Free Essay

Learning and Knowledge

In:

Submitted By ktm3006270
Words 7525
Pages 31
Running head: KNOWLEDGE DOMAINS AND DOMAIN LEARNING

Knowledge Domains and Domain Learning
Liliana Maggioni and Patricia A. Alexander
University of Maryland

In press

Keywords: discipline, domain, learning, history, knowledge, mathematics, reading, science, writing.
Abstract
The roots of current disciplines and domains of study reach well back in history. An exploration of their development shows that these areas of knowledge have not only reflected cultural changes but have also influenced societies, especially through formal educational systems. Besides being characterized by their focus on a particular part of the world, disciplines are also distinguished by a specific way of thinking about their respective domains of study. Psychological research has identified several features of these pathways to knowledge (e.g., reading, writing, history, mathematics, and science) that generally define the landscape of academic practice.
Although educational psychologists’ interest in how knowledge develops within specific domains has fluctuated across time, formal education throughout the world is organized around particular fields of studies and students soon come to characterize their experience of school in terms of subject matters. Thus, in education, domains seem to make a difference. The term knowledge domain refers to the part of the world investigated by a specific discipline. In other words, the domain can be characterized as the object (e.g., plants, numbers, or the past) of a specific body of knowledge (e.g., botany, mathematics, or history). Herein, we offer a survey of the development of increasingly specialized disciplines and of the corresponding identification of ever narrower knowledge domains. In particular, we consider the cultural trends that accompanied these changes and the major influences on the structure of formal education. Then, we turn to consider the characteristics of learning and generating knowledge within specific domains. Specifically, we consider the findings of educational research in understanding the processes of learning and teaching in history, mathematics, and science. We also include contributions regarding the processes of reading and writing, given their relevance in the school curriculum and the tendency in educational research to conceptualize these activities as domains of learning in their own right.
The Development of Disciplines Besides being characterized by its systematic knowledge of a particular domain, each discipline is also distinguished by a specific way of thinking about associated phenomena. Thus, disciplinary knowledge includes a specific taxonomy, vocabulary, concepts, theories, research methods, and standards of justification. Histories of science, such as those offered by Noah Edward Fehl (1966), A. Rupert Hall and Marie Boas Hall (1988), and Walter Libby (1917) illustrate that disciplines developed over the centuries as privileged pathways toward understanding of almost any topic. The reciprocal influences between the prevalent cultural climate, disciplinary developments, and formal education are well exemplified by cultural histories of Western education, such as the one composed by Freeman Butt (1955). These studies concur in affirming that organized bodies of knowledge arose in conjunction with the human needs of gaining understanding of the world, establishing some control on the physical environment, and organizing social life. In ancient Egypt, for example, efforts at controlling and predicting the floods of the Nile fostered remarkable advances in geometry and astronomy. However, it was in the Greek cities of Asia Minor and later in Athens that the investigation of philosophers into the nature and origin of the universe introduced the method of rational inquiry that so deeply influenced the development of knowledge in Western civilization. In fact, Western thought owes much of its systematization of knowledge to the Greeks and to their reliance on critical reason in speculating about the origin and nature of the universe. The Greeks came to regard rationality as the human faculty allowing the acquisition of knowledge and truth; hence, the intellectual formation afforded by mathematics and philosophy was considered a privileged road to knowledge and learning and acquired centrality in education. In time, increased complexity in the knowledge and skills characterizing competence in specific areas fostered the development of formal instruction and the establishment of schools as separate institutions. The process of organization of knowledge was greatly favored by reliance on the written form, a practice relatively soon adopted by the liberal arts, but not by the practical arts. In the practical arts, informal apprenticeship remained the prevalent way of passing technical skills from one generation to the next. On the other hand, formal education tended to focus on those bodies of knowledge that were systematically organized in written form. The importance of accurate definitions in furthering thoughts and the power accorded in Athenian life to those who could speak effectively prompted the development of grammar and rhetoric. The need to think clearly encouraged the development of logic or dialectics. In addition, Greek philosophy began to organize into bodies of knowledge studies regarding the ultimate reality of things (metaphysics), the theory of knowledge (epistemology), human nature and human conduct (ethics, political science, economics, sociology, and psychology), the physical world (astronomy, geography, physics, mechanics, hydraulics, mineralogy, botany), and the living world (zoology, physiology, anatomy). The search for intellectual discipline and the problems investigated by the developing sciences favored the progress in the field of mathematics and the refinement and systematization of theories and concepts in arithmetic, geometry, and trigonometry. In addition, Greek inquiry extended to the human past, investigating the causes of historical events and thus laying the foundation of history. Finally, although the fine arts were not organized into systematic bodies of knowledge, the Greeks cultivated several forms of literary criticism, thus giving systematic organization to the theory of art and aesthetics. Roman culture widely drew from Greek thought, furthering the systematization of knowledge and its organization in forms suitable for teaching. By the end of the fourth century, the liberal arts had been circumscribed to the study of compendia (written in Latin) of those Greek works deemed suitable to the spiritual and intellectual development of the pupils. Specifically, the Trivium, that is, the elementary liberal art curriculum included grammar, rhetoric, and logic. The higher liberal arts, or Quadrivium, incorporated mainly mathematical studies and comprehended arithmetic, geometry, astronomy, and music. The boundaries of these disciplines did not necessarily include the same content that they comprise in modern times. For example, grammar also involved the study of poetry and literature. In addition, the content reflected changes in the Roman intellectual climate. For instance, the purpose of rhetoric changed from preparation to active participation in the debates over public policies to the study of elegant language to be employed in public celebrations. At the same time, the Romans did not tend to value the sciences for their own sake, but applied the theoretical knowledge gained from the Greeks in geometry, astronomy, and natural philosophy to the solution of practical problems. The disciplines included in the Trivium and the Quadrivium continued to constitute the backbone of knowledge during the Middle Ages. However, many changes were made in each of the seven disciplines in response to the varying needs and interests of the time. Latin increasingly became the language of educated people; hence, grammar gained preponderance among the elementary liberal arts during the Early Middle Ages when the non-Latin people began to take part in the intellectual life of Europe. Nonetheless, Medieval Latin became increasingly different from classical Latin, reflecting the influence of the various languages spoken by the European peoples. Rhetoric lost most of its celebratory purpose and focused on the use of the written language for drawing up legal and feudal documents. Logic was increasingly identified with the rules of deductive thinking and, as such, became distinguished from philosophy. Arithmetic, geometry, and astronomy saw important developments during the Middle Ages. The cultural exchanges with the Arab world and the translation into Latin of the most important Greek, Arabs, and Hindus works laid the foundations for the scientific and mathematical investigations of later centuries. Within the Quadrivium, music retained its theoretical nature, even if its performance gained importance in medieval life. Although knowledge of the Greek tradition was praised by Christian philosophers for the contribution it gave to truth, and thus as an aid to the understanding of God and the soul, the intellectualism of pursuing knowledge for knowledge’s sake was rejected. In the Benedictine tradition, where the monastery became the home of practical agriculturalists, as well as of religious, artists, and scholars, the alliance between learning and concrete reality foreshadowed the importance of factual knowledge and the relation between science and technology that came to characterize modern science. Beginning in the twelfth century, teachers and students began to organize themselves into guilds for protections against the king, the bishop, or anyone else who tried to control them. In time, the term universitas began to refer specifically to guilds of teachers (faculties) and students. Reflecting the articulation of knowledge of the time, the typical guilds of teachers were the faculties of liberal arts, law, medicine, and theology. Specific universities became famous for one specific faculty, thus deeply influencing the development of a particular discipline. In addition, the university system fostered an expansion of the liberal arts curriculum, adding the works of Aristotle on the physical sciences, ethics, politics, and metaphysics. The system also promoted the institutionalization of the educational curriculum (with its degrees, licenses to teach, exams, and titles) and thus the grouping of studies into separate faculties. The Renaissance celebrated the return to the classics (Latin, Greeks, and Hebrew) and highly regarded rhetoric as a way to cultivate polite letters and expression. Logic fell in disrepute due to the humanist opposition to scholasticism. However, these changes had a deeper effect on the content of the Trivium and Quadrivium than on the disciplines taught in the universities, where medieval philosophy conserved its predominance. In the sciences, humanists turned away from the deductive methods of argumentation and advocated the use of the inductive method (observations of facts and generalization). The belief that the method of induction is the proper method to gain scientific knowledge is at the root of the classification of human knowledge provided by Francis Bacon in the seventeenth century. Here the disciplines came to be characterized mainly as “histories” of nature, collections of descriptions regarding a vast array of natural phenomena. At the same time, a method common to all sciences began to take shape. The scientist should observe nature, collect facts, identify their common qualities, and express these similarities in general formulas. Empiricism highlighted the patient work of scientists in acquiring facts. However, empiricism dismissed the guiding role of theory in deciding observations and experiments. Although the almost exclusive reliance on induction disappeared in later work on method by Descartes, the uniqueness of the scientific method to gain knowledge was not challenged. Actually, the certitude granted by mathematics was upheld as the goal of scientific knowledge, and the scientific method was considered applicable and appropriate to all fields of human thought. Whereas in the physical sciences this new method of gaining knowledge proved compatible with the emergence of a unitary principle of explanation, i.e., the mechanism, a close relation between abstract thought and scientific investigation failed to surface in the biological sciences. The complexities involved in studying living things and the philosophical impossibility of reconciling the existence of human soul with a completely mechanistic physiology supported the specificity of different bodies of knowledge. The search for natural laws was extended to the study of society and political economy during the era of Enlightenment. The rules for scientific data gathering began to be applied also in the field of the social sciences and historians began to identify progress as the fundamental law of history. This process was furthered by the role played by Darwinism in the biological science, where the process of change assumed a central explanatory role. From biology, the idea that change is an inherent part of natural and human development influenced the social sciences and their methodological approach, which increasingly tried to emulate the scientific approach of the natural sciences. In the nineteenth century, the rise of positivism in philosophy further promoted the assumption that reality obeys general and universal laws. The purpose of the disciplines became to discover by observation and experimentation relationships able to explain nature, the universe, human nature, and social institutions. Psychology, anthropology, and scientific medicine were all deeply influenced by this way of thinking. In the nineteenth century, the effect on education of the increasingly important role attributed to the sciences in the overall knowledge landscape was delayed by the lingering humanist belief that a truly liberal education has to be strongly based on a deep acquaintance with the classics. Secondary instruction was particularly successful in protecting this view; in contrast, in the universities, and especially in Germany where professors and students were recognized a remarkable degree of independence and freedom, mathematics and science became the dominant studies during the second half of the nineteenth century. In the American colleges and universities, the attempt to extend to all fields of knowledge the application of the scientific method and the rise of professional organizations of scholars and specialists in various fields encouraged the subdivision of traditional bodies of knowledge into specialized “subjects.” What at the beginning of the century was studied under the label of “natural history” was by the end of the century subdivided into the various biological and natural sciences, thus including botany, zoology, physiology, psychology, paleontology, ornithology, entomology, and anthropology. Natural philosophy was articulated into specialized physical sciences (astronomy, physics, chemistry, mineralogy, geology, meteorology, and physical geography); similarly, history, economics, political sciences, sociology, and anthropology acquired their own specificity within the field of moral philosophy. The scientific discoveries of the twentieth centuries in astronomy and physics challenged the positivistic assumptions of a rigid and indestructible matter, obedient to rigid laws. More generally, these new insights questioned the close correspondence between what the universe is really like and the picture rendered by science at a certain point in time and brought the debate about the epistemic status of scientific knowledge to the forefront. At the same time, and almost paradoxically, the process of disciplinary subdivision went even further as scholars tended to specialize in ever narrower aspects of one discipline. However, as noted by David Easton and Corinne S. Schelling (1991), real world problems are rarely confined to a specific knowledge domain and the parceled understandings afforded by the increasing specialization do not easily reassemble into a unitary view of the issue at hand. Proposed solutions include interdisciplinary training and teamwork within specific research topics, although the point of departure for these attempts at integration tends to remain the specialized knowledge granted by the various disciplines. On one hand, the overview of the development of disciplinary knowledge showed that a certain partition of the world in different domains reflects broad cultural and institutional trends. On the other hand, the nature of the object investigated makes specific methodological choices more effective and fruitful than others, thus characterizing each discipline as a specific way of knowing. In the next section, we turn to consider the influence that these differences in thinking have on learning and teaching.
Domain Learning Do teaching and learning differ across domains? As documented by Lee S. Shulman and Kathleen M. Quinlan (1996), educational psychologists’ answer to this question has changed considerably across time. During the first two decades of the twentieth century, the answer has been mainly in the affirmative. In particular, John Dewey (1902/1916) advocated the need to psychologize the subject matter of the studies by making explicit the research work that generated knowledge in a specific domain and by referring it to the present experience of the child. His experimental research was thus located within the naturalistic setting of the laboratory school. In the next decades, following the lead of researchers such as Thorndike, educational psychologists’ interest shifted toward the search for general theories of learning. Even when their research was nested within a particular subject matter, the general theoretical framework constrained the analysis of the disciplinary tasks. In addition, the controlled experiment in laboratory setting became the preferred methodological approach, further detaching psychological research from the educational setting. Beginning in the 80s, subject matters regained centrality. For instance, studies on classroom teaching increased understanding of the importance of pedagogical content knowledge. Further, comparisons between novices and experts uncovered the role played by heuristics typical of a certain discipline in the performance on domain specific tasks. Moreover, the classroom with its complexities came to be considered a viable and preferred setting for studying these issues. Overall, researchers found that performance of domain specific tasks calls for particular psychological processes. These findings spurred investigations of what pedagogical practices can best favor the development of these processes and thus improve students’ learning. In the ensuing sections, we consider some of the outcome of this research for the domains of reading, writing, history, mathematics, and science. Traditionally, these domains occupy a large portion of the educational curriculum, especially in the early years. Thus, it is not by chance that a large body of research about domain learning focused on these areas. We also mention a few controversial issues regarding the translation of research results into pedagogical practice. A general review of the work of educational psychologists in regard to learning within these specific domains may be found in the relevant chapters of the first and second editions of the Handbook of Educational Psychology (Alexander & Winne, 2006; Berliner & Calfee, 1996). Alexander (2006) offers an introductory overview of these topics.
Reading
Theories of learning have played a critical role in identifying what is meant by reading and writing, and more generally by literacy. Thus, definitions of readings span from the ability to decode (i.e., breaking the linguistic code) and to encode (i.e., convert written signs fluently into meaning), to being well-learned in a variety of topics, to a set of cultural practices (with more or less emphasis on the power structure engrained in them). The method chosen to study the process of reading and writing contributed to influence the definition of literacy, too. For example, the exclusive focus on observable behavior characterizing research in the behaviorist tradition precluded the possibility to study understanding. Thus, these researchers focused mainly on handwriting, grammar, word recognition, and perception of print. Further, assuming that meaning is inherent in the text and the individual’s role consists in uncovering it, understanding was mainly studied by observing vocabulary and recall. The pedagogical implications of this approach, still present in current curricula, include the suggestion to break the reading process into steps and to teach reading as a series of skills and subskills. As well exemplified by Jerome Bruner (1990), the cognitive revolution focused on explaining mental processes; thus, understanding how meaning was generated became crucial. However, within the cognitive tradition, researchers conceived meaning in different terms. Specifically, those researchers working within the framework of information-processing theory assumed that meaning is transported from the author to the readers, while constructivists posited that meaning is constructed by the readers on the basis of information provided by the author. This difference notwithstanding, cognitive psychologists fostered understanding of how individuals make sense of information conveyed by texts. By investigating the nature of readers’ schemata and how information was organized in memory, researchers studied the role of background knowledge (Anderson & Pearson, 1984). Other studies focused on how different texts work, paying attention to their various structures (e.g., narrative or expository; Alexander & Kulikowich, 1994). Finally, research on the control that individuals maintain during meaning-making has explored individuals’ knowledge of their own cognitive processes (i.e., metacognition). In particular, researchers investigated individuals’ knowledge of the tasks and goals required by reading or writing (i.e., declarative knowledge), of the strategies that allow one to pursue these goals (i.e., procedural knowledge), and individuals’ awareness of how, when, and where to use a specific strategy (i.e., conditional knowledge; Garner, 1987). This work provided the background for research in strategy instruction and, limits notwithstanding, shifted the attention of educational psychologists from product to process. Finally, cognitive psychologists fostered understanding of two basic processes of reading: phonemic awareness and automaticity. Phonemic awareness is the ability to think and manipulate sounds and plays a central role in the study of reading development. Automaticity regards the ability to process perceptual information necessary to the decoding of print with a minimum cognitive load, thus freeing attention for meaning making purposes; within this process, accuracy and speed emerged as good predictors of comprehension (Stanovich, 1990). These findings suggest the soundness of a balanced reading program, in which mastering basic processes and meaningful engagement with text reinforce each other. The social constructivist perspective highlighted the social dimension of learning. Thus, the definition of text was extended beyond the printed words to include conversation, media, and more generally, social discourse. This perspective also implied a crucial epistemological shift, since it defined knowledge as the consensus reached by the community of knowledgeable peers for the time being. Thus, according to this view, the external world may exist, but knowledge is not defined anymore by a correspondence to it (Bruffee, 1986). Such a view also suggested that thinking is an internalized version of conversation; thus learning happens first on the social plane and then it is internalized by the individual (Vygotsky, 1934/1986). Pedagogically, this perspective advocated practices such as reciprocal teaching and whole language approach. This framework also drew attention on the influence of the context on the meaning making process.
Writing
The importance of writing for communication, learning, and knowledge transmission can hardly be overstated. However, the scientific study of the process of writing is relatively recent (Graham, 2006). In the previous section, we considered the influence of different theoretical approaches on the study of literacy in general and described their pedagogical suggestions in terms of reading. In this section, we focus on that body of research that specifically addressed the writing process and on its pedagogical implications. Relying mainly on analyses of think-aloud protocols collected from individuals of different ages and levels of expertise while composing texts, researchers in this domain emphasized that writing is a self-directed process. Often proceeding in a non-linear fashion, this process requires individuals to attend simultaneously to several cognitive demands, and thus entails a high level of effort. Although most of the research focused on cognitive processes, motivational and contextual factors were also found to influence writing (e.g., competence beliefs, and environmental support). Most theorists view writing as the interplay of three main components (Hayes & Flower, 1980). The first comprises factors external to the writer, such as topic, audience, and text produced so far. The second component regards the cognitive processes involved in writing and includes planning (e.g., setting goals, generating ideas, and organizing ideas into a plan), text production (e.g., translating plans into a written text), and reviewing (critically reading the text, determining how to address emerged problems, and editing). The final component is the writer’s long-term memory, which includes knowledge of the topic, of the intended audience, and of rhetorical devices, as well as general plans to perform the writing task. This body of research suggests that teaching writing should address all its components (Hillocks, 1984). In particular, direct teaching of strategies for planning and revising has proven particularly effective (Graham, 2005). At the same time, mastering of basic skills such as handwriting and spelling is also crucial to allow individuals to attend to the multiplicity of writing demands. The learning environment can sustain students’ development of writing strategies by offering appropriate scaffolding and peer-interactions. Finally, increased awareness of the processes of reading and writing, of their differences as well as of their similarities, can foster the development of programs that facilitate development in both domains (Shanahan, 2005).
History
Although there is general agreement in identifying the domain of history with the past, psychological research has associated strikingly different processes to learning in history. In the first decades of the twentieth century, for example, learning history was viewed as the acquisition of temporal perspective and moral ideals (Hall, 1911), the maturation of chronological and causal thinking (Judd, 1915), and the development of historic sense (Bell, 1917). Educational researchers overall agreed that learning history could not be reduced to answering factual questions; however, they also realized that this component of learning was the easiest to test. Behaviorism further restricted the study of domain specific topics; in the case of history, research was limited to study how to apportion facts in order to facilitate memorization. It was only with the cognitive revolution that the attention of researchers focused on the psychological processes involved in learning history (Wineburg, 1996). Aided by the use of qualitative methodologies, educational psychologists explored learners’ background knowledge, trying to uncover beliefs and conceptions that may foster or hinder thinking in history (e.g., ideas about time and chronology, sparse information about historical people and events, and beliefs about the nature of history). They also studied how historians and novices generated historical understanding while reading primary and secondary sources about specific events (Wineburg, 2001). This body of research suggested that learning history entails developing familiarity with concepts and ideas that allow a description of the past (i.e., substantive knowledge) and also becoming acquainted with the strategies employed by historical inquirers to research and interpret the past (i.e., procedural knowledge). In particular, the development of substantive knowledge includes being able to answer who, what, when, where, and how questions about the past (i.e., first-order knowledge), but also developing an understanding of concepts such as causation, change, historical significance, empathy, evidence, and account that allow historical investigators to interpret the past (i.e., second-order knowledge; Lee, 2005). By comparison, procedural knowledge regards being able to use strategies such as assessing the status of sources, corroborating sources, contextualizing events, constructing evidence-based arguments, and writing accounts (VanSledright & Limón, 2006). The difference of these processes with the experience of memorization of information commonly associated to learning history is striking. Although research in the past twenty years has increased understanding of the process of learning history and highlighted its specificity, the intense debate about the role of history in the school curriculum makes it difficult to translate these findings into pedagogical practice. In schools, the goal of developing historical thinking is often countered by the will to use history to build a collective, national identity. While the first purpose is well served by pedagogical approaches that foster a view of history as a critical inquiry into the past, the second is better pursued by the transmission of a specific narrative viewed as coinciding with the past. In addition, more research is needed to understand how to prepare teachers able to foster historical thinking in their students.
Mathematics
Historically, the characterization of mathematics evolved from a science of numbers and space to a science of patterns. More recently, the view of mathematics as a human activity defined within historical, cultural, and social contexts also emerged. In psychological research, specific aspects of mathematics learning were often used for studying general theories and pedagogical approaches. However, the specificity of thinking within this domain was usually ignored (De Corte, Greer, & Verschaffel, 1996). As surfaced in other domains considered herein, different psychological theories of cognitive development espoused distinctive views of mathematics, used a various array of research methodologies, focused on particular aspects of mathematics learning, and thus reached different pedagogical conclusions. Behaviorism and connectionism, for example, favored drill and practice of well-defined information, skills, and associations. The view of mathematics learning changed remarkably with the cognitive revolution, shifting the focus on the processes involved in thinking mathematically. In characterizing the domain, researchers highlighted the dual nature of mathematics. Rooted in the perception and description of the order of objects and events, once mathematics succeeds in modeling these structures through a process of symbolical representation, these representations become amenable to study in an abstract fashion, independent from their real world roots. The pedagogical consequence of acknowledging this feature of the domain is a shift of focus from computation to modeling (i.e., the thinking of the mathematician; Davis, 1992). A second characteristic of the domain regards the fact that, historically, mathematical concepts evolved hierarchically, through a series of restructuring of previously developed concepts, definitions, and functions. The parallel with Piagetian characterization of cognitive growth through a series of restructuring engendered by a situation of disequilibrium perhaps explains why mathematics was so often used by cognitive psychologists to study general cognitive development. Pedagogically, this feature of the domain supports practices that help the child relive the developmental process of the discipline (Freudenthal, 1991). This indication does not suggest that children need to reinvent the product of this development (e.g., the definition of rational numbers), rather that they have to trod the same cognitive path of people facing mathematical problems (e.g., abstracting and formalizing). Finally, development in mathematics requires that individuals are comfortable with multiple modes of representation (e.g., verbal/syntactic, visual/spatial, and formal/symbolic) and fluent in translating from one system to the other. In particular, researchers theorized that mathematical thinking implies the interplay of external representations (e.g., language, symbols on a page, and objects), and internal mental processes, which include internal representations, affective processes, and executive functions (e.g., planning, and monitoring; Goldin, 1992). Pedagogically, this theory suggests that learning mathematics means fostering the development of internal representations well connected and consistent with conventional external representations; fluid movement across different systems of representation needs also to be promoted. In schools, mathematics is often associated to “doing” mathematics rather than thinking mathematically. Thus, problem solving takes a central place in the curriculum and a large body of research in mathematics focused on investigating this process. From these studies, it emerged that problem solving involves the interplay of four factors: domain-specific knowledge; heuristic methods; metacognitive knowledge; and affection (beliefs and emotion). Domain-specific knowledge refers to definitions, formulas, symbols, algorithms, and concepts typical of the domain. Although this knowledge in experts is well organized and thus flexibly accessible, misconceptions and defective skills often hinder problem solving in novices. In addition, experts tend to categorize problems according to their mathematical structure, whereas novices focus on problem surface characteristics (Chi, Glaser, & Farr, 1988; Confrey, 1990). Heuristics are general guidelines that facilitate a systematic approach to the task and thus favor arriving at a solution (e.g., identifying the data of the problem, looking for related problems, and checking the results). However, knowledge of isolated heuristics is usually not very helpful. Research suggests that a more successful approach requires teaching heuristics concurrently with metacognitive skills and while exposing students to a variety of situations, so that learners may understand when and how to use a certain heuristic (Schoenfeld, 1992). Metacognitive knowledge regards knowledge of one’s own cognitive functioning and self-monitoring of these cognitive activities. Skilled problem solvers demonstrate high control of their actions, including planning, monitoring, and if necessary make the required corrections to a previously implemented strategy. Finally, affective components of problem solving include beliefs about the self and about mathematics, and emotions such as interest in the task (McLeod, 1990). Research on problem solving has shown that it is possible to teach students to plan and monitor more effectively (Schoenfeld, 1985). In addition, when the learning environment is structured in such a way as to favor reflective practice and talk among students about their thinking, mathematical learning is usually improved (Lampert, 1990).
Science
Reflecting a trend evidenced in other domains, the definition of what it means to learn science and of what psychological processes are central to its development have been influenced by theories of cognitive development. The meaning of learning science has increasingly broadened to include an understanding of substantive concepts and of the nature of science, logical reasoning, procedures used to develop scientific explanations, and metacognitive awareness (Linn, Songer, & Eylon, 1996). Comparisons between expert and novice thinking demonstrated that novices’ knowledge is organized around concrete factors (e.g., formulas), while experts’ knowledge tends to be hierarchically organized around abstract elements (Chi, Feltovich, & Smith, 1981). Beginning in the late seventies, researchers increasingly distinguished between development of logical skills and development of scientific concepts (Pfundt & Duit, 1991). In addition, research extended beyond the acquisition of science information to investigate learners’ ideas about the nature of science (Hestenes, 1992). The acknowledgment of the role of metacognition in learning paralleled this shift in focus, suggesting that monitoring one’s own cognition is an important component in learning science, once one abandons the idea that science consists of universal truth contained in textbooks (White, 1988). The interpretation of students’ misconceptions also changed; previously conceptualized as instances signaling faulty reasoning on the students’ part, these ideas began to be interpreted as alternative intuitions or framework to model the world. Researchers noted that students’ misconceptions may originate from inaccurate implications drawn from accurate observations, on a focus on inessential characteristics of phenomena, or on the adoption of standards of evidence and views of science markedly different from those espoused by the scientific community (Kuhn, Amsel, & O’Loughlin, 1988). From these insights, two lines of research developed; the first one focused on conceptual change, the second on the restructuring of knowledge. Consistent to Piagetian theory, conceptual change tended to be promoted by using cognitive conflict as a mechanism to foster scientific understanding. More recently, researchers studied what can motivate students to change their current understandings and found that, in addition to facing disconcerting evidence about their current concepts, students also need to confront clear alternatives (Strike & Posner, 1985). This new approach prompted teachers to focus on fostering and guiding the reasoning process of students while they investigate phenomena rather then contradicting the conclusions they reach. The investigation of experts’ organization of scientific knowledge showed that scientists often use qualitative models (e.g., free-body diagrams) as aid in problem-solving. They also tend to chunk knowledge in patterns that can be used in a variety of situations, developing production rules that link a condition (e.g., “the object in contact have different temperature”) to a consequent action (e.g., “the objects will tend toward equilibrium”). In addition, experts tend to entertain several models of scientific phenomena and to choose on which one to rely according to the specific problem they face (Reif & Larkin, 1991). Studies of novices found that students often entertain conflicting interpretations of scientific phenomena. Some researchers hypothesized that students’ scientific knowledge tends to be fragile and fragmented (diSessa et al., 2002); other studies showed that students tend to contextualize their views, applying one set of ideas to interpret phenomena within the classroom context and another set to deal with the same instance out of class (Gilber & Boulter, 2000). In addition, novices’ ability to generalize a model across a range of experiences is usually limited. These findings supported a change in the goals set for science education, with the focus shifting from acquiring science information and concepts to developing the ability to reason scientifically about phenomena (Linn & Eylon, 2006). Pedagogically, this view suggests that students are exposed to a variety of explanatory models and provided with criteria for selecting among them according to the problem considered. Far from engaging students in inquiry or discovery activities for their own sake, this approach highlights the development of background knowledge, modeling and discussion of key processes and strategies, instructional guidance, feedback, use of evidence to test one’s own ideas, and monitoring of one’s progress (Alexander, 2006).
Concluding Thoughts The roots of current disciplines and domains of study reach well back in history. These areas of knowledge and practice have not only reflected societies and cultures of their time, but have also influenced them, especially through formal educational systems. The differences in domains continue to shape the landscape of academic practice due to their inherent and socially-constructed nature. Here we have explored several of those features that define contemporary domains and their instantiation in educational practice.
Bibliography
Alexander, P. A. (2006). Psychology in learning and instruction. Upper Saddle River, NJ: Pearson Merrill Prentice Hall.
Alexander, P. A., & Kulikowich, J. M. (1994). Learning from physics text: A synthesis of recent research. Journal of Research in Science Teaching: Special Issue on Print-Based Language Arts and Science Learning, 31, 895-911.
Alexander, P. A., & Winne, P. H. (Eds.). (2006). Handbook of educational psychology (2nd ed.). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Anderson, R. C., & Pearson, P. D. (1984). A schema-theoretic view of basic processes in reading comprehension. In P. D. Pearson, R. Barr, M. Kamil, & P. Mosenthal (Eds.), Handbook of reading research (Vol. 1, pp. 255-293). New York: Longman.
Bell, J. C. (1917). The historic sense. Journal of Educational Psychology, 8, 317-318.
Berliner, D. C., & Calfee, R. C. (Eds.). (1996). Handbook of educational psychology. New York: Simon & Schuster Macmillan.
Bruffee, K. A. (1986). Social construction, language, and the authority of knowledge: A bibliographic essay. College English, 48, 773-790.
Bruner, J. (1990). Acts of meaning. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Butts, F. R. (1955). A cultural history of western education: Its social and intellectual foundations (2nd ed.). New York: McGraw-Hill Book Company, Inc.
Chi, M. T., Feltovich, P., & Smith, E. L. (1981). Categorization and representation of physics problems by experts and novices. Cognitive Science, 5(2), 121-152.
Chi, M. T., Glaser, R., & Farr, M. J. (Eds.). (1988). The nature of expertise. Hilldale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Confrey, J. (1990). A review of the research on student conceptions in mathematics, science, and programming. In C. B. Cazden (Ed.), Review of research in education (Vol. 16, pp. 3-55). Washington, DC: American Educational Research Association.
Davis, R. B. (1992). Reflections on where mathematics education now stands and on where it may be going. In D. A. Grouws (Ed.), Handbook of research on mathematics teaching and learning (pp. 724-734). New York: Macmillan.
De Corte, E., Greer, B., & Verschaffel, L. (1996). Mathematics teaching and learning. In D. C. Berliner & R. C. Calfee (Eds.), Handbook of educational psychology (pp. 491-549). New York: Simon & Schuster Macmillan.
Dewey, J. (1902/1916). The child and the curriculum. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. diSessa, A. A. (2002). Why “conceptual ecology” is a good idea. In M. Limón & L. Mason (Eds.), Reconsidering conceptual change: Issues in theory and practice (pp. 29-60). Dordrecht, The Netherlands: Kluwer.
Easton, D., & Schelling, C. S. (Eds.). (1991). Divided knowledge: Across disciplines, across cultures. Newbury Park, CA: Sage Publications.
Fehl, N. E. (1966). Science and culture. Hong Kong: Chung Chi Publications.
Freudenthal, H. (1991). Revisiting mathematics education. Dordrecht, The Netherlands: Kluwer.
Garner, R. (1987). Metacognition and reading comprehension. Norwood, NJ: Ablex.
Gilbert, J. K., & Boulter, C. J. (2000). Developing models in science education. Dordrecht, The Netherlands: Kluwer.
Goldin, G. A. (1992). On developing a unified model for the psychology of mathematical learning and problem solving. In W. Geeslin & K. Graham (Eds.), Proceedings of the Sixteenth Annual Meeting of the International Group for the Psychology of Mathematics Education (Vol. 3, pp. 235-261). Durham, NC: University of New Hampshire Press.
Graham, S. (2005). Strategy instruction and the teaching of writing: A meta-analysis. In C. MacArthur, S. Graham, & J. Fitzgerald (Eds.), Handbook of writing research (pp. 187-207). New York: Guilford.
Graham, S. (2006). Writing. In P. A. Alexander & P. H. Winne (Eds.), Handbook of educational psychology (pp. 457-478). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Hall, A. R., & Hall, B. M. (1988). A brief history of science. Ames, IA: Iowa State University Press.
Hall, G. S. (1911). Educational problems (Vol. II). New York: Appleton.
Hayes, J., & Flower, L. (1980). Identifying the organization of writing processes. In L. Gregg & E. Steinberg (Eds.), Cognitive processes in writing (pp. 3-30). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Hestenes, D. (1992). Modeling games in the Newtonian world. American Journal of Physics, 60(8), 732-748.
Hillocks, G. (1986). Research on written composition: New directions for teaching. Urbana, IL: National Council of Teachers of English.
Judd, C. H. (1915). Psychology of high-school subjects. Boston: Ginn.
Kuhn, D., Amsel, E., & O’Loughlin, M., with the assistance of Schauble, L. (1988). The development of scientific thinking skills (Developmental Psychology series). Orlando, FL: Academic Press.
Lampert, M. (1990). When the problem is not the question and the solution is not the answer: Mathematical knowing and teaching. American Educational Research Journal, 27, 29-63.
Lee, J. P. (2005). Putting principles into practice: Understanding history. In M. S. Donovan & J. D. Bransford (Eds.), How students learn: History in the classroom. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press.
Libby, W. (1917). An introduction to the history of science. Boston: Houghton Mifflin Company.
Linn, M. C., Songer, N. B., & Eylon, B. (1996). Shifts and convergences in science learning and instruction. In D. C. Berliner & R. C. Calfee (Eds.), Handbook of educational psychology (pp. 438-490). New York: Simon & Schuster Macmillan.
Linn, M. C., & Eylon, B. (2006). Science education: Integrating views of learning and instruction. In P. A. Alexander & P. H. Winne (Eds.), Handbook of educational psychology (pp. 511-544). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
McLeod, D. B. (1990). Information-processing theories and mathematics learning: The role of affect. International Journal of Educational Research, 14, 13-29.
Pfundt, H., & Duit, R. (1991). Students’ alternative frameworks (3rd ed.). Kiel, Germany: Institut für die Pädagogik der Naturwissenschaften [Institute for Science Education at the University of Kiel].
Reif, F., & Larkin, J. H. (1991). Cognition in scientific and everyday domains: Comparison and learning implications. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 28(9), 733-760.
Shanahan, T. (2005). Relations among oral language, reading, and writing development. In C. MacArthur, S. Graham, & J. Fitzgerald (Eds.), Handbook of writing research (pp. 171-185). New York: Guilford.
Schoenfeld, A. H. (1985). Mathematical problem solving. New York: Academic Press.
Schoenfeld, A. H. (1992). Learning to think mathematically: Problem solving, metacognition, and sense-making in mathematics. In D. A. Grouws (Ed.), Handbook of research on mathematics teaching and learning (pp. 334-370). New York: Macmillan.
Shulman, L. S., & Quinlan, K. M. (1996). The comparative psychology of school subjects. In D. C. Berliner & R. C. Calfee (Eds.), Handbook of educational psychology (pp. 399-422). New York: Simon & Schuster Macmillan.
Stanovich, K. E. (1990). A call for an end to the paradigm wars in reading research. Journal for Reading Behavior, 22, 221-232.
Strike, K. A., & Posner, G. J. (1985). A conceptual change view of learning and understanding. In L. H. West & A. L. Pines (Eds.), Cognitive structure and conceptual change. Orlando, FL: Academic Press.
VanSledright, B., & Limón, M. (2006). Learning and teaching social studies: A review of cognitive research in history and geography. In P. A. Alexander & P. H. Winne (Eds.), Handbook of educational psychology (pp. 545-570). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Vygotsky, L. S. (1986). Thought and language. (A. Kozulin, Trans.). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. (Original work published in 1934)
White, R. T. (1988). Metacognition. In J. P. Keeves (Ed.), Educational research, methodology and measurement: An international handbook (pp. 70-75). Oxford, England: Pergamon Press.
Wineburg, S. (1996). The psychology of learning and teaching history. In D. C. Berliner & R. C. Calfee (Eds.), Handbook of educational psychology (pp. 423-437). New York: Simon & Schuster Macmillan.
Wineburg, S. (2001). Historical thinking and other unnatural acts: Charting the future of teaching the past. Philadelphia: Temple University Press. Further Readings
Donald, J. (2002). Learning to think: Disciplinary perspectives. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
Elby, A. (2001). Helping physics students learn how to learn. Phys. Educ. Res., Am. J. Phys. Suppl., 69(7), 54-64.
Gardner, H. (1999). The disciplined mind: What all students should understand. New York: Simon & Schuster.
Harris, R. W. (1960). Science, mind, and method. Oxford: Basil Blackwell.
Hashweh, M. (1996). Effects of science teachers’ epistemological beliefs in teaching. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 33(1), 47-63.
Hirst, R. H. (1973). Liberal education and the nature of knowledge. In R. S. Peters (Ed.), Philosophy of education (pp. 87-101). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Larkin, J., McDermott, J., Simon, D. P., & Simon, H.A. (1980). Expert and novice performance in solving physics problems. Science, 208(20), 1335-1342.
Messer-Davidow, E., Shumway, D. R., & Sylvan, D. J. (Eds.). (1993). Knowledges: Historical and critical studies in disciplinarity. Charlottesville: University Press of Virginia.
Moll, L., Amanti, C., Neff, D., & Gonzales, N. (1992). Funds of knowledge for teaching: Using a qualitative approach to connect homes and classrooms. Theory into Practice, 31, 132-141.
Seixas, P. (2000). Schweigen! die Kinder! Or, does postmodern history have a place in the schools? In P. N. Stearns, P. Seixas, & S. Wineburg (Eds.), Knowing, teaching, and learning history (pp. 19-37). New York: New York University Press.
VanSledright, B. (2002). In search of America’s past: Learning to read history in elementary school. New York: Teachers College Press.
Venezky, R. L. (1984). The history of reading research. In P. D. Pearson, R. Barr, M. Kamil, & P. Mosenthal (Eds.), Handbook of reading research (Vol. 1, pp. 3-39). New York: Longman.
Wilson, S. M. (1990). Mastodons, maps, and Michigan: Exploring uncharted territory while teaching elementary school social studies (report No. 24). Elementary Subjects Center Series, Michigan State University.
Wineburg, S., & Wilson, S. (2001). Wrinkles in time and place: Using performance assessment to understand the knowledge of history teachers. In S. Wineburg, (Ed.), Historical thinking and other unnatural acts: Charting the future of teaching the past (pp. 173-214). Philadelphia: Temple University Press.

Similar Documents

Free Essay

Knowledge and Learning

...Sarah Wilson December 2nd, 2013 Knowledge/Learning essay Monday 6pm Intro Philosophy Regarding the two clashing models of the human mind in early modern epistemology (Locke’s tabula rasa and Leibniz’s Statue in Marble) I believe that nurturing, upbringing, environment and experiences are most important to a person’s success in life. Personally (I think) when a baby is born they do not possess any superior talents at that stage of their lives. While I understand (from my own experiences as a mother) that babies are capable of basic physiological functions like urination and defecation, they to possess a seemingly instinctual desire to suck/nurse. That instinct, I hypothesize, could be argued or prioritized as in-born talent according to Leibniz, seeing as to how a baby wants to do that right out of the womb. To elaborate a little further, it is my belief that as the child ages, it may become evident that they have a propensity to do certain activities particularly well, exhibiting what appears to be a “natural” talent or aptitude for what they are doing. Perhaps this activity comes easily to the child (and time goes on adolescent, then adult) because of the way their brain is hardwired? Thinking from a rationalist perspective, it is plausible that the reason why the child excels at what they do is because of the cognitive ability that they have in relation to said activity. If the child “has the smarts” to do something well, logic would lead one to believe that they would excel...

Words: 732 - Pages: 3

Free Essay

Knowledge and Learning

...as possible. An intelligent reader will try his or her hand at all sorts of texts. Like a curious chef who tries to accommodate different flavours and exotic ingredients in a large selection of dishes and recipes, the astute reader will explore a never-ending spectrum of texts and narratives, because reading in itself is a wonderful adventure that may lead to the discovery of the world about us (the other) and to the discovery of ourselves (the self). What else do we hope for? There seems to be two main purposes for reading: pleasure and learning, both of which may actually boil down to the one and same, that is, sheer pleasure. Learning is an intellectual pleasure, par excellence. A society that has an enticing reading policy is an open society which keeps changing and developing. On the contrary, a society that does not seek pleasure for its own sake cannot think of promoting reading as a source of entertainment and knowledge. This type of society is only interested in power and monopoly stacked in the hands of a political oligarchy or party; and it is keen on the control and containment of its subjects. It is a closed society curled upon itself, resisting change and development. It is disheartening to note that many higher-education institutions still store tens of thousands of books behind closed doors and prevent students and young researchers from having free access to the bookshelves. Free access to books and periodicals is the rule in most...

Words: 333 - Pages: 2

Premium Essay

Knowledge Org Learning

...Q1. How can social media such as LinkedIn, Facebook, and Twitter be used to improve knowledge sharing, build social capital, support innovation, and aid problem solving in one of the following context - - A multinational corporation - Small business networks in Australia or your home country - Networks of community based Not for Profit organisations in Australia or your home country Discuss the associated risks and benefits for this context Table of Contents Introduction 4 Building Social Capital 5 Improving Knowledge Sharing 6 Aid Problem Solving 7 Supporting Innovation 8 Risks of Social Media 9 Conclusion 10 References 11 Introduction This essay examines how social media can be used to assist multinational organisations through enhancement of knowledge assets and social capital. Social media is an accessible and influential tool that can be used by organisations to cultivate and improve abilities to learn and share new knowledge. This knowledge can be used to solve problems through the effective usage of social media. Social networking has caused a fundamental and transformational shift in the way that people are connecting, knowledge is being shared and information is flowing However, as the information is communicated via the internet, risk of miscommunication and loss of vital information may happen. With proper management of knowledge sharing through social media, the organisation can provide excellent services and products to...

Words: 3073 - Pages: 13

Premium Essay

Knowledge Management in Organizational Learning

...Knowledge Management and Organizational Learning William R. King Katz Graduate School of Business, University of Pittsburgh wking115@yahoo.com For centuries, scientists, philosophers and intelligent laymen have been concerned about creating, acquiring, and communicating knowledge and improving the re-utilization of knowledge. However, it is only in the last 15–20 years or so that a distinct field called “knowledge management” (KM) has emerged. KM is based on the premise that, just as human beings are unable to draw on the full potential of their brains, organizations are generally not able to fully utilize the knowledge that they possess. Through KM, organizations seek to acquire or create potentially useful knowledge and to make it available to those who can use it at a time and place that is appropriate for them to achieve maximum effective usage in order to positively influence organizational performance. It is generally believed that if an organization can increase its effective knowledge utilization by only a small percentage, great benefits will result. Organizational learning (OL) is complementary to KM. An early view of OL was “encoding inferences from history into routines that guide behavior” (Levitt and March, 1988, p. 319). So, OL has to do with embedding what has been learned into the fabric of the organization. 1 The Basics of Knowledge Management and Organizational Learning To understand KM and OL, one must understand knowledge, KM...

Words: 4815 - Pages: 20

Premium Essay

Learning Strategy and Knowledge Management

...E-Learning Strategy 1 E- Learning Strategy and Knowledge Management (Insert Your Full Name Here) (Insert Your Institution’s Name Here) April 18, 2016 E-Learning Strategy 2 E- Learning Strategy and Knowledge Management Introduction The role of the Human Resource department in the workplace has shifted dramatically as these consultants, specialists and teams apply proven methods of management to what would otherwise be an extensive and unproductive employee base. The field of human resources encompasses the type and level of management necessary for the active and effective recruitment, hiring, day-to-day employment practices, job termination policy, job description policy and much more (Hesselbein, Goldsmith, and Somerville, 2001). While the requirement to maintain a strong and productive labor field had never truly been lost on the savvy employers of the past, the recent advances in human resources knowledge have actually brought this field into the active role that it entails in order to implement positive changes upon a company’s strength and capability among its competitors. It is through the development and conveyance of an understanding of positive, hands-on techniques that, when successfully executed within a company’s employees, a human resources team can actually change conditions on the ground, human resources professional everywhere have started to notably widen profit margins to a greater extent, by increasing not only competence, but productivity...

Words: 6084 - Pages: 25

Premium Essay

Knowledge Management & Organizational Learning

... Knowledge Management & Organizational Learning Knowledge has always been available to mankind. Many people have had their concerns and doubts about how to effectively create, acquire, communicate and improve on how to re-use knowledge. Within the last two decades a unique field called Knowledge Management (KM) has transpired. With the sudden emergence of KM, a brand new realization has been presented to society. It’s a known fact that human begins have not been able to draw on the full potential of their brains. (King 2008). Just like with the human brain it’s also the backbone on how companies and organizations are started. Is it possible through KM, for organizations to seek and obtain potentially useful knowledge? Can it be made available to those when needed and appropriate for them to achieve maximum effective usage in order to make an influential and impactful gain in an organizations performance? To understand if KM could help an organization maximize potentially useful knowledge and achieve influential gains and performances one has to fully understand knowledge. Knowledge is commonly defined as a “justified personal belief”. Knowledge is separated into two distinctive fundamentals, tacit knowledge and explicit knowledge. Tacit knowledge is knowledge that a person already possesses and helps determine how they already think and act. It links physical and cognitive skills, and cognitive frameworks. A common inspection of tacit knowledge is that it’s...

Words: 2081 - Pages: 9

Premium Essay

Knowledge Management & Organisational Learning

...1. Introduction For many years, organisations have been anxious about constructing, attaining, converse knowledge and improving the consumption of knowledge. However, in the early 1990s, Knowledge Management (KM) started emerging as human beings are unable to utilise their full potential of their brains. This leads to organisation are unable to totally exploit the knowledge that all human beings hold. In today’s global economy, Knowledge has become essential and it creates significant competitive advantage for any organisation (Gunnlaugsdottir, 2003). By using KM, organisations are able to attain or construct valuable knowledge and to make it accessible to those who can utilise it correctly to accomplish utmost efficient procedure in order to completely control organisational performance. Organisational Learning (OL) is corresponding to KM. In the early centuries, OL are referred to as decoding suggestion from learning into practice that direct performance (Levitt and March, 1988). In other words, OL has to execute with embedding what has been taught into the framework of the organisation. 2. Definition of Knowledge Management and Organisational Learning 3.1 Knowledge Management Knowledge management (KM) refers to the process in which organisations acquire or create useful knowledge and make it available for employees to utilize at the proper time and place in order for them to achieve the most effective usage in for optimal organisational performance. It involves...

Words: 2758 - Pages: 12

Premium Essay

Learning Knowledge & Effective Performance

...Knowledge Management A practical perspective HAN 13080004 Learning and knowledge management report Module title : Learning Knowledge and Effective Performance Module lecturer : Matthew Nobel Table of content Introduction...........................................................................................................................3 1. Learning knowledge : a brief overview............................................................................3 2. Training and learning of PSC1 : ........................................................................................3 a. Training Method :................................................................................................4 b. Information and communication technology (ICT) :..........................................5 3. Evaluation and recommendation :...................................................................................5 4. Conclusion :........................................................................................................................7 Introduction The purpose of this report is to evaluate an actual learning and knowledge management situation of a company named "Central Plant Protection Joint Stock Company no.1" (PSC1) to give a more in depth look on this subject. The information given in this report was partly research and was also conducted by a personal interview with a board members of the company, Mr. Long. The report is structured into four...

Words: 2291 - Pages: 10

Premium Essay

Organizational Learning for Organizational Knowledge

...Organizational Learning for Organizational Knowledge The Full Future for Knowledge Management All organizations have one true goal, and that is to grow and flourish, to be successful in whatever the scope of their business model entails. In order to effectively accomplish this goal all organizations must acquire the ability to transform information into knowledge and learn from that knowledge. Thomas H. Huxley probably stated it best when he said “the greatest end of life is not knowledge but action’ (Lewis, 2008), this holds an inherent thread of truth for almost any organization. Any organization that fails to learn from the knowledge they hold, is most likely doomed to failure as an organization. Organizations all have missions; they state these concisely in their mission statements, briefly outlining their reason for being. This is their focus and ultimate goal and while these goals are normally quite lofty the real question is how they are going to accomplish this goal. All resources are going to be directed to the achievement of this goal, and in reality, probably the most overlooked yet undoubtedly most important resources is found in organizational knowledge. Organizational knowledge can take many forms; it can be books, lessons and training, files on a network or computer, even what an individual employee knows and more. In its most broad sense, it is each and every one of these things, from all sources and employees collectively under the umbrella that...

Words: 1111 - Pages: 5

Premium Essay

Learning Knowledge and Effective Performance

...REINVENTING THE WHEEL AT APEX DOOR COMPANY Jim Delaney, President of Apex Door Company, has a problem. No matter how often he tells his employees how to do their jobs, they invariably “decide to do things their own way”, as he puts it, and arguments ensue between Delaney, his employee and the employee’s supervisor. One example is in the door-design department. The designers are expected to work with the architects to design doors that meet the specifications. Although its not “rocket science”, as Delaney puts it, the designers often make mistakes- such as designing in too much steel- a problem that can cost Apex tens of thousands of wasted dollars, especially considering the numbers of doors in, say, a 30-story office tower. The order processing department is another example. Although Jim has a specific, detailed way he wants each order written up, most of the order clerks don’t understand how to use the multipage order form, and they improvise when it comes to a question such as whether to classify a customer as “industrial” or “commercial”. The current training process is as follows. None of the jobs have training manuals per se, although several have somewhat out-of-date job descriptions. The training of new employees is all on the job. Usually, the person leaving the company trains the new person during the 1- or 2-weeks overlap period, but if there is no overlap, the new person is trained as well as possible by other employees who have occasionally filled in on the job...

Words: 279 - Pages: 2

Premium Essay

Topic: Teacher’s Knowledge, Identity and Major Learning Theories

...Topic: Teacher’s Knowledge, Identity and Major Learning Theories Using your knowledge of teacher’s knowledge bases, major learning theories and teaching preferences define your identity as ‘teacher’. Then reflect upon this position in terms of strengths, weaknesses, opportunities and threats in respect to teaching practice. Introduction In this paper I will explore teacher knowledge bases, major learning theories, teacher identity and teaching preferences by commenting on a VET teacher who we will call Mr Black. I will then reflect upon his application of each in term of strengths, weaknesses, opportunities and threats in respect to his teaching practice. The subject, Mr Black, is retained by one organisation to conduct training and assessments for a specific group of tradesmen who will be working on a major infrastructure upgrade project. The duration of his contract is to be no more than 4 years. Mr Blacks’ background is in a related technical field and he holds a Certificate IV in Training and Assessment therefore he falls into the VET stream of Education Robertson (Robertson I. , 2008) has suggested that Certificate IV in Training and Assessment may provide enough opportunity to develop the applied skills required for an expert teacher. Teachers working in Vocational Education and Training (VET) face in an increasingly complex and diverse environment as highlighted by Robertson (Robertson I. , 2007) Teachers and teaching roles come in all shapes and sizes and so it is true...

Words: 3150 - Pages: 13

Premium Essay

Managing Knowledge: the Link Between Culture and Organizational Learning

...Essay: Managing Knowledge: The Link between Culture and Organizational Learning According to the article, I’m totally agree with it, because the main idea is that to be successful in business environment you must Know your company and make your employees know it as well, start knowing the goals, objectives, activities, personal, infrastructure, organization, processes, culture, in general all the things that involves your company, have an entire knowledge about it. Other interesting point is the Stata’s quote that talk about the organization’s ability to learn faster all the aspects about the company than the competitors is an important source of competitive advantage, this is really interesting, because if all people involved into an organization knows exactly what is it about, create new fresh ideas, and communicate them to the other members of the organization in an efficient way, this allows the company to become successful and better than the competitors. I think that every company, has an organizational culture and learning process clearly defined, when an enterprise has an organizational culture that leads the entire company, the personal feels they are committed to the enterprise, and they are satisfied with their performance. About the learning processes I think is vital to the organization that all people involved know exactly what are their functions, and not only their owns, also the manager’s, CEO’s, and Director’s functions, with this, they feel more...

Words: 328 - Pages: 2

Premium Essay

Case Analysis: Mckinsey & Company- Managing Knowledge and Learning

...organizations and government agencies worldwide. 'Forbes' estimated the firm's 2005 revenues at $3.8 billion in its list of largest private companies. Company Intent To be the global leader in consulting industry, provide expertise consulting service to the worldwide clients. Company Mission To help the clients make positive, lasting, and substantial improvements in their performance and to build a great firm that is able to attract, develop, excite, and retain exceptional people. Key issue This case discrbed the development process of McKinsey&Compay from 1926 to 1996. In particular, it focuses on the way in which McKinsey has developed structures, systems, processes, and practices to help it develop, transfer, and disseminate knowledge among its 3,800 consultants in 69 offices worldwide. Concludes by focusing on three young consultants operating in each dimension of the...

Words: 2526 - Pages: 11

Premium Essay

Technology

...Article published in Education Today, issue 2 of 2005, Aries Publishing Company, Thames, New Zealand. Article by: Dr Tom H Brown Deputy Director Department of Telematic Learning & Education Innovation University of Pretoria South Africa 0002 +27 12 420-3884 (Tel) +27 12 420-3828 (Fax) +27 82 908-3884 (Cell) eMail: tom.brown@up.ac.za Beyond constructivism: Exploring future learning paradigms Abstract Educational practice is continually subjected to renewal, due to developments in information and communication technology (ICT), the commercialisation and globalisation of education, social changes and the pursuit of quality. Of these, the impact of ICT and the new knowledge economy are the most significant. Changes in our educational practice lead, in turn, to changes in our approaches to teaching and learning. These changes also impact on our teaching and learning paradigms. Currently, as over the past few decades, we teach and learn in a constructivist learning paradigm. This article discusses past and present paradigm shifts in education and then explores possible future learning paradigms in the light of the knowledge explosion in the knowledge era that we are currently entering. 1. The impact of ICT on education The electronic information revolution currently experienced in the world can be compared to and reveals the same characteristics as the first information revolution started by Gutenberg’s printing press. This means that, just as present-day society accepts...

Words: 4227 - Pages: 17

Premium Essay

Process Management

...How to integrate technology-enhanced learning with business process management Nicola Capuano, Matteo Gaeta, Pierluigi Ritrovato and Saverio Salerno Abstract Purpose – The purpose of this paper is to propose an innovative approach for providing an answer to the emerging trends on how to integrate e-learning efficiently in the business value chain in medium and large enterprises. Design/methodology/approach – The proposed approach defines methodologies and technologies for integrating technology-enhanced learning with knowledge and human resources management based on a synergistic use of knowledge models, methods, technologies and approaches covering different steps of the knowledge life-cycle. Findings – The proposed approach makes explicit and supports, from the methodological, technological and organizational points of view, mutual dependencies between the enterprise’s organizational learning and the business processes, considering also their integration in order to allow the optimization of employees’ learning plans with respect to business processes and taking into account competencies, skills, performances and knowledge available inside the organization. Nicola Capuano, Matteo Gaeta, Pierluigi Ritrovato and Saverio Salerno are all ` based at the Universita di Salerno, Fisciano, Italy. Practical implications – This mutual dependency, bridging individual and organizational learning, enables an improvement loop to become a key aspect for successful business process improvement...

Words: 7954 - Pages: 32