of power and resorted instead to such “soft” power strategies as legitimate power of dependence (power of the powerless), reciprocity, and praise (personal reward power). The analysis of metamorphic effects of power would seem especially useful in the training and counseling of managers.
Power from the Perspective of the Target

In our discussion of the Power/Interaction model we have thus far focused primarily on the perspective of the influencing agent. We might also examine powerlinteraction from the perspective of the target of influence, following essentially the same basic framework, reviewing the motivations of the target, the target’s consideration of counterinfluence strategies, preparatory strategies for noncompliance, and readjustment following feedback on effects of the influence strategy (Raven, 1992). Indeed, while more attention has been given to the metamorphic effects of power on the power holder, the parallel effects on the target of influence may be even more dramatic. The classic “Stanford Prison Experiment,” conducted by Zimbardo, Haney, Banks, and Jaffe (1973), dramatically illustrates the effects of power on both guards and prisoners. A full analysis of the power/interaction process would then have to explore both perspectives.
Power in Conflict and Negotiation

The article by Raven and Kruglanski (1970) was an initial attempt to examine conflict in terms of two parties, each attempting to influence one another, where each was, simultaneously or sequentially, an influencing agent and a

The Bases of Power

-

243

target. Each could then go through the process described above: (a) reviewing available power strategies, preparatory devices and their own and their opponent’s motivations, (b) attempting influence and (c) reexamining their strategies and their evaluation of their opponent. We had speculated as to what would happen if each used various bases of power and we arrived logically at some conclusions. For example, when both used coercive power, each attempting to threaten the other, we might expect to find for each the same metamorphic effects of power that occurs when one power holder threatens one target-greater distancing from one another, greater need for surveillance, greater distrust of the opponent, greater attribution of negative qualities to the other while holding oneself in either higher esteem, . . . indeed, a greater escalation of conflict. By contrast, when both parties effectively used referent power, emphasizing their communality and mutuality, we might find less distancing, less distrust, greater cooperation, and deescalation of conflict. Our speculations in this regard led us to conclusions that were, indeed, consistent with those of a number of other theorists who were concerned about bargaining, negotiation and the deescalation of conflict (e.g., Deutsch, 1973; Pruitt, 1981; White, 1988), and with more recent research on the effects of threats in conflict (e.g., Lawler, Ford, & Blegen, 1988). I also tend to read such strategies into recent works aimed at training people to minimize conflict through effective negotiation (Fisher & Ury, 1981; Ury, 1991), though their presentation is not presented in terms of a power/interaction model. Some Recent Empirical Developments Applications of the Model The model that I have presented above has now been applied in a number of other settings. Let us review a few recent examples. Hospital infection control. Earlier in this article, I discussed the application of the bases of power analysis to the issue of compliance of patients to recommendations by physicians (Raven, 1988; Raven & Litman-Adizes, 1986; Rodin & Janis, 1982). The typology also proved helpful in examining the ways in which doctors and other health practitioners are influenced with regard to following hospital policies. Specifically, in response to a request from The National Center for Disease Control (CDC), we examined how hospitals could be more effective in getting hospital staff members to follow proper procedures in patient care, so as to minimize risk of hospital patients acquiring “nosocomial” infections in the hospital. The CDC had recommended a number of procedures for health care personnel-proper washing of the hands, correct use of intravenous and urinary

244

Raven

catheters, wearing protective masks in isolation wards, etc. These were unfortunately violated all too frequently. Our study, with interviews and questionnaires, was carried out in a representative sample of 437 U.S. hospitals, with various hospital staff participating, including 7188 hospital nurses. We found that we could characterize the influence techniques utilized by infection control nurses and medical epidemiologists according to the bases of power. From our analyses, we suggested ways in which infection control training could be improved. It is of particular interest, and in keeping with our earlier discussion, that self-confidence and perceived selfefficacy played an important role in the power strategies that were utilized. Though informational power was most frequently used, it was especially likely to be used by infection control nurses who were high in self-efficacy, along with expert power. Infection control nurses who were less self-confident were less likely to use either information or expert power; they were more likely to use legitimate and coercive power. There might be several explanations for these results, but they are consistent with the theory that people who are low in selfconfidence tend to use “stronger” power strategies, which would allow them to think that they were responsible for the change (Raven et al., 1982).
Cross-cultural comparisons. We have been attempting to examine cultural differences through questionnaires. I have been collaborating with Joseph Schwarzwald and Meni Koslowski, of Bar Ilan University, Israel, and with Aroldo Rodrigues, of the National University in Rio de Janeiro, in the development of a cross-culturally valid questionnaire. In addition, in an ongoing study, Adeny Schmidt, several of her students, and I have been using a cartoon completion instrument (following Goodchilds & Raven, 1974) to study the ways in which children of different ages and cultures attempt to influence one another. Most recently, we examined similarities and differences in four nations, the United States, Japan, Italy, and Argentina. Though children in all four cultures tended to choose informational influence, such was more true for Americans and Japanese; it was less true for children in Italy and Argentina-the latter were more likely to use coercive power. Japanese children were least likely to use, or to comply with, coercive power, but were much more likely to use and comply with referent power. We were particularly pleased to have an instrument whereby complex concepts, such bases of power, could be tested with children (Schmidt & Raven, 1985a, 1985b; Schmidt et al., 1993; Schmidt et al., 1987). Power o political figures. We have recently been examining the power f strategies used by political figures and have found the power/interaction model to be quite helpful. The motivational factors of the influencing agents, the careful selection and testing of power strategies, the preparatory and self-presentational

The Bases of Power

245

devices, the effects of successive influence attempts on the relationships between the figures, all emerge in our analyses. In my discussion of the influence of Hitler in his confrontation with Austrian prime minister Von Schuschnigg, the stage-setting devices and use of feigned madness for intimidation were striking. In Truman’s attempts to convince MacArthur to cease plans to cross the 38th parallel into North Korea, differing preparatory devices were utilized, including various devices which Truman used to establish and maintain his legitimate position power (Raven, 1990). More recently, Gregg Gold and I have examined the strategies used by Winston Churchill in World War I1 to induce Franklin Roosevelt to give Britain 50-old-destroyers and commit the U.S. to more active alignment with Britain (Gold & Raven, 1992). Our data consist of a large volume of correspondence exchanged between Churchill and Roosevelt during this critical period. These psychohistorical analyses, which utilize qualitative approaches that are more characteristic of historians and political scientists, offer a dramatic view of the influence process, which, while less controlled, give a more complete and dynamic view of the social influence process. We conclude that we might all be much further ahead in analysis of wars, if, instead of the intense and meticulous analysis of military and battle strategy, equal intellectual effort was dedicated to the analysis of successful and unsuccessful social influence and power strategies between the major participants (Raven, 1990).
Development of Consistent Measures and Definitions

Podsakoff and Schreischeim (1985) carried out a very thorough review of field studies in organizational settings that utilized the French and Raven bases of power. Their review was very complimentary with regard to the impact that the bases of power approach had had on organizational research. However, they also had some very important and constructive criticisms and suggestions for further development. One of their major concerns was that many of the investigators were defining and conceptualizing the bases of power differently and that there were further problems with inconsistency in operational definitions. Thus, it was difficult to draw conclusions that integrate the results of these numerous studies. In order to at least maintain some consistency in conceptual definition in my own writings and those of people working with me, 1 have developed-and periodically revise-an extensive glossary of terms relating to interpersonal influence and social power (Raven, 1993). They also argued convincingly that measurements that have been used are often problematic. Many are based on single-item scales, which tend to be unreliable and do not sample the broad constructs defined in the bases of power. There is also a tendency to use ranking, ipsative scales, which may artifactually

246

Raven

minimize the prevalence of bases of power that are not highly ranked (Schriescheim, Hinkin, & Podsakoff, 1991). Clearly this is an area where development in research on the bases of power model is sorely needed. There have been several noteworthy attempts to remedy these measurement problems (e.g., Falbo, 1977; Frost & Stahelski, 1988; Hinkin & Schriescheim, 1989; Imai, 1989; Schriescheim, Hinkin, & Podsakoff, 1991). We are currently exploring a series of scales that include the original six bases of power, plus the personal forms of reward and coercion, and the three additional forms of legitimate power (equity, reciprocity, and responsibility). The initial data on these scales, which have now been tested in the United States, Brazil, Israel, and Germany are quite promising (Raven, Schwarzwald, & Koslowsky, in preparation).

Summary
In this article, I have attempted to trace the background and development of the bases of power approach that John R. P. French and I initiated so many years ago. The typology consisting of five (or six) bases of power is now described as the “best known framework” for studying social or interpersonal power (Mintzberg, 1983), and has, over the past few decades, “played a major role in the literature of social and industrial psychology” (Podsakoff & Schriesheim, 1985, p. 405). The purpose of this article is, first, to give due honor and recognition to John R. P. French for his important role in the development of this system of analysis, particularly in its critical seminal stages. Second, to give some idea of the vast range of problem areas to which this system has been applied. Third, to show how the French and Raven theoretical model has developed and been extended over the years, drawing upon and benefiting from other developments within social psychology and related fields. In doing so, I have presented a “Power/Interaction Model of Interpersonal Influence” in which the bases of social power occupy a central position. I have reviewed the influencing agent’s motivations, assessment of his or her power-related resources, examination of costs and benefits for their usage, selection of a power strategy, implementation of various stage-setting and preparatory devices to enhance the selected power strategy, and review of the success or failure of the power strategy. In the influence process, a feedback loop exists whereby the influencing agent may reconsider and reexamine relationships with the target and evaluate future power strategies. Having presented the Power/Interaction Model from the perspective of the influencing agent, I next alluded to a similar model from the perspective of the target of influence, and then discussed applications to conflict, bargaining, and negotiations, wherein both are either sequentially or simultaneously influencing agents and targets. Certainly we cannot claim to have written the final chapters in the study of social power and interpersonal influence, and there are,

T h e Bases of Power

247

of course, many important and significant works that have approached these issues from rather different directions. However, we hope that we have, at least, helped to develop some new directions and given additional meaning to an area of research and application that still stands among the most important in the field of social psychology. For his significant contributions to such developments, we owe special thanks to Jack French, for whose work in this and other areas he has most appropriately been honored with the Kurt Lewin Award.

References
Abdalla, I. A. H. (1987). Predictors of effectiveness of supervisory social power. Human Relations, 40, 721-740. Acton, J. E. E. D. (1948). Essays on power and freedom. Boston: Beacon. (Originally published in 1887). Ansari, M. A , , & Kapoor, A. (1987). Organizational context and upward influence tactics. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 40, 39-49. Berkowitz, L., & Daniels, L. R. (1963). Responsibility and dependence. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 66, 429-436. Broverman, I. K., Vogel, S. R., Broverman, D. M., Clarkson, F. E., & Rosenkrantz, P. S. (1972). Sex role stereotypes: A current appraisal. Journal of Social Issues, 28(2), 59-78. Busch, P., & Wilson, D. T. (1976). An experimental analysis of salesman’s expert and referent bases of power in the buyer-seller dyad. Journal of Marketing Research, 13, 3-1 1. Cartwright, D. (1959). Power: A neglected variable in social psychology. In D. Cartwright (Ed.), Studies in social power (pp. 1-14). Ann Arbor, MI: Institute for Social Research. Cartwright, D. (1965). Influence, leadership, and control. In J. G . March (Ed.), Handbook of organizations (pp. 1-47). Chicago: Rand McNally. Cobb, A. T. (1980). Informal influence in the formal organization: Perceived sources of power among work unit peers. Academy o Management Journal, 23, 155-161. f Coch, L., & French, J. R. P., Jr. (1948). Overcoming resistance to change. Human Relations, 1 , 512-532. Cope, R. G. (1972). Bases of power, administrative preference, and job satisfaction: A situational approach. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 2 , 457-465. Deutsch, M. (1973). The resolution of conflict: Constructive and destructive processes. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press. Falbo, T. (1977). Multidimensional scaling of power strategies. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 35, 537-547. Falbo, T., & Peplau, L. A. (1980). Power strategies in intimate relationships. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 38, 618-628. Festinger, L. (1950). Informal social communication. Psychological Review, 57, 27 1-292. Festinger, L. (1953). An analysis of compliant behavior. In M. Sherif & M. 0. Wilson (Eds.), Group relations at the crossroads (pp. 232-256). New York: Harper. Festinger, L. (1954). Theory of social comparison processes. Human Relations, 7, 117-140. Fisher, R., & Ury, W. (1981). Getting to yes. Boston: Houghton-Mifflin. French, J. R. P., Jr. (1956). A formal theory of social power. Psychological Review, 63, 181-194. French, J. R. P., Jr., & Raven, B. H. (1959). The bases of social power. In D. Cartwright (Ed.), Studies in social power (pp. 150-167). Ann Arbor, MI: Institute for Social Research. Frost, D. E., & Stahelski, A. J. (1988). The systematic measurement of French and Raven’s bases of social power in workgroups. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 18, 375-389. Gaski, J. F. (1986). Interrelations among a channel entity’s power sources: Impact of the exercises of reward and coercion on expert, referent, and legitimate power sources. Journal of Markering Research, 23, 62-77. Gold, G . I., & Raven, B. H. (1992). Interpersonal influence strategies in the Churchill-Roosevelt bases-for-destroyers exchange. Journal of Social Behavior and Personality, 7 , 245-272.

248

Raven

Goodchilds, J. D., & Raven, B. H. (1974). The measurement of power perception through cartoon strip completion. Paper presented at the Western Psychological Association Convention, San Francisco. Gouldner, A. W. (1960). The norm of reciprocity: A preliminary statement. American Sociological Review, 35, 161-178. Harary, F. (1959). A criterion of unanimity in French’s theory of social power. In D. Cartwright (Ed.), Studies in social power (pp. 168-182). Ann Arbor, MI: Institute for Social Research. Hinkin, T. R., & Schreischeim, C. A. (1990). Relations between subordinate perceptions of supervisor influence tactics and attributed bases of supervisory power. Human Relations, 43, 221237. Hovland, C. I., Janis, I. L., & Kelley, H. H. (1963). Communication andpersuasion. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press. Hunt, S . D., & Nevin, J. R. (1974). Power in a channel of distribution: Sources and consequences. Journal of Marketing Research, 1 1 , 186-193. Hurwitz, J. I., Zander, A. F., & Hymovitch, B. (1953). Some effects of power on relations among group members. In D. Cartwright & A. F. Zander (Eds.), Group dynamics: Research and theory (pp. 483-492). Evanston, IL: Row, Peterson. Imai, Y. (1989). The relationship between perceived social power and the perception of being influenced. Japanese Psychological Record, 31, 97-107. Jamieson, D. W., & Thomas, K. W. (1974). Power and conflict in the student-teacher relationship. Journal of Applied Behavioral Science, 10, 321-336. Janis, I. L., & Feshbach, S. (1953). Effects of fear-arousing communication. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 48, 78-92. Johnson, P. (1976a). Woman and interpersonal power. In I. Frieze, P. Johnson, J. E. Parsons, D. N. Ruble, & G. L. Zellman (Eds.), Women and sex roles: A socialpsychologicalperspective (pp. 301-320). New York: Norton. Johnson, P. (1976b). Women and power: Toward a theory of effectiveness. Journal of Social Issues, 32(3), 99-1 10. Jones, E. E., & Pittman, T. S . (1982). Toward a general theory of strategic interaction. In J. Suls (Ed.), Psychological perspectives o self (Vol. 1, pp. 231-263). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. f Kabanoff, B. (1985). Potential influence structure as sources of interpersonal conflict in groups and organizations. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 36, 113- 141. Kipnis, D. (1976). The powerholders. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press. Kruglanski, A. W. (1970). Some mechanisms of attributing trustworthiness in supervisor-worker relations. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 6, 214-232. Lawler, E. J., Ford, R. S . , & Blegen, M. A. (1988). Coercive capability in conflict: A test of bilateral deterrence vs. conflict spiral theory. Social Psychology Quarterly, 5 1 , 93-107. Lewin, K. (1935). A dynamic theory of personality. New York: McGraw Hill. Lewin, K. (1947). Frontiers in group dynamics. Human Relations, 1 , 5-41. Lewin, K. (1951). Constructs in field theory. In K. Lewin (D. Cartwright, Ed.), Field theory in social science: Selected theoretical papers (pp. 30-42). New York Harper. (Originally published in 1944.) Lewin, K. (1952). Group decision and social change. In G. E. Swanson, T. M. Newcomb, & E. L. Hartley (Eds.), Readings in social psychology (2nd ed., pp. 459-473). New York: Holt, Rinehart, and Winston. Lewin, K., Lippitt, R . , & White, R. K. (1939). Patterns of aggressive behavior in experimentally f created “social climates.” Journal o Social Psychology, 10, 271-299. Lippitt, R., Polansky, N., Redl, F., & Rosen, S. (1952). The dynamics of power. Human Relations, 5 , 37-64. McCormick, N. B. (1979). Come-ons and put-offs: Unmamed students’ strategies for having and avoiding sexual intercourse. Psychology o Women Quarterly, 4 , 194-21 1. f MeliB-Navarro, J. L., & Peir6-Silla, J. R. (1984). Perception de las relaciones de poder en ambientes organizaciones: Estudio empinco e implicaciones para un disefio de estructura de poder. [The perception of power relations in ambient organizations: An empirical study and implications for a model of the structure of power.] Proceedings o 1 Congress0 del Colegio Oficial de f Psicologos (pp. 135-144). Madrid, Spain.

The Bases of Power

249

Merton, R. K. (1957). Social theory and social structure (2nd ed.). Glencoe, IL: Free Press. Mintzberg, H. (1983). Power in and around organizations. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall. Newcomb, T. M. (1958). Attitude development as a function of reference groups. In E. E. Maccoby, T. M. Newcomb, & E. L. Hartley (Eds.), Readings in social psychology (3rd ed., pp. 265275). New York: Holt. Podsakoff, P. M., & Schriescheim, C. A. (1985). Field studies of French and Raven’s bases of social power: Critique, reanalysis, and suggestions for future research. Psychological Bulletin, 97, 387-41 1. Pruitt, D. G . (1981). Negotiation behavior. New York: Academic Press. Rasinski, K., Tyler, T. R., & Fridkin, K. (1985). Exploring the function of legitimacy: Mediating effects of personal and institutional legitimacy on leadership endorsement and system support. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 49, 395-407. Raven, B. H. (1965). Social influence and power. In I. D. Steiner & M. Fishbein (Eds.), Current studies in social psychology (pp. 371-381). New York Holt, Rinehart, Winston. Raven, B. H. (1983). Interpersonal influence and social power. In B. H. Raven & J. Z. Rubin (Eds.), Social psychology (pp. 399-444). New York: Wiley. Raven, B. H . (1988). Social power and compliance in health care. In S. Maes, C. D. Spielberger, P. B. Defares, & I. G. Sarason (Eds.), Topics in health psychology (pp. 229-244). London/New York: Wiley. Raven, B. H. (1990). Political applications of the psychology of interpersonal influence and social power. Political Psychology, I I , 493-520. Raven, B. H. (1992). A power/interaction model of interpersonal influence: French and Raven thirty years later. Journal of Social Behavior and Personality, 7 , 217-244. Raven, B. H. (1993). A glossary of terms related to interpersonal influence and social power. Unpublished manuscript. University of California, Los Angeles, Department of Psychology, Los Angeles, CA. Raven, B. H., Centers, R., & Rodrigues, A. (1975). The bases of conjugal power. In R. S. Cromwell & D. H. Olson (Eds.), Power infamilies (pp. 217-234). New York Sage. Raven, B. H . , Freeman, H. E., & Haley, R. W. (1982). Social power and compliance in hospital infection control. In A. W. Johnson, 0. Grusky, & B. H. Raven (Eds.), Contemporary health services: Social science perspectives (pp. 139- 176). Boston: Auburn House. Raven, B . H . , & Kruglanski, A. W. (1970). Conflict and power. In P. G . Swingle (Ed.), The structure of conflict (pp. 69-109). New York Academic Press. Raven, B . H., & Litman-Adizes, T. (1986). Interpersonal influence and social power in health promotion. In Z. Salisbury, S. Kar, & J. Zapka (Eds.) Advances in health education and promotion (pp. 181-210). Greenwich, CT: JAI Press. Raven, B. H., Schwarzwald, J., & Koslowsky, M. (in preparation). Conceptualization of Raven’s Powerllnteraction model of interpersonal influence. Rodin, J., & Janis, I. L. (1982). The social influence of physicians and other health care practitioners as agents of change. In H. S. Friedman & R. M. DiMatteo (Eds.), Interpersonal issues in health care (pp. 33-50). New York: Academic Press. Rollins, B. C . , & Thomas, D. W. (1975). A theory of parental power and compliance. In R. E. Cromwell & D. H. Olson (Eds.), Power in families (pp. 38-60). New York: Sage. Schlenker, B. R. (1980). Impression management: The self-concept. social identity, and interpersonal relations. Monterey, CA: BrooksICole. Schmidt, A,, & Raven, B. H. (1985a). Children’s cognitions of persuasion strategies and compliance. Paper presented at the Biennial Meeting of the Society for Research in Child Development, Toronto. Schmidt, A,, & Raven, B. H. (1985b. August). Developmental changes in perception ofpersuasive strategies in children. Paper presented at the 93rd Annual Convention of the American Psychological Association, Los Angeles. Schmidt, A,, Raven, B. H., Pastorelli, C., & Caprara, G. V. (1993). Uses ofpower in social influence among peers: A four-country study. Paper presented at the Biennial Meeting of the Society for Research in Child Development, New Orleans. Schmidt, A,, Yanagihura, A., & Smith, R. (1987). Percepcidn del us0 y 10s resultados de las bases del poder por 10s nirios de tres culturas: Estados Unidos, Japdn y Argentina. (Perception of

250

Raven

the use and effects of the bases of power for children in three cultures: United States, Japan, and Argentina). Paper presented at the XXI Interamerican Congress of Psychology, Havana, Cuba. Schriesheim, C. A., Hinkin, T. R., & Podsakoff, P. M. (1991). Can ipsative and single-item measures produce erroneous results in field studies of French and Raven’s (1959) five bases of f power? An empirical investigation. Journal o Applied Psychology, 76, 106-1 14. Shaw, I. I . , & Condelli, L. (1986). Effects of compliance outcome and basis of power on the powerholder-target relationship. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 12, 236-246. Sherif, M. (1936). The psychology of social norms. New York: Harper. Sinha, J. B. P., & Singh-Sengupta, S. (1991). Relationship between managers’ power and the perception of their non-managers’ behavior. Indian Journal of Industrial Relations, 26, 341351. Stahelski, A. J., Frost, D. E., & Patch, M. E. (1989). Use of socially dependent bases of power: French and Raven’s theory applied to workgroup leadership. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 19, 283-297. Stein, L. I. (1971). Male and female: The doctor-nurse game. In J. P. Spradley & D. W. McCurdy (Eds.), Conformity and conflict: Readings in cultural anthropology (pp. 185- 193). Boston: Little-Brown. Strickland, L. H. (1958). Surveillance and trust. Journal of Personality, 26, 200-215. f Student, K. R. (1968). Supervisory influence and work group performance. Journal o Applied fSychology, 52, 188-194. Sutton, S. R. (1982). Fear-arousing communications: A critical examination of theory and research. In J. R. Eiser (Ed.), Social psychology and behavioural medicine (pp. 307-337). New York and London: Wiley. Tashakkori, A. T., Thompson, V. D., & Simonian, L. (1989). Adolescents’ attributions of parental power: A re-examination of the ‘theory of resources’ in cultural context. International Journal o Psychology, 24, 13-96. f Ury, W. (1991). Getting past no. New York: Bantam. Walster (Hatfield), E., & Festinger, L. (1962). The effectiveness of “overheard” persuasive communications. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 65, 395-402. Walster (Hatfield), E., Walster, G. W., & Berscheid, E. (1978). Equity theory and research. Boston: Allyn & Bacon. Weber, M. (1947). The theory o social and economic organization. Oxford: Oxford University f Press. White, R. K . (1988). Specifics in a positive approach to peace. Journal o Social Issues, 44(2), 191f 202. Winter, D. G. (1973). The power motive. New York: Free Press. Wrong, D. H. (1980). Power: Its forms, bases, and uses. New York: Harper & Row. Zander, A. F., & Cohen, A. R. (1955). Attributed social power and group acceptance: A classroom experimental demonstration. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 5 1 , 490-492. Zander, A. F., Cohen, A. R., & Stotland, E. (1959). Power and relations among professions. In D. Cartwright (Ed.), Srudies in socialpower (pp. 15-34). Ann Arbor, MI: Institute for Social Research. Zimbardo, P. G., Haney, C., Banks, W. C., & Jaffe, D. (1973, April 8). A Pirandellian prison: The mind is a formidable jailer. New York Times Magazine, pp. 38-60.

BERTRAM H. RAVEN is Professor of Psychology at the University of California, Los Angeles. He received his Ph.D. in Social Psychology from the University of Michigan. While there, he was associated with the Research Center for Group Dynamics and worked closely with John R. P. French in the development of the Bases of Social Power model which is associated with their names. Though he has carried out research in various areas of social psychology (group performance; social comparison and self-evaluation; interdependence in group

The Bases of Power

25 1

problem solving; cognitive dissonance and change in attitudes, beliefs, and behavior), it is theory and research in social power that has occupied his major attention. In addition to theoretical development, he has applied the basic theory to issues in health, education, and political behavior. Dr. Raven has been an active member of SPSSI; among other roles, he has served as both Editor of the Journal ofSociul Issues (1970- 1973) and President of the Society (1973-1974). He is currently working on a book that will present in greater detail the Power/Interaction Model of Interpersonal Influence that is discussed in this article.

Similar Documents

Premium Essay

Power Bases

...together disparate themes into a coherent narratives no other politician of his generation. As ever, Clinton is generous to his friends and ruthless toward his enemies. Legitimate Power: As we speak he is no longer the president of the United States and for the same reason he does not have a legitimate power, but when he was The President of the United States his power base was laid down by the Constitution and by the people that elected him as President, having at that moment Legitimate Power. Referent Power: Bill has the power of interpersonal attraction and today as we speak, the place to hear him think out loud is at the annual Clinton Global Initiative conference in New York on March 30 - April 1, 2012, so for being a good team leader, having charisma, being is fair, doing good in everything that he does and have the power to gather hundreds of heads of state, business man, nonprofit executives, academics, and even Hollywood stars, not just to talk about the world's problems but to do something about them, and base on this, I am confident to say that he has a Reference Power. Expert Power Base: Throughout the articles that I read there were some wise phrases that Bill Clinton said and that call my attention to share it, and from my point of view this make reference to the Expert Power Base, base on the knowledge that he achieved throughout his mandate at the presidency of the United State when he travel around the world. With his knowledge he express his ideas and even...

Words: 446 - Pages: 2

Free Essay

Bases of Power

...BASES  OF  INDIVIDUAL   POWER   (1)LEGITIMATE  POWER   q Also  called  ‘Authority’,  it’s  derived  from  a  persons   posiLon  in  an  organizaLon   q OrganizaLons  judgment  about  who  is  formally   permiRed  to  influence  whom?   q Some  people  are  more  likely  to  invoke  than  others.   …”Look,  I  am  the  boss  here”.   q One  extreme  is  military.  Chain  of  command.  Other   extreme  could  be  universiLes.  Tend  to  downplay   differences  between  Dean,  prof,  lecturer  etc.   q When  authority  works,  it  o\en  does  so,  because   people  have  accepted  it.     (2)  Reward  Power   q Means  that  the  power  holder  can  influence  by   providing  a  posiLve  outcome.   q Reward  power  o\en  back  up  legiLmate  power.  I.e.   managers  are  given  the  chance  to  give  raises  etc.   q Of  course  any  organizaLonal  member  can  aRempt  to   exert  power  over  others  by  praise,  compliments  and   flaRery,  which  are  also  rewards.   (3)Coercive  Power   q Means  that  the  power  holder  can  influence...

Words: 328 - Pages: 2

Free Essay

Power Bases

...Group Dynamics Writing Assignment 5 Power Bases 07/27/12 Mitt Romney started out as a Republican front runner in the 2012 presidential race. Winning Maine and Nevada early, he fell to Rick Santorum in Colorado, Missouri and Minnesota. Don’t let the fact that Mitt Romney won past CPAC Straw Polls, which are much more a beauty contest then the 3 caucuses last week that Santorum clobbered Romney in, with the Romney campaign referring to a couple of them as beauty contests themselves. With Romney’s money, busing in tons of college students and picking up the tab for their attendance AND STRAW POLL vote, and that is exactly what he did. Even Santorum jumped in on that and commented that he doesn’t rig straw polls. According to the article I read, Will Romney’s ‘Good Enough’ Strategy be Good Enough for Voters? By Chris Stirewalt, Published February 15, 2012, at FoxNews.com, Romney and his Super-PAC can defeat Santorum in the Republican race because Romney has a stronger hold in the Republican base. They trust him more and feel he is the strongest of the candidates thus far. However, he has a problem with flip-flopping on the issues and is seen by many of his Republican base as a weak candidate. Nevertheless, if you are a Republican, the vibes are very bad. Mitt Romney has shown a discouraging inability to appeal to the party’s base, while the race has damaged both Romney and the party. Newt Gingrich, in particular, sacrificed the party to his own ego by launching left-wing attacks...

Words: 553 - Pages: 3

Premium Essay

Bases of Power

...Bases of Power There are five bases of power in the given organization. The first base of power is reward power. Reward power is defined as being the opposite of coercive power and that “people comply with the wishes or directives of another because doing so produces positive benefits” (Robbins & Judge, 2007, pg. 471.) Employee 1 in the scenario wants to receive the bonus that he/she will be granted upon the successful completion of his/her annual evaluation. To ensure his/her tasks are finished and that they are correct, Employee 1 oftentimes works over his/her scheduled 40 hour work week by working late and on weekends at the encouragement of the marketing manager. The marketing manager often reminds the Employee 1 and his/her peers of the yearly bonus. The employee complying with the wishes of the marketing manager for he/she to work late so he/she will receive a good performance evaluation, thus resulting in the receipt of the desired bonus is an example of Employee 1 being affected by reward power. The marketing manager uses the second base of power, which is legitimate power. Legitimate power is defined as “the formal authority to control and use organizational resources” (Robbins & Judge, 2007, pg. 472.) The marketing manager being the person responsible for the evaluation of the employees, thus affecting whether or not Employee 1 receives the bonus he/she really wants is a demonstration of this power. The distribution of a bonus to an employee is an example of...

Words: 1000 - Pages: 4

Premium Essay

The Five Bases of Power

...The Five Bases of Power BCOM/230 The Five Bases of Power “Power refers to a capacity that A has to influence the behavior of B so that B acts in accordance with A’s wishes. This definition implies a potential that need not be actualized to be effective, and a dependency relationship.” (Robbins & Judge, 2009, "A Definition of Power"). In total, there are five bases of power. They are coercive power, reward power, legitimate power, expert power, and referent power. Each power is different in some way which makes it unique. “The coercive power base is dependent on fear. A person reacts to this power out of fear of the negative results that might occur if she failed to comply. It rests on the application, or the threat of application, of physical sanctions such as the infliction of pain, the generation of frustration through restriction of movement, or the controlling by force of basic physiological or safety needs.” (Robbins & Judge, 2009, "Bases of Power"). Coercive power exists when people are frightened of what could happen to them if they do not give into the leader’s wishes. An extreme example of coercive power would be Hitler’s power in Germany when he ruled the country. He put true fear into people that did not agree with him, this made him gain followers whether those followers were really in agreement with him or not. “The opposite of coercive power is reward power. People comply with the wishes or directives of another because doing so produces positive...

Words: 1097 - Pages: 5

Premium Essay

5 Bases of Power

...assignment, determine which Base of Power is being demonstrated, as well as provide my justifications for that determination. The 5 Bases of Power, described by social psychologists French and Raven, are as follows (Bases of Social Power - French Raven, including list below): 1. Reward Power - power given based on the perception that you can reward a person, or remove negative consequences 2. Coercive Power - power given based on the perception that you can punish a person 3. Legitimate Power - power given based on your appointment to the position, and the authority that grants you. 4. Referent Power - power given based upon perceived charm, charisma, and likability. 5. Expert Power - power given you based on recognition of your ability, skills, experience, etc. In scenario 1, the manager is demonstrating Reward Power. Per the assignment: " The marketing manager encourages employees to work beyond the requisite 40 hours a week by reminding them of the yearly bonus for receiving a superior rating on their next evaluation." Employees go the extra mile, and make sure work is accurate, based entirely upon the expectation of a bonus. There is no negative implication stated, so I do not think Coercive Power is being demonstrated. In addition, the manager is demonstrating legitimate power, in that his position is used to exercise control over employee's behavior based upon his appointment as the manager. In scenario 2, I believe that there are two bases of power being demonstrated. First...

Words: 974 - Pages: 4

Premium Essay

Five Bases of Power

...Behavior & Leadership Five Bases of Power – Task 3 Tammy Lydeen Western Governors University January 21, 2011 Within an organization leaders and managers use their power in order for their employees to reach the organization’s goals. There are five bases of power which can be broken down into two groups: formal power and personal power. Along with the five bases of power there is also the power of dependency which states that Employee B depends on Employee A if Employee B has goals and needs that Employee A can fulfill. Dependency can be increased when the resources that are being controlled are important, scarce and non-substitutable. Formal Power A formal power is based on an employee’s position within the organization. v Coercive Power – A coercive power requires forcing an employee to do something that they do no want to do. This power can cause an unhealthy behavior and employee dissatisfaction in the workplace. The most common threat is the threat of being demoted or fired. Although the most common threats are being demoted or fired the marketing manager also uses a coercive power when they tell the employees that their yearly evaluation and bonus can be effected if they do not work over the required 40-hour work week. Employee 1 needs to work the extra hours so that they can achieve the yearly bonus and take a vacation that would not be affordable without the end of the year bonus. v Reward Power – A reward power is when the leader uses their...

Words: 797 - Pages: 4

Premium Essay

The Five Bases of Power

...Power is defined as a capacity that one person (or group) has to influence the behavior of another person (or group) so that the second person acts according to how the first person wants. There are two general sources of power, formal and personal; those sources of power can be broken down into different bases of power. Formal power can be coercive power, reward power, or legitimate power; personal power can be expert power or referent power (Robbins & Judge, 2009). Each form of power will be discussed on an individual basis to determine how the power affects communications within an organization. Formal power is based on an individual’s position within an organization; the individual gains power through the ability to coerce or reward, or from formal authority. “Coercive power is dependent on fear, suppression of free will, or the use of punishment for existence” (“Coercive power,” 2012). Others in an organization also use coercive power by withholding important information; knowing this important information has others dependent on them. As an employee, I feared losing my job when I was not moving as fast as my manager wanted me to during lunch rush. My manager had told me previously that I was not fast enough at assembling sandwiches as others; I was told frequently that this was a cause to be fired if I did not improve my speed. I did not let that manager have coercive power over me for long; I had another employee help me learn ways to speed up my sandwich making...

Words: 1094 - Pages: 5

Premium Essay

Five Bases of Power

...as the five bases of power. These bases of power were the foundation for many of the discussions involving power and authority in the late half of the 20th century. The five bases of power are coercive, legitimate, reward, referent, and expert. The bases can be utilized by themselves or combined with another base when demonstrating power in any situation. This article will explore each of the five bases and describe them more in depth, including formality and informality of the bases. Coercive power, one of the first bases of power, carries with it a manifestation of fear based on the possible negative outcomes of this power and if the subject did not comply with the orders given. Coercive power is the power that comes from a person's authority to punish. This formal base of power is effective in the short term only; and in contrast more often leads to rebellion against authority rather than respect and compliance based on respect. A manager could use coercive power in an instance where an employee was failing to perform his job correctly or efficiently, by threatening the employee with disciplinary action if the issue is not rectified. This base of power creates a general feeling of fear and uneasiness among employees and can be detrimental to the morale of the employees and the company. When employees share a general consensus that a manager or supervisor has a right to give out orders because of this job title and company position this formal base of power is referred...

Words: 872 - Pages: 4

Premium Essay

Five Bases of Power

...Five Bases of Power Student Name LET1 (Task 3) WGU August 25, 2013 Course Mentor Name Abstract Power can be defined in many ways from various people. Some people may think power means success, some people may think power means corruption, and unfortunately some people may think power means nothing. Basically power pertains to the possession of influence and authority over others. The five bases of power are coercive, referent, legitimate, expert, and reward power (Abudi, 2011). To understand these five bases of power better in an organization a description with examples for each will be discussed. Five Bases of Power In 1960 John French and Bertram Raven conducted a study in leadership roles on bases of power Coercive, referent, legitimate, expert, and reward power were identified as the five bases of power. Through one or more of these bases, power can be manifested. Most interactions in an organization are established with trust and are constructed through some form of power. Depending on how power is used can result in negative or positive outcomes in an organization. The proper use of power among managers and employees can result in excellent social and interpersonal relationships (Abudi, 2011). In the given scenario, Corporation A is a business that creates marketing programs for the real estate industry. The employees with different job roles that work at Corporation...

Words: 1008 - Pages: 5

Premium Essay

Bases of Power Paper

...Communication and Bases of Power Student Name BCOM/230 Date: September 9, 2013 Instructor Name Communication and Bases of Power Power influences our behavior and the way we communicate in the workplace. Power is defined as the affected behavior of one person from the influence of another person with higher authority. Depending on how the power is used, it may have a positive or negative effect in an organization. According to Robbins and Judge (2009), there are five bases of power that can influence individuals or groups in the workplace; they are coercive power, reward power, legitimate power, expert power, and referent power (p. 452). Coercive power is a formal kind of power that I believe is the most negative among the other forms of power. The person who uses this kind of power places fears to subordinates; therefore, I believe communication from group members to leaders is limited by electronic means, like email, because members are intimidated by the coercive power of the leader. If the individual does not comply with the company’s policies, norms or does not reach a goal desired by management, then the fear of being fired, demoted, or punished is in place. The person is intimidated by threads from management. I find that threads of been fired due to lack of compliance is a delicate approach to change the behaviors of others. In these cases, members may be displeased of the thread and may start looking for other positions outside of the company; hence the...

Words: 938 - Pages: 4

Premium Essay

The Five Bases of Power

...The Five Bases of Power Julie Kisman BCOM/230 April 20, 2015 Gloria Davis The Five Bases of Power All too often, we are stuck in a working environment the majority of our day, years, and lives, of people who have different personalities and governing styles than we do. “Power means many different things to different people. For some, power is seen as corrupt. For others, the more power they have, the more successful they feel. For even others, power is of no interest at all (Abudi, 2013).” If we can learn to recognize our superiors leadership styles that will put us so much closer to developing a better working relationship with that person. Let’s review these forms of power with real working experience in each and be that much closer to identifying with our employer. Formal Power Coercive Power Coercive Power is a leadership style that is dependent on fear. Scheduling is a very real example of coercive power in my place of work. The law states, that if you fall below 20 hours in a working week, you will be removed from the schedule until you can prove your availability. Robots do not write schedules, or individual employees, but rather an actual human, with human emotions. Say you have a disagreement with a schedule writer, or hurt their feelings, they, unfortunately, have the power to reduce your hours, eventually having you removed from the schedule entirely. In this situation, the department head who is writing the schedule, is using their coercive power...

Words: 807 - Pages: 4

Premium Essay

Five Bases of Power

...Five Bases of Power Reward Power -the ability of a manager to give some sort of reward to employees Employee 1, work ethics shows that she is driven, loyal, focused and goal oriented. She understands the concept of reward power since she has worked there for 12 years and her hard work has earned her a yearly bonus. Coercive Power - the ability of a manager to force an employee to comply with an order (directly or inadvertently) through the threat of punishment or penalty. The Marketing Manager encourages employees to work more than forty hours per week. He reminds the employees that if they work more hours they could receive a superior evaluation and a yearly bonus. This is coercive behavior because in order for them to receive a bonus, they must have a superior evaluation, which requires them to work more than 40 hours per week. Since most Real Estate Agents are commissioned employees, many of them have second jobs or family commitments that will not allow them to work more than forty hours. In addition, some employees maybe more successful working 30 hours as opposed to another who works 50 hours a week, however, according to the Marketing Manager the employee who worked fewer than 40 hours wouldn’t receive a superior evaluation or a bonus. The Marketing Manager is utilizing coercive power by asking the employees to do more for the company in order to receive a bonus. Expertise Power - Ability to influence others based on expertise and knowledge As the CPA of...

Words: 450 - Pages: 2

Free Essay

5 Bases of Power

...In the given scenario it is evident that Employee 1 works for a manager who utilizes Reward Power. This employee works more than 40 hours per week because the manager reminds the team of the yearly bonus which is contingent on working more than the required 40 hours. Employee 1 comes into the office on the weekends and stays late to ensure that work is completed accurately. Employee 1 has plans for the reward and has emotionally tied himself to working towards the reward. The other powers that this manager demonstrates are Coercive Power and Legitimate Power. The way he reminds them of the annual performance review is coercive and threatening. Lastly because he is the manager he does have Legitimate Power and the team must respect and listen to his instruction. Employee 2 has Expert Power in the organization. Because he is the only CPA in the organization he has been given special permission to have a compressed work week and only has to work 4 days per week. This employee realizes that he has Expert Power and has taken advantage of this power especially since the entire company is dependent on only him to prepare the company’s financial statements. Employee 3 demonstrates Personal Power. Although he does not hold any official titles and has been at the company for a short period. This employee is well liked and respected among his peers. People are drawn to his personality and charisma. Because he is enthusiastic and positive he pitched a new idea that the team is eager...

Words: 416 - Pages: 2

Premium Essay

Bases of Power Let 1

...11 Bases of Power According to Robbins & Judge (2007) power is defined as the “capacity that A has to influence the behavior of B so B can act with A’s wishes.” There are five bases of power that are divided into two groups according to Robbins & Judge (2007). Formal Power The first group in the power in the power bases is formal. Formal power is based on the position held in organization (Robbins & Judge (2007). Formal Power is divided into three power bases: coercive power, reward power and legitimate power (Robbins & Judge, 2007). Coercive Power Coercive power is based on the fear of consequences when the person does not follow instructions of their leader. This type of fear can be a physical or mental pain discomfort based on the frustrations of the unknown. Conceive power can be displayed by threats of loosing job and privileges, being demoted and other types of consequential actions. Coercive power could also be used to conceal key information (Robbins & Judge, 2007). Employee 2 could possibly use conceive power based on how valuable he is his position to get more pay or even better schedule than the one he requested or other job advantages. Reward Power Reward power is power one has to reward another person. Some examples of rewards could be monetary, promotions, and recognitions. According to Robbins & Judge (2007) coercive power and reward power are counterparts of each other. Rewarding for good deeds is using reward power, and...

Words: 841 - Pages: 4