Free Essay

Marketing Journal

In:

Submitted By WhiteFog
Words 15661
Pages 63
Journal of Consumer Marketing
Emerald Article: To buy or not to buy? A social dilemma perspective on green buying
Shruti Gupta, Denise T. Ogden
Article information:
To cite this document: Shruti Gupta, Denise T. Ogden, (2009),"To buy or not to buy? A social dilemma perspective on green buying", Journal of Consumer Marketing, Vol. 26 Iss: 6 pp. 376 - 391
Permanent link to this document: http://dx.doi.org/10.1108/07363760910988201 Downloaded on: 28-05-2012
References: This document contains references to 76 other documents
To copy this document: permissions@emeraldinsight.com
This document has been downloaded 4703 times.
Access to this document was granted through an Emerald subscription provided by INDEPENDENT UNIVERSITY BANGLADESH
For Authors:
If you would like to write for this, or any other Emerald publication, then please use our Emerald for Authors service.
Information about how to choose which publication to write for and submission guidelines are available for all. Additional help for authors is available for Emerald subscribers. Please visit www.emeraldinsight.com/authors for more information.
About Emerald www.emeraldinsight.com
With over forty years' experience, Emerald Group Publishing is a leading independent publisher of global research with impact in business, society, public policy and education. In total, Emerald publishes over 275 journals and more than 130 book series, as well as an extensive range of online products and services. Emerald is both COUNTER 3 and TRANSFER compliant. The organization is a partner of the Committee on Publication Ethics (COPE) and also works with Portico and the LOCKSS initiative for digital archive preservation. *Related content and download information correct at time of download.
To buy or not to buy? A social dilemma perspective on green buying
Shruti Gupta
The Pennsylvania State University – Abington, Abington, Pennsylvania, USA, and
Denise T. Ogden
The Pennsylvania State University – Lehigh Valley, Fogelsville, Pennsylvania, USA
Abstract
Purpose – The purpose of this paper is to draw on social dilemma theory and reference group theory to explain the attitude-behavior inconsistency in environmental consumerism. This research seeks to better understand why, despite concern towards the environment (attitude), consumers fail to purchase environmentally friendly or green products (behavior).
Design/methodology/approach – A survey instrument was developed that used scales to measure eight independent and one dependent variable.
In addition, socio-demographic data were also collected about the study participants. To discriminate between green and non-green buyers, classification with discriminant analysis was used.
Findings – The framework presented contributes to the environmental consumerism literature by framing the attitude-behavior gap as a social dilemma and draws on reference group theory to identify individual factors to help understand the gap and suggest ways in which to bridge it. Results from the study reveal that several characteristics of the individual – trust, in-group identity, expectation of others’ cooperation and perceived efficacy – were significant in differentiating between “non-green” and “green” buyers.
Practical implications – The results of the study offer several managerial implications. First, marketers should reinforce the role trust plays in solidifying collective action. Second, because of the strong influence of reference groups in green buying, marketing communications managers should use spokespeople who are relatable. Third, the study showed that expectation of others’ cooperation significantly identifies green buyers. Fourth, to address the perception of personal efficacy, it is important that green marketers emphasize the difference that individual action makes for the collective good. Originality/value – The research draws on both social dilemma and reference group theories to investigate the determinants of and the mechanisms to explain the rationale behind the attitude-behavior gap as it pertains to a specific environmental issue – energy conservation.
Keywords Environmental politics, Buying behaviour, Consumer behaviour, Influence
Paper type Research paper
An executive summary for managers and executive readers can be found at the end of this article.
Introduction
The industry for green products is estimated at over $200 billion in 2006. The documentary on global warming, An
Inconvenient Truth, as well as celebrities speaking in defense of green living, has spurred interest in issues surrounding environmental conservation and protection in the marketplace
(Mintel, 2006). This current wave of interest in environmental conservation is not a new phenomenon with research and opinion polls from a decade ago (Roper
Organization, 1992) having shown that American consumers are concerned about the environment. However, the same research also revealed that despite expressing concern towards the environment the consumers were unwilling to purchase or pay a higher price for environmentally friendly products (Jay, 1990; Ottman,
1992; Schlossberg, 1991). A more recent report by the
RoperASW (2002) produced somewhat disappointing results with overall environmental concern among the general population on a decline with 59 per cent of the general population not even thinking of participating in environmentally friendly activities. Despite a waning interest in overall environmental protection, the same poll surprisingly revealed that contrary to the above pattern, Americans were willing to purchase and even pay more for specific products that help conserve energy or are less polluting, such as energy efficient appliances, hybrid cars and electricity.
These mixed results reveal a challenging consumer environment for marketers of environmentally friendly consumer products, i.e. green products who seek guidance on “how to” identify and effectively market environmental friendly products to the green consumer segments (Osterhus,
1997). At the same time, research to date has failed to answer the puzzling question about why despite concern towards the
The current issue and full text archive of this journal is available at www.emeraldinsight.com/0736-3761.htm Journal of Consumer Marketing
26/6 (2009) 376–391 q Emerald Group Publishing Limited [ISSN 0736-3761]
[DOI 10.1108/07363760910988201]
The authors would like to thank the Rubin Endowment at Penn State
Abington for the generous financial support used for data collection in this study. 376 environment (attitude) consumers fail to purchase environmentally friendly or green products (behavior)?
The objective of this paper is to investigate the above attitude-behavior dilemma in green buying. This research argues that the attitude-behavior gap in environmental consumerism exists because it presents a social dilemma to the consumers. For a small subset of consumers who are “true believers” in environmental protection and conservation, the personal importance of the environmental issue is likely to ensure unconditional participation. However, most consumers, despite holding a positive attitude toward environmental conservation make purchase decisions to maximize self-interest because in their view, the costs of cooperation outweigh the uncertain utility obtained from it.
Therefore, the decision to buy (collective social gain) or not buy (self-interest) the green product despite positive attitude towards environmental conservation may be conceptualized as a social dilemma.
This paper also draws on reference group theory that suggests that consumer decision to make the trade-off between such self and collective group interests may also be dependent on the pressure to comply with the expectations and behaviors of significant reference groups (e.g. environmentally friendly group’s choice to cooperate and therefore buy green products). Consequently, this research draws on both social dilemma and reference group theories to investigate the determinants of and the mechanisms to explain the rationale behind the attitude-behavior gap as it pertains to a specific environmental issue – energy conservation. The issue of energy conservation has been studied as a social dilemma (referred to as a resource conservation dilemma), where the short-term incentive to consume the energy resource leaves a long-term undesirable consequence of shortage and higher prices for everyone (Komorita and Parks,
1996). Such a resource conservation dilemma arises when the rate of resource consumption by the group exceeds the supply or replenishment rate (Kramer and Goldman, 1995). In order to conserve this resource, consumers find themselves in a social situation where they are forced to make tradeoffs between bearing the costs of their personal sacrifice and the benefit of cooperative behavior for the group (Messick and
Brewer, 1983).
Conceptual background
This section presents a brief background of the attitudebehavior gap in environmental consumerism followed by a review of the theory in social dilemma. Next, a conceptual framework that identifies consumers’ individual attributions regarding cooperation in a social dilemma, particularly, ingroup identification, social value orientation, trust, personal efficacy and the costs of purchasing and consuming a green product (i.e. preference and substitutability of the product) on their decision to buy energy efficient products is presented.
Attitude-behavior inconsistency in environmental consumerism For marketers of green products, the gap between proenvironmental attitudes and green purchase behavior is a daunting challenge where, an attitude is defined as “an enduring set of beliefs about an object that predisposes people to behave in particular way toward the object” (Weigel, 1983
p. 257). Theory in the area of consumer attitude argues that individuals behave in ways consistent with their attitudes.
However, research in environmental consumerism has produced inconclusive evidence in support of consumer attitude theory with mixed results that support both a positive relationship between attitude toward the environment and behavior (Arbuthnot, 1977; Kellgren andWood, 1986) as well as weak relationships (Mainieri et al., 1997; Tanner and Kast,
2003; Webster, 1975; Wicker, 1969). A more recent study by
Mintel (2006) resonates earlier results and found that despite pro-environmental attitudes, intention to recycle, concern about car pollution and willingness to pay more for environmentally-friendly products, few consumers translated these attitudes into regular green buying behavior. These inconclusive results therefore compound the challenge for green marketers who struggle to correctly identify the green consumer segment. In order shed light on this dilemma, it is important to review the literature on the processes and variables that shape consumer attitudes. The section next presents a brief summary of predominant consumer attitude theory followed by a review that highlights explanations offered by researchers to understand the attitude-behavior inconsistency. The attitude-behavior link
Consumer behaviorists have long stressed on the ability of attitudes to predict peoples’ action. A number of situational and dispositional factors have been suggested that improve the predictive ability of consumer attitude. For example, attitudes have been shown to significantly predict behavior when: . the person ascribes to a specific personality type with a
“high need for cognition” (Cacioppo et al., 1986);
. relevant attitudes are consistent with the beliefs (Norman,
1975); and
. attitudes are based on high levels of issue specific knowledge and/or personal experience (Davidson et al.,
1985; Fazio and Zanna, 1981; Smith and Swinyard, 1983) and others (see Ajzen, 1989 for review).
Research also shows that a number of methodological issues should be addressed if attitudes are to predict behavior. For example, attitude and behavior should be measured at the same level of correspondence (Ajzen and Fishbein, 1977) and at the same time (Davidson and Jaccard, 1979) among others
(see Petty et al., 1991) for review).
Several theories explain the process by which attitudes predict behavior. First, the “theory of reasoned action” (Ajzen and Fishbein, 1980, p. 5) assumes that “people consider the implications of their actions before they decide to engage or not engage in a given behavior”. As a result, people form intentions to perform behaviors which in turn stem from a person’s attitude towards the behavior as well as his or her perception of others’ opinions (social norms). The model primarily argues that people engage in processing that leads to the formation of attitudes, norms and intentions prior to performing the behavior. A criticism of the theory of reasoned action was that some studies failed to support the link between behavioral intention and behavior and attributed the inconsistency to the lack of control over a person’s action.
Therefore to address the above limitation, the “Theory of planned behavior” (Ajzen, 1988; Ajzen, 1991) extends the theory of reasoned action by including a new component,
“perceived behavioral control”. This concept originates from the self efficacy theory (SET) and is similar Bandura’s (1982)
To buy or not to buy? A social dilemma perspective on green buying
Shruti Gupta and Denise T. Ogden
Journal of Consumer Marketing
Volume 26 · Number 6 · 2009 · 376–391
377
concept of self-efficacy which refers to the belief that one can successfully execute a behavior required to produce an outcome. Perceived behavioral control is the subject’s perception of the ease or difficulty of performing the behavior. Generally, behaviors that are perceived to be easier to perform will be completed over difficult behaviors.
Perceived behavioral control is also linked to control beliefs which are beliefs about the presence of factors that may hinder or help performance of a behavior. Therefore, the intention to perform a behavior is enhanced under conditions of favorable attitude towards the behavior and subjective norm and greater perceived behavioral control. The model also suggests that when a person is given a sufficient degree of actual control over the behavior, he/she will be expected to carry out the behavioral intention (Ajzen, 2002). By including the behavioral control component in attitude theory researchers are able to explain complex behavior which is often dependent on performance of other behaviors (Conner and Armitage, 1998), such as is the case with ecological behavior (Hines et al., 1986-1987; Kaiser et al., 1999).
In contrast with the theories of reasoned action and planned behavior, Fazio (1986) has proposed that attitudes guide behavior through an automatic and spontaneous process instead of a deliberate one as argued by the earlier two theories. According to Fazio (1989), two conditions dictate the likelihood that behaviors will be consistent with attitudes, when: 1 the attitude is accessed spontaneously by the mere presence of the attitude object; and
2 the attitude influences the perception of the object so that when the attitude is favorable (or unfavorable) the traits of the object are also perceived as favorable (or unfavorable).
Despite theoretical reasoning presented above that suggests the attitude-behavior link, the predictive ability of attitude in the domain of environmental consumerism has been debatable. Researchers have attempted to explain this inconsistency between attitude and behavior by attributing it to a number of factors: low correlations among environmental behaviors, different levels of specificity in the attitudebehavior measures, effects of external variables and lack of measurement reliability and validity (Mainieri et al., 1997).
Past research has shown that pro-environmental behaviors performed by the same individual are not significantly correlated (Tracy and Oskamp, 1983-1984) where an individual who performs one type of such behavior, e.g. carpooling is also expected to engage in other similar behaviors such as recycling. Lack of measurement specificity between attitude and behavior suggests that the inconsistency exists as a result of researchers failing to measure behaviorspecific attitude instead focusing on a generalized view of environmental attitude (Mainieri et al., 1997) and behavior.
Therefore, it has been recommended that in order to predict specific behavior, i.e. purchase of green products, the attitudes measured need to be pointed at a specific environmental issue (Heberlein and Black, 1976; Weigel et al., 1976). Additionally, personal (knowledge, motivation or attitudes) and situational (social norms, other attractive choices or economic constraints) factors may also confound the relationship between environmental attitudes and behavior (Mainieri et al., 1997).
In summary, a review of the literature in environmental consumerism reveals a unanimous pessimism regarding the ability of general environmental attitudes to predict purchase behavior (Berger and Corbin, 1992) with agreement that attitudes are more likely unrelated or slightly related to overt behavior (Wicker, 1969). Therefore, based on the explanations offered by earlier researchers and the growing significance of green products in today’s market place, this paper seeks to explain the attitude-behavior inconsistency by framing it as a social dilemma and subsequently highlight factors borrowed from the literature to discriminate between green and non-green buyers. Next, the theory on social dilemma is explained in context of a specific environmental issue – energy conservation.
Energy conservation as a social dilemma
Social dilemmas are situations in which members of a group face a choice either to cooperate in order to maximize group gain or to defect for self interest (Messick and Brewer, 1983).
In such a situation, “each individual receives a higher payoff for a socially defecting choice than for a socially cooperative one, yet all individuals have a higher payoff if all cooperate than if all defect” (Dawes, 1980, p. 173). The choice to defect is the optimal choice (Messick and Brewer, 1983) since each individual is better off acting in his or her own self-interest regardless of what the other group members decide, but in doing so all individuals are worse off than if they had cooperated towards collective gain (Hardin, 1968; Kollock,
1998; Komorita and Parks, 1996). This “tragedy of the commons” was first suggested by Hardin (1968). Schultz and
Holbrook (1999) call for development of marketing solutions to the commons dilemma, especially in environmental marketing. Dawes (1980) cites the example of when citizens are asked to lower thermostats in order to conserve energy to illustrate the concept of a social dilemma. An individual who chooses to defect in self interest will keep the thermostat high and not suffer from the cold. However, if all individuals choose to defect and decide to keep their thermostats high, then everyone will be worse off as the energy supply runs out and everyone has to suffer from the cold. Such examples of resource dilemma point to the issue of social interdependence in which all participants have identical incentive structures and as a result the cumulative consequence of each individual responding to the incentive leads to a disastrous outcome for the larger group (Messick and Brewer, 1983).
Social dilemmas are defined by three properties (Dawes,
1980; Messick and Brewer, 1983; van Lange et al., 1992):
1 a non-cooperative choice guided by self-gain is more rewarding to the individual than a cooperative one, irrespective of the decision made by other members of the group; 2 relative to a cooperative choice, a non-cooperative choice is always detrimental to the others in group; and
3 in a non-cooperative choice, the collective harm to the group is greater than the gain to the individual.
Thus, social dilemmas involve the interplay between selfish motives and cooperation that requires individual sacrifice in the short run for the benefit of all individuals in the long run.
Cooperation in social dilemmas: factors that influence green buying
This paper uses social dilemma theory to explain the attitudebehavior gap inconsistency in environmental consumerism.
To buy or not to buy? A social dilemma perspective on green buying
Shruti Gupta and Denise T. Ogden
Journal of Consumer Marketing
Volume 26 · Number 6 · 2009 · 376–391
378
The framework in this paper suggests that the difference between green and non-green buyers will depend on certain key factors related to the individual: social value orientation, trust in others, reference group influence (in-group identity, expectation of others’ cooperation) perceived efficacy and factors that influence the costs of cooperation to the individual (product substitutability and product preference).
Social value orientation
Research to understand the choice in a social dilemma to cooperate or defect is also influenced by pre-existing individual differences such as social value orientations.
Social value orientations are “...preferences for particular distribution of outcomes to oneself and others” (Messick and
McClintock, 1968; van Lange et al., 1992 p. 17). The three most prominent social value orientations are (Kramer and
Goldman, 1995; van Lange et al., 1992):
1 cooperation which is the tendency to maximize both self and others’ outcomes;
2 individualism which focuses on self gain; and
3 competition which stresses on relative gain for self over others. In the case of resource conservation dilemma, co-operators are more likely to reduce personal consumption of the resource and therefore, resist the temptation to defect than individualists or competitors. Therefore, it is suggested that:
H1. Social value orientation is a significant discriminating variable between green and non-green buyers.
Trust
In addition to social value orientations, individual differences in trust have also been studied in social dilemma literature.
Individuals have been found to differ in their willingness to believe that others are honest or dishonest and as a result trusting others is risky or not (Yamagishi, 1988). It is suggested that such differences in trust has a strong bearing on the decision to cooperate or defect in social dilemmas, where, high-trusters are more likely to cooperate than lowtrusters
(van Lange et al., 1992). Therefore, it is expected that: H2. Likelihood to trust is a significant discriminating variable between green and non-green buyers.
A key factor that influences consumers’ decision to cooperate rather than defect is the conformity pressure they are likely to feel, both internally and from external sources, to make choices similar to those made by other group members
(Komorita and Parks, 1996). Though social dilemma research has suggested that group size and communication influences individual choice to cooperate (Messick and Brewer, 1983), not much research has investigated into for whom and why such conformity pressure exists. This paper draws on reference group theory to explain the above phenomenon.
Reference group influence
A reference group is a person or a group that influences another person’s decision. A person uses reference groups as a basis of comparison in forming affective and cognitive responses (Peter and Olson, 1999). Reference group influences has been widely used in the social sciences and numerous researches have examined reference groups in a consumer context. Because of the role of influence, reference groups can greatly impact behaviors. In addition, reference groups are an important source of product information, meaning and brand selection (Bearden and Etzel, 1982;
Childers and Rao, 1992; Escalas and Bettman, 2005;
Moschis, 1985). The use of cultural heroes such as sports or television/film celebrities in endorsing products and services illustrate the belief that individuals who belong to a group to which other individuals aspire enact self-concept development, contribute to the formation of values and attitudes and influence purchase decisions (Bearden and
Etzel, 1982; Childers and Rao, 1992; Sheth et al., 1999).
Reference group influence can be informational, utilitarian and value-expressive (Park and Lessig, 1977). Informational influence provides information to consumers that enhance knowledge or help the individual cope with the environment.
Utilitarian influence occurs when the reference group mediates rewards and punishments. This is especially effective if the person believes his behavior will be public.
Finally, value-expressive influence related to an individuals’ self-concept. Value-expressive reference groups are used to bolster one’s ego. In addition an individual may be influenced by value-expressive groups because he/she likes a particular group. All three types of influence can place pressure on a person to alter their behavior and strengthen or lesson the attitude – behavior inconsistency.
It is expected that reference group will influence green purchase behavior. If a group to which a person belongs or aspires to belong, displays behaviors congruent with their proenvironmental attitude, the individual is under more pressure to conform. In contrast when a reference group does not display congruent behaviors, the individual is less likely to translate their own behavior into action.
In-group identity
Research in social dilemma has demonstrated that enhancing in-group identity promotes cooperation in resource conservation dilemmas (Kerr, 1995). In an experiment,
Dawes et al. (1977) found that when a member of a group had the opportunity to discuss the dilemma with the others, individuals were less likely to defect in pursuit of self interest.
Within group communication fosters a greater likelihood of cooperation because of several factors (Messick and Brewer,
1983):
. establishes group norms and induces conformity pressures; . enhances individual belief that others in the group are committed to cooperate;
. provides the opportunity to persuade members who might originally be inclined to defect by stressing on moral values to enhance collective gain; and
. builds a sense of group identity among members.
However, it has been indicated that the external validity of this finding in the case of real world dilemmas is somewhat limited since in a large collective society, members do not have the opportunity to discuss their choices with each other.
Messick and Brewer (1983) and van Lange et al. (1992) suggest that one way to reap the benefits that stem from group communication is to foster social group identity. Research in the area of social categorization (Tajfel, 1981) suggests that salience of membership with a group fosters cooperation among its members. It has been suggested that when individuals strongly identify with a group, they are more likely to make decisions that are driven by collective rather than self interests (Kramer and Goldman, 1995). Simple
To buy or not to buy? A social dilemma perspective on green buying
Shruti Gupta and Denise T. Ogden
Journal of Consumer Marketing
Volume 26 · Number 6 · 2009 · 376–391
379
awareness of identity with the group increases conformity pressures to cooperate despite the absence of opportunity to discuss individual choice with the others (also Kollock, 1998).
Individuals who identify with an in-group cooperate because they perceive other group members as trustworthy, honest and cooperative. They feel that their actions are representative of the group which increases awareness of the impact of individual choice by enhancing the degree of personal responsibility to maximize collective gain (Messick and
Brewer, 1983). In addition, the perception of group identity also leads to the feeling of “we-ness” which reinforces social control and self-restraint when faced with the temptation to defect in a resource dilemma situation (van Lange et al.,
1992). Therefore, making group identity salient to the individual or increasing his awareness of his belongingness to the group has been shown to increase cooperation
(Kollock, 1998). Two views explain how group identity influences cooperative behavior in social dilemmas (Kollock,
1998):
1 the process of categorization as suggested by social identity theory (Tajfel, 1981); and
2 belief of interdependency among the members and expectations of reciprocity among others (Yamagishi and
Kiyonari, 2000).
Based on the above discussion, it is suggested that:
H3. In-group identity is a significant discriminating variable between green and non-green buyers.
Expectation of others’ cooperation
Both social dilemma theory and reference group theory suggest a strong interrelationship between people’s expectation of overall cooperation from the group members and their own decision to cooperate (Dawes, 1980; van Lange et al., 1992). In social dilemmas, people are more likely to cooperate if they expect others to do the same and choose to not cooperate in order to protect themselves from being a
“sucker.” Several explanations have been offered to explain the expectation-choice relationship (van Lange et al., 1992):
. social norms play an instrumental role in social dilemma and individuals infer these norms when made aware of others’ expectations;
. the tendency to conform;
. their own intended choice being the cause; and
. post hoc justification to explain choice in terms of others’ expectations. Therefore, it is suggested that:
H4. Expectation of others’ cooperation is a significant discriminating variable between green and non-green buyers. Perceived efficacy
Social dilemma research suggests that cooperation hinges on individuals’ perceived efficacy, “. . .or the extent to which one believes that his or her own contributions help to achieve the collective goals” (van Lange et al., 1992 p. 18). Studies reveal that individuals are less likely to defect or free ride if they believe that their cooperative actions will “make a difference” for the betterment of the common good (Sen et al., 2001).
This construct has also received attention in the environmental behavior literature (Berger and Corbin, 1992;
Ellen et al., 1991; Kinnear et al., 1974). However, despite producing positive results in experimental setting, Olson
(1965) and Kerr (1989) show that perceived efficacy declines with group size and therefore, individuals in a large-scale social dilemma situation (i.e. energy conservation dilemmas) are more likely to defect than to cooperate. Therefore, in addition to previous studies that have examined the direct influence of perceived efficacy to explain green buying behavior (Berger and Corbin, 1992; Ellen et al., 1991;
Kinnear et al., 1974) in large group settings (that suffer from lack of external validity), this study will also examine the indirect influence of perceived efficacy. It is suggested that individuals’ perceived efficacy is likely to interact with their expectations of others’ cooperation in moderating the relationship between pro-environmental attitude and behavioral intent. Specifically, when consumers believe that an individual’s contribution will make an insignificant difference towards energy conservation (i.e. low efficacy) their pro-environmental attitudes will predict purchase intent largely under the influence of their expectations of others’ cooperation. This is because consumers, under low efficacy conditions, are uncertain about the impact of their individual contribution and tend to be more inclined to others’ opinions to guide their own decision (van Lange et al., 1992). However, if consumers feel that their individual contribution will make a significant impact in energy conservation (i.e. high efficacy) then their expectation of others’ cooperation will be less relevant in the attitude-behavior relationship. In other words, when consumers believe that they themselves can make a difference by cooperating, their likelihood of doing so is likely to be less contingent on how others behave (Wiener, 1993).
Thus, it is expected that perceived efficacy will interact with expectation of other’s cooperation in explaining green purchase behavior:
H5a. Perceived efficacy is a significant discriminating variable between green and non-green buyers.
H5b. The moderating influence of the effect of expectation of others’ cooperation on green purchase behavior will be greater when perceived efficacy is low than when it is high.
Factors influencing the costs of cooperation
From a broader perspective, social dilemmas may be conceptualized in the context of social exchange theory
(Thibaut and Kelley, 1959) that makes two assumptions:
1 all social interactions involve some type of bargaining relationships where people trade rewards and costs; and
2 people are always motivated to maximize rewards and minimize costs (Komorita and Parks, 1996).
As a result, social dilemma research acknowledges the pivotal role played by the costs associated with cooperation, where, individuals are required to make tradeoffs between accepting the cost of cooperation and the benefits that their cooperative behavior will produce for the group (Messick and Brewer,
1983). Apart from suggesting the type of costs such as loss of self-esteem, anxiety and guilt (Komorita and Parks, 1996) surprisingly little research has explicitly explored the effects of individual costs on cooperation.
If consumer boycotts are a form of social dilemma (Sen et al., 2001), two factors borrowed from this literature are argued to influence the costs that consumers incur in their decision to cooperate in a resource dilemma. The first factor
To buy or not to buy? A social dilemma perspective on green buying
Shruti Gupta and Denise T. Ogden
Journal of Consumer Marketing
Volume 26 · Number 6 · 2009 · 376–391
380
stems from consumers’ intrinsic preference for the green product: consumers with higher (versus lower) preference for green products will more likely cooperate and subsequently purchase green products. In this situation, the overall preference for the green product reduces the perception of individual cost of cooperating in a social dilemma. A second factor is related to consumers’ perception of substitutability between the conventional and green products. When consumers perceive green and conventional products as substitutes, they will be more likely to purchase green products. In this case, the costs incurred to cooperate (i.e. buy the green product) is minimal if not non-existent to the consumer. However, in the event when green and conventional products are not perceived as substitutes, the likelihood that consumers will defect is high. This is the case because the cost of cooperation by buying a product that is an unacceptable substitute of the conventional version presents a big cost to the individual who will attempt to alleviate this cost by defecting and purchasing the conventional product.
According to social dilemma theory, the decision to not cooperate stems from their unwillingness to being seen as a
“sucker” (see Komorita and Parks, 1996 for review), while the choice to cooperate leads to personal loss on account of “free riders” (i.e. non-cooperators who benefit from others’ cooperation) benefit from individual’s cooperation for collective gain. Research (van Lange et al., 1992) suggests that consumers will be more willing to cooperate, despite free riders, when the costs of cooperation to be incurred by the individual are lower. Thus, when consumers’ cost of cooperation is low (i.e. they perceive green and conventional products as substitutes or their product preference for conventional product is low), they less likely to be concerned about the issue of free riding and subsequently be more willing to cooperate. Conversely, when consumers’ cost of cooperation is high (i.e. no substitutes for conventional products are available or product preference for conventional products is high), they are more likely to be concerned about being perceived as a “sucker,” and will consequently be unwilling to cooperate.
H6a. Perception of substitutability is a significant discriminating variable between green and non-green buyers. H6b. Product preference for green products is a significant discriminating variable between green and non-green buyers. In the next section, the above hypotheses are tested empirically to support the argument made in the paper that the attitude-behavior gap in environmental consumerism exists because it presents a social dilemma to the consumers.
Study design
In order to test the hypothesized relationships, a survey instrument was developed that used scales to measure eight independent and one dependent variable. In addition, sociodemographic data was also collected about the study participants. A pilot study was conducted with 29 undergraduate business students who were debriefed regarding the questionnaire items and the objective of the study. After completing the survey, students provided feedback on instructions that needed additional clarification, section layout and order. Discussion with students allowed the researchers to modify the survey by re-arranging the question order both within a section and in the survey and providing easy to follow instructions to complete each section of the survey. Independent variables
The scales utilized for the study were taken from existing literature. All responses for the interval scales with the exception of in-group identity and social value orientation were made on a seven-point Likert scale ranging from 1 ¼ strongly disagree
(negative) to 7 ¼ strongly agree (positive).
Social value orientation
For this categorical variable, participants had to complete a cognitive task (Van Lange et al., 1997) with nine choice situations where they had to select the most preferred option for themselves. Based on the choices made, participants were classified as co-operators, individualists or competitors. For the purpose of this study, any subject who made six or more consistent choices to be classified as a co-operator was coded as “1”. The others that included individualists and competitors along with participants who made choices that were inconsistent with any of the three social value orientation classification schemes were coded as “0”.
Trust
Four scale items (De Cremer and Stouten, 2003) with two reverse scored were used to measure this construct. Items included statements such as: “I trust that others buy CFLs; I do not trust that others will buy CFLs; I think that others trust me to buy CFLs and, I think that others do not trust me to buy CFLs” were measured on a seven point Likert scale
(1 ¼ strongly disagree and 7 ¼ strongly agree). A Cronbach alpha score of 0.696 showed that the scale was reliable.
In group identity
To measure this variable subjects were asked to read a narrative profile of an average consumer and a celebrity, both committed to purchasing green products. Next, six pairs of circles that visually presented six different representations of identity overlap were presented to the participants (Aron et al.,
1992). In each pair, the blue circle represented the identity of the participant and the red circle represented the regular or celebrity consumer. First, subjects were asked to pick the pair of circles that best expressed their perception of identity overlap between themselves and the average consumer. Next, the similar set of circles was presented and the subjects performed the same choice task to indicate their perception of identity overlap between the self and the celebrity consumer.
For each of the two measures of group identity, subjects who picked pairs 1, 2 and 3 (distantly spaced circles) were coded
“0” (low group identity) while the subjects who selected pairs
4, 5 and 6 (closely overlapping circles) were coded “1” (high group identity).
Expectation of others’ cooperation
Two items (Wiener and Doescher, 1994): “In time, most household consumers will purchase CFLs” and “Most people are willing to make sacrifices to help conserve energy” measured this construct by recording participants’
(dis)agreement on a seven point Likert scale. Cronbach’s alpha score was 0.578.
To buy or not to buy? A social dilemma perspective on green buying
Shruti Gupta and Denise T. Ogden
Journal of Consumer Marketing
Volume 26 · Number 6 · 2009 · 376–391
381
Personal efficacy
Two scale items (Ellen et al., 1991): “There is not much that any one individual can do about energy conservation”, and;
“The energy conservation efforts of one person are useless as long as other people refuse to conserve” were reverse scored on a seven point Likert scale. Cronbach’s alpha for the scale was 0.835.
Substitutability
Perception of whether CFLs and regular light bulbs are substitutable products was measured by asking respondents to indicate their view on a seven point semantic differential scale
(where 1 ¼ completely different and 7 ¼ identical) for a total of four word pairs that included items such as “Are completely different/Identical”, “Not similar at all/Similar”,
“Have few features in common/Have many features in common” and “Are very different/Are not different” (Sujan and Bettman, 1989). Cronbach’s alpha score was 0.896.
Product preference
Three items (Sen et al., 2001) measured on a seven point
Likert scale asked respondents to indicate their preference for
CFL bulbs. Questions asked included: “How much would you say you like or dislike CFLs?”; “When you buy light bulbs, to what extent do you buy CFLs?”; and, “When you buy light bulbs, to what extent are you “loyal” to CFLs?”
A Cronbach alpha score of 0.909 showed that the scale was reliable. Attitude
Pro-environmental attitude was measured by asking respondents to indicate their view towards energy conservation on a seven-point semantic differential scale for a total of seven word pairs that included items such as Bad/
Good, Harmful/Beneficial, Useless/Useful among others.
These seven items from Marketing Scales Handbook (Bruner et al., 2005) were selected for its close relevance for the question being examined. A Cronbach’s alpha score of 0.886 showed that the scale was reliable.
Dependent variable
The dependent variable, green buyer was measured as a binary variable. In order to identify green buyers from nongreen buyers, the survey asked the respondents the following question: “When you buy light bulbs, to what extent do you buy CFLs?” Responses were measured on a seven-point
Likert scale with 1 ¼ Never Buy and 7 ¼ Always Buy. From the above, a dichotomous dummy variable that used the polar extremes approach (Hair et al., 1998) was created with respondents who answered 1 and 2 coded as “0” (non-green buyers) and those who answered 6 and 7 coded as “1” (green buyers). All other responses in the middle were eliminated from the dataset for all further statistical analysis. The rationale for polar extremes approach in building a discriminate model is that there is no clear division of characteristics associated with the middle group. Therefore for this study by eliminating the middle group the two extreme groups provide greater insight into which variables account for the differences between green and non-green buyers. After elimination of the middle group, 190 of the 321 respondents were left in the analysis.
Sample
To test the hypotheses, an internet based survey was administered. An online sample (n ¼ 321) was recruited from an internet panel owned by a private marketing research company. A total of 7,028 subjects were invited to participate through e-mail which provided a link to the web survey from which 26 partially and 321 entirely completed the survey. The subjects represented the general population (and were not exclusively university students), contributing to the external validity of the study results. No specific consumer criteria were used to recruit the sample. From the final sample of 190 participants, an additional 16 cases were not usable and subsequently eliminated from final analysis due to missing data. This reduced the final sample for the study to 174 where
59 were classified as non-green buyers and 115 as green buyers. Further analysis of the demographic data (on the entire dataset, n ¼ 190) show that 75 of the respondents were male
(43.1 per cent) whereas 99 (56.9.4 per cent) were female. In terms of marital status, 58 (33.3 per cent) were single, 85
(48.9 per cent) were married, 22 (12.6 per cent) divorced, three (1.7 per cent) widowed and six (3.4 per cent) indicated other. Regarding education levels, 36 (20.7 per cent) had high school level education, 62 (35.6 per cent) had trade school or some college, 53 (30.4 per cent) had an associate or undergraduate college degree, and 16 (9.2 per cent) had master’s degree or higher. In terms of household income, 52
(29.9 per cent) indicated less than $25,000, 57 (32.8 per cent) had between $25,000 and $50,000, 28 (16.1 per cent) between $51,000 and $75,000, 19 (10.0 per cent) indicated income between $76,000 and $100,000, 10 (5.7 per cent) between $101,000 and $150,000, and 8 (4.6 per cent) indicated household income over $151,000.
Finally, in order to assess the respondents’ comfort level with taking an online survey, each was asked to rate his or her technical proficiency with a computer (1 ¼ Not very proficient, 7 ¼ Very proficient) and to report the number of hours per week spent on line, either for work or personal use.
Results showed a fairly sophisticated technical group
(M ¼ 4.95 in proficiency) who spend an average of 22.95 hours per week on line.
Procedure
The survey instrument presented the invited participants with a brief description of the study, procedure, risks and benefits of participation, duration and offered them the opportunity to indicate their willingness to participate. A total of 321 subjects agreed to participate in the study while 15 subjects declined and subsequently were allowed to exit the survey. Next, each of the 321 subjects were asked to read a description of an incandescent light bulb and compact fluorescent light bulb and shown images of each product. Following this introduction, the survey measured presented all the measurement scales for the predictor variables. The survey instrument was programmed in a way that did not allow the respondent to return to the previous web page. Subjects were also required to respond to all questions in a section in order to progress to the next web page. The last page in the survey recorded socio-demographic data and finally thanked the subjects for their participation in the study.
Analysis
The objective of this paper stems from the general consensus in green marketing that unlike other consumption behaviors, consumer attitude is a weak and insignificant predictor of
To buy or not to buy? A social dilemma perspective on green buying
Shruti Gupta and Denise T. Ogden
Journal of Consumer Marketing
Volume 26 · Number 6 · 2009 · 376–391
382
green buying. Instead, this paper argues that attitude-behavior inconsistency in green buying exists because it presents itself as a social dilemma to consumers. Preliminary analysis of the study generates support for the attitude-behavior inconsistency in environmental consumerism where, the mean of consumer attitude for both the green (M ¼ 6.74) and non-green buyer (M ¼ 6.00) groups were high (measured on a scale of 1-7) though significantly different ( p , 0.05).
Therefore, to discriminate and predict green and non-green buyers, classification with discriminant analysis (SPSS software) was used. Discriminant analysis is a multivariate technique used to study the differences between two groups with respect to two or more independent variables simultaneously (Johnson and Wichern, 1998). The sample size (n ¼ 174) was large enough to place confidence in the results where 20 subjects per variable is considered adequate for a discriminant analysis (Stevens, 1992).
The classification was based on prior probabilities computed on the basis of group size. Missing values were excluded to provide a more robust analysis. The predictor variables used in the study were social dilemma variables
(social value orientation, trust, in-group identity, expectation of others’ cooperation, perceived efficacy) and variables related to costs of cooperation (substitutability and product preference). Two separate discriminant analysis models were built to test the hypotheses. The first analysis examined the predictive ability of the social dilemma and reference group influence variables in effectively discriminating between the green and non-green buyers. The second analysis focused on the costs of cooperation incurred by consumers in the purchase of green products (perception of substitutability and product preference) as the discriminating variables.
Results of discriminant analysis with social dilemma variables Before hypotheses testing, statistical analysis first checked for multicollinearity between the independent variables. One of the key assumptions for discriminant analysis is that high correlation should not exist among the independent variables.
It has been suggested that correlation coefficients among the independent variables should be less than 0.75. Table I shows that this assumption was met. Correlation coefficients ranged from 0.005 to 0.268 indicating that multicollinearity is not a concern in the data.
Additional analysis of the data also revealed no serious violation of the assumptions of independence and multivariate normality. To test the assumption of equal variancecovariance,
Box’s M-test statistic was applied. Though an initial examination of the test statistic, Box’s M ¼ 43.087;
F (21, 52599.285) ¼ 1.963; p ¼ 0.005 suggests a violation of the equal variance-covariance assumption it is important to note that this test might be sensitive to factors other than covariance differences (e.g. sample size) (Hair et al., 1998).
Test results show that the corresponding F value is only
1.963, which indicates that the departure from the null is not large (Noble and Schewe, 2003). In addition with large sample sizes when Box’s M produces unexpected results log determinants of the group covariance matrices provide a better insight. Here, the log determinants are relatively equal at 24.148 and 23.702, a difference of 0.45 that suggests that the two groups are drawn from the same distribution.
Discriminant analysis is robust to the violation of homogeneity of variance assumption as long as there are no extreme outliers in the data (Meyers et al., 2005). An examination of the data revealed no extreme outliners.
Descriptive statistics of the groups (green versus non-green buyers) on the independent variables are shown in Table II.
The discriminant analysis model with the social dilemma and reference group influence variables showed that the overall multivariate relationship was statistically significant at the 0.05 level (Wilks’ L ¼ 0.560; chi square (6, n ¼ 174) ¼ 97.923; p , 0.001 indicating that green buyers differed significantly from non-green buyers in reference to the means of the independent variables. The predictive ability of the independent variables in discrimination was also supported by the statistic reflected in the group centroids (21.230 versus
0.631) that clearly suggests that the two groups are different from each other. The model produced a canonical correlation of 0.663 that indicated that it accounted for 44 per cent of the variance in the dependent variable.
Finally, the univariate F-tests of the discriminant analysis
(Table III) also indicate that significant differences exist between the means of the green and non-green buyers. With the exception of social value orientation, all other independent variables: trust, in-group Identity (both with average consumer and celebrity), expectation of others’ cooperation and perceived efficacy significantly differentiated and predicted membership in the two groups.
Therefore, with the exception of H1, H2-H5a were supported.
Additional support for H2, H3, H4, H5a and lack of support for H1 is seen in the within group correlations between the predictors and the discriminant function as well as the standardized weights (Table IV). Based on these coefficients, expectation of others’ cooperation and trust demonstrated the strongest relationship. Identification with a regular consumer, efficacy and identity with celebrity demonstrated moderate relationships. Social value orientation displayed the weakest relationship.
Further analysis examined the performance of the classification procedure (hit rate) as presented in Table V.
Table I Correlation coefficients between the independent variables
Social
value Trust
In-group identity with average consumer
In-group identity with celebrity
Expectation of others’ cooperation Personal efficacy Social value 1.000 20.083 20.058 20.077 20.065 20.094
Trust 20.083 1.000 0.117 0.112 0.165 0.246
In-group identity with average consumer 20.058 0.117 1.000 0.211 0.005 0.268
In-group identity with celebrity 20.077 0.112 0.211 1.000 0.148 0.225
Expectation of others’ cooperation 20.065 0.165 0.005 0.148 1.000 0.203
Personal efficacy 20.094 0.246 0.268 0.225 0.203 1.000
To buy or not to buy? A social dilemma perspective on green buying
Shruti Gupta and Denise T. Ogden
Journal of Consumer Marketing
Volume 26 · Number 6 · 2009 · 376–391
383
The analysis showed that 85.1 per cent of the cases in the dataset were correctly classified. As a cross-validation, to assess how well the classification procedure would predict in a new sample it is estimated that 83.3 per cent of the cases would be accurately classified using the leave-one-out technique. In order to take into account chance agreement a Kappa coefficient showed a value of 1.00 indicating high accuracy in prediction ( p , 0.000) Since the group sizes were unequal, the proportional chance criterion was used to calculate the chance classification. Hair et al. (1998) suggests that the classification should be at least 25 per cent greater than chance for the discriminant function to be meaningful for interpretation. The proportional chance criterion is [(59/
174)2 þ (115/174)2] ¼ 55%. In addition the maximum chance criterion was also calculated. This criterion determines if the results exceed the per cent of respondents that would be correctly classified if all observations were assigned to the segment with the greatest probability of occurrence (Noble and Schewe, 2003). Because non-green
Table II Descriptive statistics of the independent variables
Variable
Range of value Green buyers means (n 5 115)
Non-green
buyers means
(n 5 59)
Green buyers standard deviation
(n 5 115)
Non-green buyers standard deviation
(n 5 59)
Social value orientationa 0 and 1 0.6174 0.5593 0.48815 0.50073
Trust 1-7 4.7630 3.3898 1.10811 1.19740
In-group identity with average consumera 0 and 1 0.7913 0.4237 0.40815 0.49839
In-group identity with celebritya 0 and 1 0.4435 0.1356 0.49897 0.34529
Expectation of others’ cooperation 1-7 5.4130 3.8390 1.15330 1.26095
Personal efficacy 1-7 5.7913 4.6017 1.43726 1.46740
Note: a Categorical variable
Table III Univariate F-tests for the independent variables
Variable Wilkes’ L F Significance
Social value orientation 0.997 0.542 0.463
Trust 0.752 56.68 0.000
In-group identity with average consumer 0.864 27.133 0.000
In-group identity with celebrity 0.905 18.012 0.000
Expectation of others’ cooperation 0,716 68.147 0.000
Perceived efficacy 0.867 26.338 0.000
Table IV Standardized coefficients and correlations of predictor variables of the discriminant function
Correlation coefficients Standardized coefficients
Expectation of others’ cooperation 0.601 0.710
Trust 0.485 0.648
In-group identity with average consumer 0.344 0.448
Efficacy 0.093 0.442
In-group identity with celebrity 0.142 0.365
Social value orientation 0.182 0.063
Table V Classification accuracy – social dilemma and reference group influence variables
Classification
Green buyers Non-green buyers
Actual group Number of cases Number Percent Number Percent
Results for the development sample:
Green buyers 115 106 92.2 9 7.8
Non-green buyers 59 42 71.2 17 28.8
Results for the validation sample:
Green buyers 115 104 90.4 11 9.6
Non-green buyers 59 18 30.5 41 69.5
To buy or not to buy? A social dilemma perspective on green buying
Shruti Gupta and Denise T. Ogden
Journal of Consumer Marketing
Volume 26 · Number 6 · 2009 · 376–391
384
buyers occurred 66 per cent of the time (115/174), it would be correct 66 per cent of the time if all observations were assigned to this group (maximum chance criterion). Because the maximum chance criterion is larger than the proportional test criterion, the model is expected to outperform the 66 per cent level. The classification accuracy of 85.1 per cent is substantially higher than the proportional chance criterion
(55 per cent) and the maximum chance criterion (66 per cent). Therefore it can be assumed that the model of the study is accurate.
Finally, to test H5b that suggested that green purchase behavior would be predicted by the interaction between expectation of others’ cooperation and personal efficacy, a logistic regression was conducted. Results from the binary logit model showed that the interaction term was significant generating support for H5b. Results showed a regression coefficient for the interaction term was 0.101, Wald statistic ¼ 33.889, df ¼ 1 and p ¼ 0.000. The Exp(B) for the interaction term was 1.107.
Results of discriminant analysis with costs of cooperation variables
The assumption that there should be no high correlation among the independent variables held true for the two variables considered in the discriminant analysis model with the variables that measured costs of cooperation. The correlation between substitutability and product preference is
0.022 indicating multicollinearity is not an issue. Further analysis of the data revealed no serious violation of the assumptions of independence and multivariate normality. To test the assumption of equal variance-covariance Box’s M-test statistic was applied. Though the result where, Box’s
M ¼ 21.405; F (3, 445252.721) ¼ 7.040; p ¼ 0.001 suggests a violation of the equal variance-covariance assumption similar group log determinants indicate equal variance covariance matrices. The log determinants are relatively equal at 0.018 and 20.283, a difference of 0.27, suggesting that the two groups are drawn from the same distribution. An examination of the data also revealed no extreme outliers; Descriptive statistics of the two groups (green versus non-green buyers) on the independent variables are shown in Table VI.
The discriminant analysis model with the costs of cooperation variables showed that the overall multivariate relationship was statistically significant at the 0.05 level
(Wilks’ L ¼ 0.094; chi square (2, n ¼ 174) ¼ 442.236; p , 0.001 indicating that green users differed significantly from non-green users. Thus, overall the two cost of cooperation variables used in the study were able to discriminate between green and non-green users. Further examination of the group centroids clearly suggests that green and non-green users are different from one another (24.5284 versus 2.227). The model produced a canonical correlation of
0.952 that indicated that it accounted for 91 per cent of the variance in the dependent variable.
Finally, the univariate F-tests of the discriminant analysis
(Table VII) showed that significant differences exists in the means of product preference between green and non-green buyers with no difference between the means for substitutability between the two groups. Therefore, the results supported H6b but failed to generate support for
H6a. Additional support for H6b and lack of support for H6a is seen in the within group correlations between the predictors and the discriminant function as well as the standardized weights (Table VIII). Based on these coefficients product preference demonstrated the strongest relationship.
Substitutability displayed a very weak relationship.
Additional analysis examined the performance of the classification procedure (hit rate) as presented in Table IX.
The analysis showed that 99.5 per cent of the cases in the dataset were correctly classified. As a cross-validation, to assess how well the classification procedure would predict in a new sample it is estimated that 99.5 per cent of the cases would be accurately classified using the leave-one-out technique. In this case, the classification accuracy 99.5 per cent is substantially higher than the proportional chance criterion (55 per cent) and the maximum chance criterion (66 per cent), indicating an accurate model.
Discussion and contribution
Research in the area of consumer attitude suggests that people behave in ways consistent with their attitudes. However, research in environmental consumerism has produced inconclusive evidence in support of attitude theory. The goal of this research was to explain the attitude-behavior inconsistency in green buying by framing it as a social dilemma to provide an understanding of why consumers do
Table VI Descriptive statistics of the independent variables – costs of cooperation
Variable
Range of value Green buyers means (n 5 115)
Non-green
buyers means
(n 5 59)
Green buyers standard deviation
(n 5 115)
Non-green buyers standard deviation
(n 5 59)
Substitutability 1-7 3.01 3.05 1.62 1.27
Product preference 1-7 6.45 2.29 0.53 0.80
Table VII Univariate F-tests for the independent variables – costs of cooperation Variable Wilks’ L F Significance
Substitutability 1.00 0.024 0.877
Product preference 0.094 1811.653 0.000
Table VIII Standardized coefficients and correlations of predictor variables of the discriminant function – costs of cooperation
Correlation
coefficients
Standardized
coefficients
Substitutability 20.026 20.004
Product preference 1.0 1.0
To buy or not to buy? A social dilemma perspective on green buying
Shruti Gupta and Denise T. Ogden
Journal of Consumer Marketing
Volume 26 · Number 6 · 2009 · 376–391
385
not purchase green products despite a positive attitude toward the environment. By drawing on theories of social dilemma and reference group influence, this article sheds light on the processes that underlie above consumer resistance (Sen et al.,
2001). In summary, this paper suggests that the decision to purchase green products presents itself as a social dilemma influenced by reference group effects and is driven by the motivation to maximize collective rather than individual gain.
This article contributes to the understanding of cooperation in social dilemmas in several ways. First, though research in social dilemma literature has suggested that group characteristics i.e. size, cohesiveness and communication impact the likelihood to cooperate, this paper examines the nature of group influence on individual cooperation. By drawing on reference group theory, results from this study show that reference groups influence individual cooperation.
Results from the study reveal that several characteristics of the individual – trust, in-group identity, expectation of others’ cooperation and perceived efficacy – were significant in differentiating between “non-green” and “green” buyers.
Only one of the variables – social value orientation of the individual – was ineffective in discriminating between the two groups. Findings from the study indicate that green buyers generally are high trusters and expect that others would also engage in green buying behavior. This means that people who exhibit high trust levels will be more likely to buy green products because they believe that others will do the same.
This finding supports the research in social dilemma which argues that high trusters are more likely to cooperate than low trusters who are more likely to defect as a result of the tendency to maximize self gain (van Lange et al., 1992).
In-group identity was also significant in discriminating between green and non-green consumers. This finding supports the research in the area of social categorization
(Tajfel, 1981) which suggests that individuals who strongly identify with a group are more likely to make decisions or perform behaviors that benefit the group rather than themselves (Komorita and Parks, 1996; Kramer and
Goldman, 1995). In such situations, despite an absence of the opportunity to discuss individual choice with the group members, simple awareness of identity with the group enhance conformity pressures to comply with group choice
(Kollock, 1998). Results from the study showed that individuals who closely identified with representatives of the green consumer segment – an average green consumer and a celebrity were more likely to cooperate by making green purchase decision. This finding on the predictive influence of in-group identity on green purchase behavior is also explained by research (i.e. Childers and Rao, 1992) which suggests that consumption decisions are influenced by reference groups.
Expectation of others cooperation was the strongest factor that discriminated between green and non-green buyers. The green buyer made cooperative decision because they expect others to do the same. The non-green buyer has no such expectation, does not feel the pressure to conform and therefore chooses to not cooperate in a social dilemma. This finding is supported by research in social dilemma that suggests that people are more likely to cooperate when they expect others to do the same and avoid cooperation to protect them from being perceived as a sucker.
A surprising finding was that social value orientation was not significant in separating green and non-green buyers.
Results from the study showed that more than half of each group members identified themselves as co-operators. An explanation for why an unexpectedly significant proportion of non-green buyers identified themselves as co-operators could be attributed to social desirability bias. In order to measure an individuals’ social value orientation, study participants were asked to perform nine choice tasks which measured the tendency to maximize self or collective gain. It might be argued that since this choice task was performed in a hypothetical situation with no personal gain or loss participants might have answered in a socially desirable way instead of choices that are made in a real situation.
Perceived efficacy is the extent to which an individual believes his/her actions make a difference in achieving collective goals
(van Lange et al., 1992). This construct was significant and showed moderate influence in discriminating between green and non-green buyers. While both groups believe their actions make a difference, Green buyers have much stronger beliefs
(M ¼ 5.8) than non-green buyers (M ¼ 4.6). Green buyers are less likely to defect in order to maximize self gain because they believe that their cooperation action will contribute to the betterment of the common good (Sen et al., 2001). The predictive influence of perceived efficacy is also supported by research that has received much attention in environmental consumerism literature (Ellen et al., 1991; Kinnear et al., 1974).
Second, this paper extends the assertion made by social dilemma research that highlights the predictive role of perceived efficacy and expectation of others’ cooperation on the likelihood to cooperate. Results from this study reveal an interaction between the two factors (Sen et al., 2001) beyond its independent influence to impact cooperation in social dilemma situations. Results from this study show that when perceived efficacy is low the influence of the effect of others cooperation on green purchase behavior is high. Under low efficacy conditions consumers are not certain about the
Table IX Classification accuracy – costs of co-operation variables
Classification
Green buyers Non-green buyers
Actual group Number of cases Number Per cent Number Per cent
Results for the development sample:
Green buyers 125 125 100 0 0
Non-green buyers 65 64 98.5 1 1.5
Results for the validation sample:
Green buyers 125 125 100 0 0
Non-green buyers 65 64 98.5 1 1.5
To buy or not to buy? A social dilemma perspective on green buying
Shruti Gupta and Denise T. Ogden
Journal of Consumer Marketing
Volume 26 · Number 6 · 2009 · 376–391
386
impact of their actions and tend to use others’ opinions to guide their behavior. At the same time, when people believe their actions matter the likelihood of buying green products is less contingent on how others behave.
Third, the results of this study supports earlier research by
Sen et al. (2001) that identifies the influence of costs of cooperation on the individual decision to cooperate or withhold consumption. However, results from this study showed that only one of the two costs of cooperation variables was significant and contributed to the discrimination between green and non-green buyers. Specifically, the results showed that green buyers overwhelmingly exhibited a higher level of product preference for the green product than non-buyers.
Unexpectedly, substitutability was not significant in contributing to the discrimination between green and nongreen buyers (see Sen et al., 2001). Results from the study showed that both the green and non-green buyers perceived the green and conventional products as different and hence not substitutable. The findings that failed to support the role of substitutability on the individual decision to cooperate suggests that marketing practice that highlight the parity between the green and conventional product might be ineffective and cooperation in such social dilemmas might require resorting to a structural solution.
Results from this study provide important information to help marketers accurately identify the green buyer segment.
The results of the study offer several managerial implications.
First, marketers should reinforce the role trust plays in solidifying collective action. Namely using themes such as “you can trust that others will follow your lead” may strengthen the trust consumers feel and subsequently weaken the desire to defect. By relaying messages that show the extent of others’ action, consumers will feel confident that their trust is warranted. Such marketing communication will also persuade
“low trusters” that it is worthy to believe others are engaging in pro-environmental behavior. Second, because of the strong influence of reference groups in green buying, marketing communications managers should use spokespeople who are relatable. In the event that a green marketer decides to use a celebrity, that person must be perceived as a role model for green behavior. On a related note, since study results showed that identification with a “regular” green consumer was stronger than with a “green” celebrity in discriminating between green and non-green buyers it is an indication for companies to save marketing dollars by using typical green consumers instead of celebrity spokespeople in marketing communications. Third, this study showed that expectation of others cooperation significantly identifies green buyers. For marketers, this finding presents an opportunity to communicate that other consumers make green choices.
When people believe that other people similar to themselves make cooperative choices by buying green, they will be more likely to buy green as well. Marketing communication material could also display testimonials of green buyers. However, these messages must be constantly reinforced so that the individual does not feel that they are acting alone. Fourth, to address the perception of personal efficacy, it is important that green marketers emphasize the difference that individual action makes for the collective good. When people realize that their individual green behavior matter, they will be less likely to defect. Therefore in developing marketing messages, emphasis should be placed on how an individual’s actions can contribute to the collective gain. These messages will reinforce beliefs that actions do matter because it makes them feel good about their actions and in turn strengthens perceived efficacy. Also when people believe their actions matter, the likelihood of buying green products becomes less contingent on how others behave.
This indicates that people with strong levels of perceived efficacy can become role models for others who are more susceptible to reference group pressure.
Substitutability was not a significant discriminating factor in the analysis indicating that both green buyers and nongreen buyers did not perceive the green product (CFL light bulbs) and the conventional product (incandescent light bulbs) as substitutable. Both groups perceived the CFL and regular incandescent light bulbs as significantly different from each other. For green marketers, this result highlights the need to stress the benefits of green products. In the case of
CFLs, positioning on product superiority, cost savings, energy conservation, and the lessened frequency of switching light bulbs are areas that could be stressed. In addition, marketers should shift focus from price of the green product to consumer value.
Finally, by using real consumers instead of student populations as is common practice in academic research this study increases the external validity of study results. Thus, green marketers can confidently use key findings of this study to not only identify the green buyer segment but also use this knowledge to make appropriate modifications to marketing strategy to target and subsequently convert non-green buyers to green buyers.
Limitations and future research
Although the strengths of the study outweigh the limitations, this study has potential methodological limitations that also present opportunities for future research. Many of this article’s limitations are those found in survey research. For example, the internet was utilized as a delivery mechanism for the survey. Thus, people who do not have a computer or who do not feel comfortable using a computer would not be included in the survey. Future research should widen the base of participants by using other methods to investigate green buying behavior. In survey research much of the validity depends on a subject’s ability to accurately assess their level of agreement with questions. Although analysis does not indicate that this is a problem, there may be some level of measurement error. A third limitation of the study was the single product used to measure green buying. The external validity of this study could be increased by future research that seeks to replicate findings of this study by investigating the consumption of other green products.
Other suggestions for future research are tied to the study results. Findings from this research showed that in-group identity is a valuable factor that influences green buying. A future research project might seek to investigate this influence further by examining the concept of source credibility. That is, in addition to identifying with a person how does credibility of the source help or hinder the effectiveness of the message to promote green buying. As suggested earlier, the lack of support for the influence of social value orientation in green buying may be attributed to the way the construct was measured in this study. Future research could develop alternative measurement tools that are more effective in capturing the individual’s social value orientation and address any social desirability bias in participant response. A third limitation of the study is tied to the results for the variable –
To buy or not to buy? A social dilemma perspective on green buying
Shruti Gupta and Denise T. Ogden
Journal of Consumer Marketing
Volume 26 · Number 6 · 2009 · 376–391
387
trust. Social dilemma research argues that despite showing that individual personality differences (level of trust) explain cooperative or defective choices, this knowledge is not very useful as a solution to dilemmas (Kollock, 1998) because it does not shed light on how to increase the level of cooperation. Finally, Messick and Brewer (1983) offer two different types of solutions for a social dilemma: structural approaches that involve changing the structure of the social dilemma and individual solutions that attempt to alter individual choice. These solutions include offering rewards to promote cooperative behavior and enforce punishment or penalty for non-cooperative behavior. Future research in environmental consumerism could investigate which structural and individual solutions would be most effective in achieving its cooperative behavior for collective gain.
Future research should examine the interplay of social dilemma variable and product features on green buying.
While energy conservation is a social good – it may be that other aspects of the product affect choice (i.e. aesthetics, safety, price, ease of replacement) and this may be productdependent.
Conclusion
The conceptual framework offered in this paper directly responds to the need in environmental consumerism literature to explain attitude-behavior inconsistency. The framework presented in this paper contributes to the environmental consumerism literature by framing the attitude-behavior gap as a social dilemma and draws on reference group theory to identify individual factors to help understand the gap and suggest ways to bridge it. Study results are valuable to both practitioners and theoreticians in their effort to better understand the environmentally conscious consumers. For theoreticians, findings from hypotheses testing show that the likelihood purchasing green products depends on certain individual characteristics: trust in others, reference group influence (in-group identity, expectation of others’ cooperation, perceived efficacy) and factors that influence the costs of cooperation to the individual (product substitutability and product preference). Since the success of green products is contingent on consumers’ tendency to cooperate rather than to defect, it is imperative for green marketers to understand which individual factors encourage cooperation. For green marketers, therefore, the findings from the study help develop marketing strategy that persuades consumers to seek the value of collective gain over self-interest.
References
Ajzen, I. (1988), Attitudes, Personality and Behavior, Dorsey,
Chicago, IL.
Ajzen, I. (1989), “Attitude structure and behavior”, in Pratkanis, A.R., Breckler, S.J. and Greenwald, A.G.
(Eds), Attitude, Structure and Function, Erlbaum, Hillsdale,
NJ.
Ajzen, I. (1991), “The theory of planned behavior”,
Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes,
Vol. 50, pp. 179-211.
Ajzen, I. (2002), “Perceived behavioral control, self-efficacy, locus of control, and the theory of planned behavior”,
Journal of Applied Social Psychology, Vol. 32 No. 4, pp. 665-83.
Ajzen, I. and Fishbein, M. (1977), “Attitude-behavior relations: a theoretical analysis and review of the research”, Psychology Bulletin, Vol. 84, pp. 888-918.
Ajzen, I. and Fishbein, M. (1980), Understanding Attitudes and
Predicting Social Behavior, Prentice-Hall, Englewood Cliffs,
NJ.
Arbuthnot, J. (1977), “The roles of attitudinal and personality variables in the prediction of environmental behavior and knowledge”, Environment and Behavior, Vol. 9 No. 2, pp. 217-32.
Aron, A., Aron, E.N. and Smollan, D. (1992), “Inclusion of other in the self scale and the structure of interpersonal closeness”, Journal of Personal and Social Psychology, Vol. 63, pp. 596-612.
Bandura, A. (1982), “Self-efficacy mechanism in human agency”, American Psychologist, Vol. 37 No. 1, pp. 122-47.
Bearden, W.O. and Etzel, M.J. (1982), “Reference group influence on product and brand purchase decisions”,
Journal of Consumer Research, Vol. 9 No. 2, pp. 183-94.
Berger, I.E. and Corbin, R.M. (1992), “Perceived consumer effectiveness and faith in others as moderators of environmentally responsible behaviors”, Journal of Public
Policy and Marketing, Vol. 11 No. 2, pp. 79-89.
Bruner, G.C. II, Hensel, P.J. and James, K.E. (2005),
Marketing Scales Handbook, Thomson/South Western,
Chicago, IL.
Cacioppo, J.T., Petty, R.E., Kao, C.F. and Rodriguez, R.
(1986), “Central and peripheral routes to persuasion: an individual difference perspective”, Journal of Personal and
Social Psychology, Vol. 51, pp. 1032-43.
Childers, T.L. and Rao, A.R. (1992), “The influence of familial and peer-based reference groups on consumer decisions”, Journal of Consumer Research, Vol. 19 No. 2, pp. 198-212.
Conner, M. and Armitage, C.J. (1998), “Extending the theory of planned behavior: a review and avenue for further research”, Journal of Applied Social Psychology, Vol. 28
No. 15, pp. 1429-64.
Davidson, A.R. and Jaccard, J.J. (1979), “Variables that moderate the attitude-behavior relations: results of a longitudnal survey”, Journal of Personal and Social
Psychology, Vol. 37, pp. 1364-76.
Davidson, A.R., Yantis, S., Norwood, M. and Montano, D.E.
(1985), “Amount of information about the attitude object and attitude behavior consistency”, Journal of Personal and
Social Psychology, Vol. 49, pp. 1184-98.
Dawes, R.M. (1980), “Social dilemmas”, Annual Review of
Psychology, Vol. 31, pp. 169-93.
Dawes, R.M., McTavish, J. and Shaklee, H. (1977),
“Behavior, communication and assumptions about people’s behavior in common dilemma situations”,
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, Vol. 35 No. 1.
De Cremer, D. and Stouten, J. (2003), “When do people find cooperation most justified? The effect of trust and self-other merging in social dilemmas”, Social Justice Research, Vol. 16
No. 1, pp. 41-52.
Ellen, P.S., Wiener, J.L. and Cobb-Walgren, C. (1991),
“The role of perceived consumer effectiveness in motivating environmentally conscious behaviors”, Journal of Public
Policy and Marketing, Vol. 10 No. 2, pp. 102-17.
Escalas, J.E. and Bettman, J.R. (2005), “Self-construal, reference groups, and brand meaning”, Journal of Consumer
Research, Vol. 32, December, pp. 378-89.
To buy or not to buy? A social dilemma perspective on green buying
Shruti Gupta and Denise T. Ogden
Journal of Consumer Marketing
Volume 26 · Number 6 · 2009 · 376–391
388
Fazio, R.H. (1986), “How do attitudes guide behavior?”, in Sorrentino, R.M. and Higgins, E.T. (Eds), The Handbook of Motivation and Cognition: Foundation of Social Behavior,
Guilford Press, New York, NY.
Fazio, R.H. (1989), “On the power and function of attitudes: the role of attitude accessibility”, in Pratkanis, A.R.,
Breckler, S.J. and Greenwald, A.G. (Eds), Attitude Structure and Function, Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ.
Fazio, R.H. and Zanna, M.P. (1981), “Direct experience and attitude-behavior consistency”, in Berkowitz, L. (Ed.),
Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, Vol. 14,
Academic Press, New York, NY.
Hair, J.F. Jr, Anderson, R.E., Tatham, R.L. and Black, W.C.
(1998), Multivariate Data Analysis, Prentice-Hall,
Englewood Cliffs, NJ.
Hardin, G. (1968), “The tragedy of the commons”, Science,
Vol. 162, December, pp. 1243-8.
Heberlein, T.A. and Black, J.S. (1976), “Attitudinal specificity and the prediction of behavior in a field setting”, Journal of
Personal and Social Psychology, Vol. 33, pp. 474-9.
Hines, J.M., Hungerford, H.R. and Tomera, A.N. (1986-
1987), “Analysis and synthesis of research on responsible environmental behavior: a meta-analysis”, The Journal of
Environmental Education, Vol. 18, pp. 1-18.
Jay, L. (1990), “Green about the tills: markets discover the eco-consumer”, Management Review, Vol. 79 No. 6, p. 24.
Johnson, R.A. and Wichern, D.W. (1998), Applied
Multivariate Statistical Analysis, Prentice-Hall, Englewood
Cliffs, NJ.
Kaiser, F.G., Wolfing, S. and Fuhrer, U. (1999),
“Environmental attitude and ecological behavior”, Journal of Environmental Psychology, Vol. 19, pp. 1-19.
Kellgren, C.A. and Wood, W. (1986), “Access to attituderelevant information in memory as a determinant of attitude-behavior consistency”, Journal of Experimental
Pscyhology, Vol. 22, pp. 328-38.
Kerr, N.L. (1989), “Illusions of efficacy: the effects of group size on perceived efficacy in social dilemmas”, Journal of
Experimental Social Pscyhology, Vol. 25, pp. 287-313.
Kerr, N.L. (1995), “Norms in social dilemma”, in Schroeder, D.A. (Ed.), Social Dilemmas: Perspectives on
Individuals and Groups, Praeger Publishers, Westport, CT.
Kinnear, T.C., Taylor, J.R. and Ahmed, S.A. (1974),
“Ecologically concerned consumers – who are they?”,
Journal of Marketing, Vol. 38 No. 2, pp. 20-4.
Kollock, P. (1998), “Social dilemmas: the anatomy of cooperation”, Annual Review of Sociology, Vol. 24, pp. 183-214.
Komorita, S.S. and Parks, C.D. (1996), Social Dilemmas,
Westview Press, Boulder, CO.
Kramer, R.M. and Goldman, L. (1995), “Helping the group or helping yourself? Social motives and group identity in resource dilemmas”, in Schroeder, D.A. (Ed.), Social
Dilemmas: Perspectives on Individuals and Groups, Praeger
Publishers, Westport, CT.
Mainieri, T., Barnett, E.G., Valdero, T.R., Unipan, J.B. and
Oskamp, S. (1997), “Green buying: the influence of environmental concern on consumer behavior”, Journal of
Social Psychology, Vol. 137 No. 2, pp. 189-204.
Messick, D.M. and Brewer, M.B. (1983), “Solving social dilemmas”, in Wheeler, L. and Shaver, P. (Eds), Review of
Personality and Social Psychology, Vol. 4, Sage, Beverly Hills,
CA.
Messick, D.M. and McClintock, C.G. (1968), “Motivational basis of choice in experimental games”, Journal of
Experimental Social Psychology, Vol. 4, pp. 1-25.
Meyers, L.S., Garnst, G. and Guarino, A.J. (2005), Applied
Multivariate Research: Design and Implementation, Sage,
Thousand Oaks, CA.
Mintel (2006), Green Living, US Marketing Research Report,
London.
Moschis, G.P. (1985), “The role of family communication in consumer socialization of children and adolescents”,
Journal of Consumer Research, Vol. 11, March, pp. 898-913.
Noble, S.M. and Schewe, C.D. (2003), “Cohort segmentation: an exploration of its validity”, Journal of
Business Research, Vol. 56, pp. 978-87.
Norman, R. (1975), “Affective-cognitive consistency, attitudes, conformity and behavior”, Journal of Personal and Social Psychology, Vol. 32, pp. 83-91.
Olson, M. (1965), The Logic of Collective Action, Harvard
University Press, Cambridge, MA.
Osterhus, T.L. (1997), “Pro-social consumer influence strategies: when and how do they work?”, Journal of
Marketing, Vol. 61 No. 4, pp. 16-29.
Ottman, J. (1992), “Sometimes, consumers will pay more to go green”, Marketing News, Vol. 26 No. 6, p. 16.
Park, C.W. and Lessig, V.P. (1977), “Students and housewives: differences in susceptibility to reference group influence”, Journal of Consumer Research, Vol. 4, June, pp. 102-10.
Peter, P.J. and Olson, J.C. (1999), Consumer Behavior and
Marketing Strategy, 5th ed., Irwin/McGraw-Hill, Boston,
MA.
Petty, R.E., Unnava, R. and Strathman, A.J. (1991),
“Theories of attitude change”, in Robertson, T.S. and
Kassarjian, H.H. (Eds), Handbook of Consumer Behavior,
Prentice-Hall, Englewood Cliffs, NJ.
Roper Organization (1992), Environmental Behavior, North
America: Canada, Mexico, United States, S.C. Johnson and
Son, Inc., Racine, WI.
RoperASW (2002), Green Gauge Report 2002, Roper
Organization, New York, NY.
Schlossberg, H. (1991), “Americans passionate about the environment? Critic says that”, Marketing News, Vol. 25, pp. 8-10.
Schultz, C.J.H. and Holbrook, M.B. (1999), “Marketing and the tragedy of the commons: a synthesis, commentary and analysis for action”, Journal of Public Policy and Marketing,
Vol. 18 No. 2, pp. 218-29.
Sen, S., Gurhan-Canli, Z. and Morwitz, V. (2001),
“Withholding consumption: a social dilemma perspective on consumer boycotts”, Journal of Consumer Research,
Vol. 28 No. 3, pp. 399-417.
Sheth, J.N., Mittal, B. and Newman, B. (1999), Customer
Behavior: Consumer Behavior and Beyond, Dryden, New
York, NY.
Smith, R.E. and Swinyard, W.R. (1983), “Attitude-behavior consistency: the impact of product trial versus advertising”,
Journal of Marketing Research, Vol. 20, pp. 257-67.
Stevens, J. (1992), Applied Multivariate Statistics of the Social
Sciences, Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Hillsdale, NJ.
Sujan, M. and Bettman, J.R. (1989), “The effects of brand positioning strategies on consumers’ brand and category perceptions: some insights from schema research”, Journal of Marketing Research, Vol. 26, November, pp. 454-67.
To buy or not to buy? A social dilemma perspective on green buying
Shruti Gupta and Denise T. Ogden
Journal of Consumer Marketing
Volume 26 · Number 6 · 2009 · 376–391
389
Tajfel, H. (1981), Human Groups and Social Categories,
Cambridge University Press, Cambridge.
Tanner, C. and Kast, S.W. (2003), “Promoting sustainable consumption: determinants of green purchases by Swiss consumers”, Psychology and Marketing, Vol. 20 No. 10, pp. 883-902.
Thibaut, J.W. and Kelley, H.H. (1959), The Social Psychology of Groups, Wiley, New York, NY.
Tracy, A.P. and Oskamp, S. (1983-1984), “Relationships among ecologically responsible behaviors”, Journal of
Environmental Systems, Vol. 13 No. 2, pp. 115-26. van Lange, P.A.M., Liebrand, W.B.G., Messick, D.M. and
Wilke, H.A.M. (1992), “Social dilemmas: the state-of-theart”, in Liebrand, W.B.G., Messick, D.M. and Wilke,
H.A.M. (Eds), Social Dilemmas: Theoretical Issues and
Research Findings, Pergamon Press, Oxford.
Van Lange, P.V.M., Otten, W., De Bruin, E.M.N. and
Joireman, J.A. (1997), “Development of prosocial, individualistic, and competitive orientations: theory and preliminary evidence”, Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, Vol. 73 No. 4, pp. 733-46.
Webster, F.E. (1975), “Determining the characteristics of the socially conscious consumer”, Journal of Consumer Research,
Vol. 2 No. 3, pp. 188-96.
Weigel, R.H. (1983), Environmental Attitudes and the Prediction of Behavior, Praeger, New York, NY.
Weigel, R.H., Vernon, D.T.A. and Tognacci, L.N. (1976),
“Specificity of the attitude as a determinant of attitudebehavior congruence”, Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, Vol. 30, pp. 724-8.
Wicker, A.W. (1969), “Attitudes versus actions: the relationship of verbal and overt behavior response to attitude objects”, Journal of Social Issues, Vol. 25 No. 4, pp. 41-78.
Wiener, J.L. (1993), “What makes people sacrifice their freedom for the good of their community?”, Journal of
Public Policy and Marketing, Vol. 12, Fall, pp. 244-51.
Wiener, J.L. and Doescher, T.A. (1994), “Cooperation and expectation of cooperation”, Journal of Public Policy and
Marketing, Vol. 13 No. 2, pp. 259-71.
Yamagishi, T. (1988), “Seriousness of social dilemma and the provision of a sanctioning system”, Social Psychogical
Quarterly, Vol. 51 No. 1, pp. 32-42.
Yamagishi, T. and Kiyonari, T. (2000), “The group as the container of generalized reciprocity”, Social Psychology
Quarterly, Vol. 63, pp. 116-32.
About the authors
Shruti Gupta is an assistant professor of Marketing at The
Pennsylvania State University at Abington. Her research interests include corporate social responsibility and consumer behavior. Shruti Gupta is the corresponding author and can be contacted at: sxg37@psu.edu
Denise T. Ogden is an associate professor of Marketing at
The Pennsylvania State University (Lehigh Valley Campus).
Her research interests include retailing, multi-cultural aspects of marketing and integrated marketing communication.
Executive summary and implications for managers and executives
This summary has been provided to allow managers and executives a rapid appreciation of the content of this article. Those with a particular interest in the topic covered may then read the article in toto to take advantage of the more comprehensive description of the research undertaken and its results to get the full benefits of the material present.
Plenty of evidence exists to suggest that consumer attitude typically influences subsequent purchase activities. In relation to environmentally-friendly consumption behavior, however, findings are inconclusive. For instance, research has revealed instances where consumers show concern for environmental issues yet remain averse to buy or pay high prices for green products. On other occasions, willingness to engage in such purchase behavior prevails despite indifference to the environment. What makes consumers go green?
In light of this apparent discord between pro-environment attitude and associated purchase behavior, it remains difficult for marketers to effectively target the green consumer segment. The present study therefore investigates various key factors with the aim being to identify what differentiates green and non-green buyers.
Previous study has indicated that attitude is an accurate predictor of behavior when certain conditions prevail. These include personality type, consistency between attitudes and beliefs, and attitude shaped through profound knowledge and/ or personal experience of the issue concerned. In certain quarters it has also been argued that simultaneous measurement of attitude and behavior is necessary for accurate predictions to occur.
Several theories have been employed to ascertain conditions when attitude will more reliably predict behavior. This has led to various claims that include:
. a person’s attitude towards a particular behavior together with his or her perception of what others think influences the degree of intention to perform that behavior;
. people are generally more akin to engage in behaviors considered easier to perform than those they perceive as difficult and having less control over;
. the influence of attitude on behavior is spontaneous rather than intentional and depends on the “attitude object” being present for the response to occur;
. researchers should measure attitude to a specific behavior rather than assuming that attitude is reflective of all environmental behaviors; and
. personal situational factors influence the attitude-behavior relationship. Scholars have pointed out that consumers face a “social dilemma” in respect of having to choose between options that either benefit themselves as an individual or the collective whole. Defecting is claimed to be the “optimal choice” as selfinterests can be protected no matter what course the rest of the group decide to adopt. In contrast, the outcome of a “cooperative choice” still depends on the action of others. While more immediate gains may be achieved by the individual, the group benefits in the long run from a cooperative choice.
However, it compounds the dilemma further to realize that benefit to the individual is invariably lower than damage to
To buy or not to buy? A social dilemma perspective on green buying
Shruti Gupta and Denise T. Ogden
Journal of Consumer Marketing
Volume 26 · Number 6 · 2009 · 376–391
390
the whole group when a “non-co-operative” choice is taken.
Social dilemma has been further described in terms of “social value orientation” whereby participation/non-participation in a certain behavior indicates what proportion of self and group interests the individual aims to serve.
Many other related factors are believed to influence consumer behavior. These are:
. Trust. The higher the trust in others, the more likely the individual will cooperate.
. Pressure to conform. Reference group influence has attracted considerable attention from researchers and it is accepted that this can profoundly impact on behavior.
Marketers seek to increase this influence by using sporting heroes or celebrities to endorse certain products or behaviors in the knowledge that some consumers will develop aspirations of belonging to the group.
. Group identity. Here the premise is that closeness within a group establishes group norms and identity and raises belief in a collective commitment to behave in a certain way. It is assumed that strong group identity makes individuals more likely to consider collective interests before their own.
. Expectation that others will cooperate. People’s desire to cooperate is stronger when they expect others to behave likewise. . Perceived efficacy. According to research, deciding to cooperate depends on individual perception of how much difference to the whole his or her effort will make. Some studies have shown that perceived efficacy declines as group size increases. There is also belief that when perceived efficacy is low its effect on behavior will be moderated by expectation that others will cooperate.
In addition, social dilemma has been described in terms of social exchange theory whereby cooperation is considered in relation to anticipated benefits and costs of engaging in green purchase behavior. Cost is low when green products and conventional alternatives are perceived as substitutes for each other. When this perception is absent, the cost of cooperation is higher and the consumer is more likely to shun the green product and buy a conventional one instead. Another cost issue that influences decision-making is the knowledge that non-co-operators still benefit when others cooperate. But this concern over such freeloaders diminishes if green and conventional products are considered interchangeable.
Survey and results
Gupta and Ogden explore these issues in an internet based survey of a sample obtained from a marketing research company. Participants reflected the general population and
174 useable responses were obtained.
The group was shown descriptions and images of incandescent light bulbs and compact fluorescent light bulbs
(CFLs) and asked to respond to various statements and complete tasks that enabled measurement of the variables under consideration. Response to a question concerning the purchase of CFLs helped was used to identify 115 green buyers and 59 non-green buyers.
General findings from this study are that:
. reference groups influence individual cooperation;
. green buyers typically rate higher in trust and expect others to participate in green purchase activities;
. those who strongly identify with the group are likely to put collective interests before their own because of added pressure to conform with group norms;
. expectation that others will conform was the most powerful differentiator of green and non-green buyers;
. perceived efficacy was also shown to exert moderate influence in discriminating between the respective segments; and
. green buyers showed a higher rating for product preference than non-green buyers.
Contrary to expectation, the social value orientation construct and product interchangeability did not appear significant.
Conversely, both consumer groups regarded green and nongreen products as different.
Marketing implications and future study
Based on this research, marketers are urged to:
. highlight differences rather than similarities between green and non-green products and emphasize benefits associated with purchasing the green product or service in question;
. reinforce the role of trust in strengthening collective action; . use role models to increase reference group effect.
Celebrity endorsement is encouraged although evidence suggests that typical green consumers are even more effective spokespeople;
. communicate and reinforce the message that others are making green choices and include testimonials; and
. stress that individual action can and does lead to collective gain. People who believe their behavior matters may act without waiting to consider the actions of others.
Using a single product to measure green consumption behavior is acknowledged as a study limitation and similar investigation of other green products is recommended. Gupta and Ogden also accept that conducting the study online may have served to exclude those with limited proficiency in computer use. They likewise suggest that the apparent lack of influence of social value orientation in this study may be at least in part due to how it was measured. The conclusion here is that developing alternative measures may prove more effective. Another area worthy of further exploration is whether and how product features might influence the relationship between social dilemma and green buying.
(A pre´cis of the article “To buy or not to buy – a social dilemma perspective on green buying”. Supplied by Marketing Consultants for Emerald.)
To buy or not to buy? A social dilemma perspective on green buying
Shruti Gupta and Denise T. Ogden
Journal of Consumer Marketing
Volume 26 · Number 6 · 2009 · 376–391
391
To purchase reprints of this article please e-mail: reprints@emeraldinsight.com
Or visit our web site for further details: www.emeraldinsight.com/reprints

Similar Documents

Premium Essay

Marketing 101 Journal

...1. Marketing- An organizational function and a set of processes for creating, capturing, communicating, and delivering value to customers and for managing customers relationships in ways that benefit the organization and it stakeholders. 2. Marketing Plan- A written document composed of an analysis of the current marketing situation, opportunities and threats for the firm, marketing objectives and strategy specified in terms of the four Ps, action programs, and projected or pro forma income (and other financial) statements. 3. Exchange- The trade of things of value between the buyer and the seller so that each is better off as a result 4. Marketing Mix- Product, price, place, and promotion- the controllable set of activities that a firm uses to respond to the wants of its target markets. 5. Goods- Items that can be physically touched 6. Services- Any intangible offering that involves a deed, performance, or effort that cannot be physically possessed; intangible customer benefits that are produced by people or machines and cannot be separated from the producer 7. Ideas – Intellectual concepts- thoughts, opinions, and philosophies 8. Value- Reflects the relationship of benefits to costs, or what the consumer gets for what he or she gives 9. B2C marketing- The process in which businesses sell to consumers 10. C2C marketing- The process in which consumers sell to other consumers 11. Market-oriented era- From the 1960s onwards...

Words: 934 - Pages: 4

Free Essay

Teacher Assessments

...As a teacher it is your responsibility to not only present new material to the student but to teach them and to verify and monitor what information they are actually obtaining and processing. There are many forms of assessments that teachers can use to evaluate students understanding of lesson material. I will suggest a few methods that I will use in my secondary education mathematics classroom. The first strategy of monitoring I will discuss will be quizzes. There are multiple advantages for this form of testing. First, is that it easily produces recorded data. I can quickly grade multiple choice questions and record the students’ grade. Being able to record this grade will allow me to present scores to the parents when I am asked for a student’s progress. Another advantage to a quiz is the ability to assess both the entire class and each individual’s understanding on a topic. I will be able to look at the overall measurement and decide whether there is a need to review the topic or lesson, or if the class is ready to advance in the given studies. Also, by presenting multiple quizzes through the term the students and I can create a set format, which can help ease classroom anxiety. Another form of monitoring students’ progression through a lesson is homework. Homework can be one of the most effective methods for tracking mathematical progress. Assigning, collecting and grading homework can be a very timely way to gather information on how each student is progressing through...

Words: 1003 - Pages: 5

Free Essay

Journals and Scientific Docs

...Journals and Scientific Documents The purpose of journals and professional papers is to inform the public on a specific subject matter. These writings relate directly to a broad spectrum dependent on the edition or publication specifics. Of professional papers, several types can provide a scope of information and credibility toward a purveyance of technical or detailed information. The structure is usually even flowing and organized with particular information segments set out about the constituency of the writing. These papers, intended for publication in special interest journals, enhance the writers research, writing, and documentation capabilities as some of the information, not being his/her own will need to be accredited as if it is not, than plagiarism will be extrapolated. Journals, usually published by or for a particular subject matter or client, are detailed and accurate. There may be a UCLA journal, National Geographic Journal, or even a mechanic’s journal. The particulars of this, geared toward a particular audience and each entry, will usually comprise two titles sharing a general viewpoint for the nonprofessional and an entry specific title for the professional. The purpose of journals is to inform a group or individual of the credible elements related to a curriculum or scientific, professional, or academic scope. The writings or information in a journal may be composed of professional papers but the opposite is not the same. ...

Words: 583 - Pages: 3

Free Essay

Difference Between Journals and Diaries: -

...Difference between journals and diaries:- The difference between a diary and a journal is that a diary is where you can write down what happened during the day and keep a record of stuff. A journal is to write your own personal feelings in, and things that happened, and how they happened. A diary is a report of what happened during the day—where you ate, who you met, the details that what was happened in the office, and who took whose side. It is a like a newspaper about you. A journal is completely different. A journal is about examining your life. It’s a GPS system for your spirit. Journals lead to insight, growth, and sometimes, achieving a goal. You can keep a journal in anything that feels comfortable and that’s portable–a spiral notebook, a bind book you have put together with lokta paper, index cards held together with a rubber band. You can use a computer; keep a blog, although that doesn’t work as well for me. But things on the internet are simply not private, password protected or not. To keep a journal on paper, pick a time of day to write. Keep it regularly. It makes it easier. Try that you didn’t stuck to an exercise program because then it can nailed it into schedule at a certain time. Writing works the same way. First thing in the morning, last thing at night, while eating lunch at your desk. Write with a good pen that feels good and whose color you like. In the beginning, you may have to set a time limit. Three minutes is good. Just write whatever comes...

Words: 593 - Pages: 3

Premium Essay

Relationship Essay

...goals.These moments that I share in this special relationship shape me to grow from the past and prepare for the future. Unfettered,capabilities to share my innermost thoughts allowed captivated watch as I begin to blossom into a mature strong being able to stand back and marveled as the story unfolded. A connection so powerful that others long for the ability to have their words caressed and accepted. My aptitude to cope with the grind of life increased as the pages filled up. The ivory shelf above my bed cradled the secret books housing the foundation of character built over the years, strengthen by an everlasting, unbreakable, and irreplaceable bond.Each page of my journal is a connection that provides me with the ability to purge, enhances personal growth, and tells the world my story. Writing in a journal is a means to purge. The remembrance of hastening home knowing that serenity would come once Iexpelled the battle rampant in my head, enabled me to function and endure long hours bursting with the bile of sentiments blaring for discharge.When reflecting...

Words: 957 - Pages: 4

Premium Essay

Autobio

...Since the very beginning of my days as a student, I have despised literature. English has always been a subject that I dreaded. I do not loathe literature solely for my lack of writing skills, because I do enjoy reading, rather it’s the fact that I tend to lose my train of thought while writing an essay or while brainstorming an idea I want to address. I struggle with organizing my thoughts and making concise relationsHIPS[?][CONNECTIONS?] between literary texts and the assignments given by my teachers. The only time I do enjoy writing is when I can write freely in my journal. By writing in a journal, I do not worry about the grammar and organized aspect of writingCOMMA and it clears my mind. I find that I am able to write what I feel [easier ]MORE EASILY in my private journal versus writing an essay that will be graded. [NICE INTRO – CONTRARY TO YOUR TITLE, THIS SEEMS AS THOUGH IT’S GOING TO BE ABOUT YOUR JOURNAL WRITING VS. ACADEMIC WRITING, NOT GRAMMAR? ALSO, YOU NEED TO GET RID OF THIS EXTRA LINE BETWEEN PARAGRAPHS.] During [my ]seventh grade, in middle school, I had a very tough English teacher. Mrs. Carson was a very nice person outside of class[,]SEMI-COLON howeverCOMMA her toughness really displayed itself when she graded papers. Her main focus[ed] was grammar and clean organization rather than THE ideas of the topic. The entire year I struggled with her; each assignment I struggled to form my papers the way she would want and failed to remember the grammar rules and...

Words: 1059 - Pages: 5

Free Essay

Family Dynamics

...The Dynamics of Writing and Family “Most successful men have not achieved their distinction by having some new talent or opportunity presented to them. They have developed the opportunity that was at hand." -- Bruce Fairchild Barton, advertising executive and congressman Uncle would say, "There are certain inalienable truths you must accept." So when I raged about Atheists being disqualified for holding office in Arkansas or how it's illegal to carry a bible in Singapore, he'd say, "Write it down. Let it out." I've filled four journals. On an idle Thursday, when my mind was free of frustration, I read the crumpled, yellowed pages of my black leather journal and saw a natural progression from black to color. I had precisely captured all my experiences, but in an entirely aesthetic way. From then on, every time I opened my journal, I found myself in an unfamiliar environment writing to capture a mood or scene with creative language. It was my therapeutic getaway from reality. In the summertime, every Sunday, after taking a rattling cold bus, two smoky, humid subway trains, and picking up the best peanuts on 114th street, I would arrive at Riverside Park, New York. I wrote at least one piece every time I was there and shared my work with the same old man who perused the New York Times and the same skinny college student who sunbathed in her bikini. After noting any criticism they gave, I would return home, edit and embellish each piece. ...

Words: 1273 - Pages: 6

Premium Essay

Ghost Ghost

...How to Find Specific Journal Articles: (by Mark James) This is a step-by-step guide to finding specific research articles that you have been given the complete reference for (e.g. for seminar readings listed on Moodle, or for articles cited in lectures). To find articles when you do not have a full reference for (e.g. when you’re looking for research on a topic/theory/model generally) please refer to the slide “Finding Sources” of the Study Skills slides provided on Moodle. All seminar readings are available to download via the University of Kent library website using the following steps. Example - how to find: Poropat, A.E. (2009). A meta-analytic analysis of the five-factor model of personality and academic performance. Psychological Bulletin, 135 (2), 322-38. Step 1: Go to http://www.kent.ac.uk/library/ - click ‘Search for journals” and enter title of the journal (e.g. Psychological Bulletin) here: Step 2: Click on one of the search results: Some journals are accessible through multiple providers (e.g. PsycARTICLES, Academic Search Complete, Wiley-Blackwell Full Collection), with varying levels of access. Check the date of the article you’re searching for was published and select a provider which holds the journal published that year. Step 3: A new window will pop up, and you will be asked to input your Kent username and password. Once provided you will be taken to the journal providers website. Select the year the reading was published (e.g. 2009)...

Words: 289 - Pages: 2

Premium Essay

Asian Journal of Research in Marketing a Study of Consumer Behavior with Respect to Various Brands of Shampoo in Nagpur City

...AJRM                                 Volume 1, Issue 3 (June, 2012)         ISSN: 2277‐6621      A Peer Reviewed International Journal of Asian Research Consortium AJRM: ASIAN JOURNAL OF RESEARCH IN MARKETING A STUDY OF CONSUMER BEHAVIOR WITH RESPECT TO VARIOUS BRANDS OF SHAMPOO IN NAGPUR CITY DR.RUHI BAKHARE* Faculty Dr. Ambedkar Institute of Management Studies and Research,Deeksha Bhoomi,Nagpur ABSTRACT ‘Consumer is king’ –the statement carries profound truth in it. Today the success of any firm depends upon the satisfaction of consumers. For satisfying the consumers the firm should know about the behavior of the consumers. In these circumstances understanding consumer is a very difficult task because of the changing technology, innovation, and changes in life style. Researchers conducted many research in this area, and they given only few suggestion, but there is no final conclusion. As per the ideas given by the researchers, there are two factors influencing the consumers such as intrinsic and extrinsic factors. It is difficult to classify consumers by conventional demographic factors and unless their thought process and buying behavior are fully understood, decisions on product designs and packaging, branding and distribution channels are likely to be misplaced. With the inevitability of change intimidating large over the horizon, Indian companies must learn from their western counterparts; not only to identify the sources, timing and direction of the changes likely...

Words: 4028 - Pages: 17

Free Essay

Business Research Methods Res/351

...Medical Journals and the Failure of Trust” the issue of pharmaceutical businesses and their research, and how it is presented is outlined. The article covers one of the largest controversies with business research conducted by GlaxoSmithKline (GSK) and how the results were skewed and then reported by many medical journals as fact, even though the research did not support what was reported. The study was GSK 329, which showed a particular drug to be effective in adolescents with depression, even though the testing showed otherwise. (Jureidini & McHenry, 2011) The biggest unethical behavior that was demonstrated by GSK, and medical journals was the skewing of results of a study. Medical journals published articles that misrepresented the findings of GSK’s research. Internal reports by the company showed that their trials of the drug had failed to benefit adolescents with depression, and positively showed that there were negative results. (Jureidini & McHenry, 2011) The internal report states that “it would be commercially unacceptable to include a statement that efficacy had not been demonstrated, as this would undermine the profile of paroxetine,” and that “to effectively manage the dissemination of data in order to minimize any potential negative commercial impact.” (Jureidini &McHenry, 2011) This indicates that they skewed the results of the study so that only selective data would be published. The authors also place blame on the medical journals that published...

Words: 854 - Pages: 4

Free Essay

Psychology

...Write that Journal Article (in seven days) Dr Inger Mewburn Director of Research Training Australian National University This slide deck is released under a Creative Commons license Are you a plotter or a pantser? Plotters like to plan and then write line by perfect line. They take a long time, but don’t have to do a lot of editing later. Pantsers like to just jump in, with minimal preparation. They make a mess and then clean it up. The cleaning up can take a long time. Ideally you can switch between these modes. ‘... file cabinets are full of unborn articles. I know many researchers who have a shameful backlog of data; some have unpublished data from the 80’ s that they “hope to publish someday”. Sure they will.’ Silva, 2007. Warning: Don’t try to do this from a cold start... Writing a journal article is like cooking a stew. You need ingredients. If you have to keep running to the shops it will slow you down. Check whether you have some or all of the following in your academic cupboard: ● ● ● Data, Ideas and/or artifacts Knowledge of at least some relevant literature Results, analyses and or theorisations Day Zero: What sort of academic are you? Academia used to be mostly about speaking Image of an early viva from William Clark “academic charisma and the origins of the research university. Now we know each other through writing “Text work is identity work” Kamler and Thomson, 2006 Where you publish matters because...

Words: 2719 - Pages: 11

Free Essay

Search for Sources

... Introduction The chart below was utilized for the initial library search for resources in professional or research interest. There are; three peer-reviewed journal articles, one book chapter, and one scholarly source. Following the chart, is a brief reflection paper on the library search and results. |Article Citation |Database |Keywords |Search Limiters |Skills in this Database | |Peer reviewed article: |Roadrunner Search, Discovery |Cultural Change, |Military Logistics, |Fair: Easy to use, need to | | |Database |Data Rights, |Defense Logistics Agency, |practice on my search limiters. | |Better Buying Power or Better Off Not: | |Heuristic Economic Model, |Quality Assurance | | |By: Hasik, James. Defense Acquisition Research | |Technical Data | | | |Journal: A Publication of the Defense Acquisition | | | | ...

Words: 985 - Pages: 4

Premium Essay

Doctor

...function. The accountant is view as a dull person that is chain to his desk all day. A survey of college students proved that accounting is treated with mental formulaic concepts, thus it has a stereotype image. Accounting may be defined as an information system that provides reports to various individuals or groups about economic activities of an organization or other entity. Accounting is information sciences used to collect, classify, and manipulate financial data for organizations and individuals Accounting is instrumental within organizations as a means of determining financial stability. Without accounting, organizations would have no basis or foundation upon which daily and long-term decisions could be made. The budgets for marketing activities, profit reinvestment, research and development, and company growth all stem from the work of accountants...

Words: 477 - Pages: 2

Premium Essay

Corporate Reputation

...Corporate Reputation Review Volume 12 Number 4 A Systematic Review of the Corporate Reputation Literature: Definition, Measurement, and Theory Kent Walker Asper School of Business, University of Manitoba, Winnipeg, Manitoba, Canada ABSTRACT A systematic review of the corporate reputation literature is conducted. The final sample of 54 articles (and one book) consists of well-cited papers, and papers in journals that have published high quality work in corporate reputation. The sample is then analyzed and the three fundamental problems in the reputation literature are addressed – the need for a comprehensive and well-accepted definition, the difficulty in operationalizing corporate reputation, and the ongoing need for more developed theory. Two main findings evolve from this analysis: (1) reputation may have different dimensions and is issue specific, and (2) different stakeholder groups may have different perceptions of corporate reputations. The implications for future research are discussed. Corporate Reputation Review (2010) 12, 357–387. doi:10.1057/crr.2009.26 KEYWORDS: corporate reputation; definition; operationalization; organizational identity; organizational image; systematic review INTRODUCTION There are many reasons why organizations and researchers should care about corporate reputation. The relationship between reputation and a sustained competitive advantage is widely acknowledged in the literature (eg, Fombrun and Shanley, 1990; Fombrun, 1996;...

Words: 16404 - Pages: 66

Free Essay

Speech

...Appraising Internationality in Spanish Communication Journals ´ david ferna ndez-quijada This article explores how journals published in a language other than English achieve a degree of internationality and can increase our knowledge of scientific publication patterns. This author offers a case study focused on Spanish communication journals from a sample of 1182 articles published from 2007 to 2009. The article examines three variables in this sample: the number of non-Spanish scholars, the use of languages other than Spanish, and how often non-Spanish journals are referred to. The results show that (a) these journals find it difficult to attract foreign scholars, (b) open-language policies have had a limited effect, and (c) internationality is constrained to the Spanish geolinguistic region. Keywords: internationality, local journals, Spanish scholarly journals, journal evaluation, communication sciences introduction The impact of published research is measured by means of well-established tools; one such tool is the Social Sciences Citation Index (SSCI). However, in the field of communication studies — and in fact in all the social sciences — the SSCI does not take account of all the research published; this is particularly visible in cases where research is published in languages other than English. For example, in 2009, only 81 out of the 1585 articles (5.11 per cent) indexed in the Communication category of the SSCI were not written in English. In the context of these...

Words: 5814 - Pages: 24