Free Essay

Sex in Th Middle Ages

In:

Submitted By hahicks
Words 6782
Pages 28
Hodges 1 [Amber Hodges graduated from Kalamazoo College in June 2003. She was a senior Sociology/Anthropology major and Psychology minor when she wrote this Observation Report in Spring of 2003].

Observation Report Amber Hodges
Introduction Siefert et. al describe children’s play as intrinsically motivated, process oriented, creative and nonliteral, governed by implicit rules, spontaneous and self-initiated, and free from major emotional distress(1997 p. 221-222). For example, the rules of a made-up game that children play cannot be discerned from a rulebook that stands independently of the activity; rather, one can discern rules by either observing or being actively involved in the play process. For children, play is the arena where they ultimately control what is going on, through communication and negotiation with each other, and as such, it is an arena where they are able to gain mastery over problems in their life, learn adult skills and roles, and further develop cognitive abilities (Siefert 1997). But, often times, children do not have complete hold over their environments. They can’t shape what and how they play through their own design. Wardle introduces the concept of an environmental press, “the forces at work in a setting which shape the behavior of people in that setting” (1999 p. 245). One principle of the environmental press is that of progressive conformity, where people’s behavior tends to become congruent with the press of the environment. Accordingly, the environment that a child plays in shapes the way that the child plays. Wide spaces encourage the use of gross-motor skills, whereas high spatial densities can contribute to aggression. The environmental press is not a force that influences children to do exactly the same thing in a certain situation; it is simply the environment’s contribution to the transactions. Additionally, the “individual brings to their situation a unique arrangement of personal resources, a particular level of development, and other attributes” (Garbarino, ibid Wardle 1999 p. 245). One final concept that is important to the shape of children’s play is that of reciprocal determinism, a concept proposed originally by Bandura to describe the way in which development is an interaction between a person, the environment and their behavior. For

Hodges 2 example, when a child first enters school, that environment is infinitely open to them. However, the child will behave in a certain way that affects the environment, perhaps by contributing to more school bullying. Over time, the environment becomes increasingly closed off to that child, and this affects his future development, as his aggressive behavior displeases prosocial children who might be models for empathy and conflict resolution. For Bandura, development is a reciprocal interaction between children and their environments (Shaffer 2000 p. 49). The definitions of play and the concept of the environmental press and reciprocal determinism proved salient during my playground observations, and I take time to explain them now both by way of introduction and also because they will be important to the observations that are to come. Throughout my experience at Woodward, I marveled both at children’s ability to make the most creative games out of the materials in front of them, materials which they had seen and played with throughout the year, but also at children’s ability to create an environment of fun or of aggression and hostility. I saw reciprocal determinism shaping the relationship that I developed with one girl, Emm (not her real name), and I saw the environmental press shaping the nature of children’s indoor play, bringing out aggression and more creative play. The playground truly is a classroom without walls for both students and adults.

* All names and initials of children have been changed to protect their identities.

I. Emm’s Interactions Date and Time: [the time for all these interactions is from 12:00-12:30] April 24, 2003, May 1, 2003, May 8, 2003, May 22 2003, Setting: Woodward Playground during 3rd Grade recess Activity Observed: Playing the chase game, walking to the drinking fountain, walking to the classroom My interaction with the children: Emm and I developed a relationship from the second week of my observations. She would be the catalyst for a game wherein she took my keys and I ran around and chased her. However, I was also in the role of comforter, for many times I soothed her when she was upset. When I first met Emm, I thought that she was the most fragile child on the playground. She was tall and extremely thin, and when I first observed her, I noticed that she moved awkwardly, that she constantly looked to me or other playground helpers for approval or reprimand. However, through the blooming of our relationship, I came to understand that she is

Hodges 3 socially competent, that she interacts well with peers, and is highly regarded by them and yet with me, or in other vertical relationships, Emm seems to be inflexible and unable to control her emotions. These issues came to the core of my struggle for evaluating the competence of Emm, and they highlight the importance of context in the expression of social competence. The evaluation of social competence had not become a topic of interest until the 1970s, when social behavior was studied along side cognitive development in settings like the school (Tan personal communication). However, the issue of what exactly constitutes social skills was not yet definitively labeled and classified. One of the complexities of gauging one’s social competence is determining who rates the competency, as often for children, peers and teachers rate social competence differently. Another complexity is whether social skills can be divided into different subsets or whether competence is a unitary whole (Tan personal communication). Researchers now believe that social competence can be broken down into different areas (Tan personal communication). For example, according to a study by Caldarella and Merrell (1997), there are five common social skills dimensions for children and adolescents: peer relationship skills, selfmanagement skills, academic skills, compliance skills, and assertion skills. Under the “relationship skills” are items such as applauds peers, invites peers to play, is sought out by peers to join activities, is sensitive to feelings of peers, makes friends easily, and shares laughter with peers. I saw evidence of all these behaviors with Emm and her friends. She is well integrated into a group of about five peers, and every day I observed her laughing, smiling, and in general playing with these peers. For example, On April 24, when I first encountered her, she was running around without a shoe! I then noticed that she was deep in the middle of a game where one of her friends takes her shoe and runs with it, or she takes one of their shoes. When I became a player in her group, I saw Emm drawing new people into the group, advising them on who was it in the chase game we played. Under the category “self-management skills,” Caldarella and Merrell (1997) placed skills such as remains calm when problems arise, accepts imposed limits, receives criticism well and responds to teasing by ignoring peers. This is where Emm has some limitations. For example, she frequently cries. Our first interaction occurred on April 24, when I tried to give her a time out, and she responded by crying. As I was trying to soothe her and fix part of her shoe, she began to cry harder, saying, “its all my fault!” We proceeded to go into the classroom and I

Hodges 4 handed her over to her teacher, where she continued to cry. The second time that I saw her, she was crying after someone had pushed her down the slide; she was not physically hurt and stopped crying after I spoke with her. Emm does not have the ability to regulate negative emotions. On my last day of playground observation (June 4) J, one of Emm’s playmates, came to me with a stricken face at the end of the recess time and said, “It looks like Emm is going to cry.” I looked over to her, and I didn’t see any overt signs that she was going to cry, but she in the end, she did cry. So, it appears that her playmates are tuned into some of her common warning signals and that for them, Emm’s crying is not out of the ordinary. Another element of self-management skills, accepts imposed limits, Emm also fails to perform in my presence. One of the rules of our chase game is that the children cannot reach into my pockets. This is something that Emm has great difficulty refraining from, despite multiple warnings and threats of a time-out. Social skills are not linear elements of a person’s personality that can be easily tested. Researchers must be able to account for the fact that social skills involve both intrapersonal and interpersonal processes in a dynamic interaction with both context and time (Tan, personal communication 4/4/03). Consequently, although I showed some examples of trying to test Emm’s social skills, I am only making generalizations, which I think differ in different contexts. For example, it seems as though Emm does fairly well with interpersonal processes, like entry behavior and perhaps the interpretation of cues. However, intrapersonal processes, like selfawareness and emotional self-regulation, Emm has difficulty with, and this gets expressed in her self-management skill set. Perhaps one can look at the differences in types of relationships that exist to describe the different types of behavior that get expressed. Vertical relationships are relationships between two or more people of different ages (Tan personal communication 4/30/03), while horizontal relationship are relationships between two or more people of the same or similar ages. Children learn different things from both relationships. For example, from horizontal relationships, children learn conflict resolution, negotiation, self-management, cooperation and sharing, and how to deal with rejection and teasing. Horizontal relationships are based on the idea of equity and interchangeable roles, that the partners in the relationship are equal and that they demand the same things from each other. In vertical relationships, a child of Emm’s age is typically at the bottom, while an adult is at the top. In this situation, children learn things such as how to listen to

Hodges 5 authority, how to follow rules, how to imitate a model and how to accept help and trust others. When a child is at the bottom of a vertical relationship, she can expect that the person on top will be sacrificing, will provide and protect, and will accept them (and their selfish behaviors). For Emm, I think that she struggles in vertical relationships, while she excels in horizontal relationships. Her acceptance from peers and her interactions with them indicate that she sees them as equals and that she works on cooperation and sharing with them (for example, she often works with two other girls as a team pitted against me to take my keys). It is in my vertical relationship with her where I see her engage in quite behaviors not normal of her age group. Besides the high frequency of crying, she also seems intent on getting me to carry her places. The first time that she did this was May 8, when I picked her up and began rocking her, saying, “Emm’s my baby”. She seemed to take this and would not let go, and continually urged me to pick her up again. As it was time to line up, she begged me to carry her to the door, or just to the end of the sand lot. A second time, on May 22, we were in the school, and she asked me to carry her past the office door, so that she could see her friend. After we talked by the office, an older woman came out, and Emm responded by throwing herself at me, trying to get me to pick her up. She said to the woman, “look, this is the girl who we take her keys from.” Interestingly, the woman said to Emm, “Remember Emm that you are a big girl.” She seemed to ignore this and just smile at me. Later, as she was getting ready to line up, she asked me to carry her again. Additionally, Emm sees this great need to conspire with me. When we are playing our game, she continually tries to get me to come with her, to talk to her because she knows the location of the keys and she will tell me if I just talk to her. On May 8, I said to her that I can see another student with my keys, waving them around. But, she did not even look over to the other student, but continually insists that she has the keys, finally ending up yelling, “Fine! Don’t believe me!” It is difficult for me to understand why this discrepancy would exist, as all I can use is her immediate behavior. However, I think that this discrepancy needs to be examined further. It is not as though Emm used me in attempts to solve her disputes with other children. For example, once she had been pushed off of the slide and was crying, and while I was talking to her the person that had pushed her came over to her and apologized before I even requested it. She doesn’t have difficult with entry behavior, and thus doesn’t look to me to help her enter into a social group. Context seems to play an important role in the expression of Emm’s social

Hodges 6 competency, but additionally factors not available to me during the observation (how she interacts with other adults, how she acts at home) is necessary before anything more definitive can be said. II. The Chase Game 1. Date and Time: [the time for all these interactions is from 12:00-12:30] April 24, 2003, May 1, 2003, May 8, 2003, May 22 2003, May 28 2. Setting: Woodward Playground during 3rd Grade recess 3. Activity Observed: playing “keep away”, where children take an object and pass it to each other in an attempt to allude the object’s owner. 4. My interaction with the children: I was the chaser. The children would take my keys or papers or pens and run around while I would chase them. If I happened to retrieve my objects, the children would run around and chase me in an attempt to get them. According to Siefert, “play tends to be governed by implicit rules...rules that can be discerned by observing the activity” (1999 p. 222). This was certainly the case for the chase game that took place during my weeks of observation. The game at first seemed chaotic and unorganized, but there were definite rules, definite teammates, and my understanding of this blossomed throughout the course of the game. This points to the fact that games, which may at first seem unorganized or unruly, actually are established and ordered. This concept can best be understood by looking at the chase game through the lens of the dynamic systems perspective, as it allows me a way to demonstrate the mechanism in which the game, although it consisted of the same actions, was constantly changing and getting more complex. One of the key concepts of a dynamic systems perspective is the emergent and dynamic frame, the patterns of interactions that become established through time (Pepler et. al p. 441). Frames can be thought of as roles that correspond to patterns of behavior, and frames serve to provide predictability in their interactions. During the chase game, I found that everyone had distinctive roles. I was always on my own team, and the children always formed a team. Throughout the weeks that we played the game, I tried to get children to be on my team and in the first two weeks, I had little success in getting this accomplished. On May 1, the day the game started, Emm would switch back and forth from being on my team--I think mostly a deceptive means to get the keys and then have me chase her. But, none of the other children would join my team, even after I continually asked them. On May 8, I targeted the girls to be on my team, but they wouldn’t budge. Finally, on May 22, I managed to convince J to be on my team, and he did

Hodges 7 so honestly. However, this was met with some resistance from the other members. I pulled J aside and asked him if he wanted to be on my team and he agreed. However, D overheard and yelled to everyone that J was on my team. J did remain on my team for the rest of the afternoon; however, I had little success recruiting him in the following weeks. Additionally, as the name indicates, frames are that set of behaviors which encase or surround the interaction, making it both unique and predictable. A distinct beginning and ending framed the chase game. After I first met Emm, I ended up walking her to class at the end of recess and giving her a sticker. The next time that I interacted with Emm she took my keys and ran off, her friends joining in on the fun. These two behaviors came to frame our interaction, such that the game would begin when the whole group of students would come up to me at the beginning of recess and take my keys and run away, and the game would end when I would walk them to class and give Emm a sticker. The children were aware of this frame as well. On May 22, Emm and the other children encircled me after they came out on the playground, saying the following: “Come on. Remember what you said.” J said. “You said that whenever we were together, we could play this game where we take your keys.” Emm said. Another element to dynamic systems perspective is positive feedback, which serves to promote the development of a frame (Pepler et. al p. 1999 441). Positive feedback occurs in loops where one person in the frame performs one behavior, and the other person responds to that behavior in a way that encourages more behavior from the first person. Because of positive feedback, established frames are sensitive to small differences. Positive feedback, on my part and on the part of the students, contributed to the increasingly aggressive and complicated nature of the game. For example, when the game began, we were just throwing the keys around. However, through the course of the weeks, I could tell that children were more and more testing the waters in what sort of behavior I would allow. On the fifth of May, children incorporated running into the field adjacent to the playground as part of the game as well as tossing the keys. I provided positive feedback in that I allowed it without explicit instruction not to, and I engaged in the same kind of behavior. On May 22, the children were more physically aggressive. When I had the keys, the children would pry open my hands or hang on me. Also, D and Emm physically held me back my holding onto my arms, wrapping

Hodges 8 their arms around my shoulders and pulling back, and even hanging on me. I encouraged this by allowing them to do it and prying open hands myself. Finally, on May 28, two children would latch onto my legs in efforts to restrain me. I tried to walk with them on my legs and eventually did tell them to remove themselves from me. Overall, my attention to these activities served as positive feedback to encourage more and more adventurous behavior. Another element of the dynamic systems perspective is coupling, “the coordination of particular behaviors or elements in the reciprocal interaction processes” (Pepler et. al 1999 p. 442). Coupling consists of the behaviors that make a particular interaction frame unique from others, and they are what make the interaction coherent. Both coupling and positive feedback are the self-organizing mechanisms in the dynamic systems perspective. In the case of my game, the main coupling that occurred was that children would take something of mine and I would chase them. In some ways this is complicated because earlier I have said that my chasing them was a source of positive feedback, but I am comfortable with this for a number of reasons. I believe that the positive feedback occurred because of my attention to them, which largely took the form of chasing, though could have been a number of other things. However, the physical act of chasing, the back and forth of the children taking something and my response of running at them as fast as I could (or alternatively, their running at me when I retained something) was the coupling. One of the final elements of the dynamic system is its stability. As Pepler says, through repeated interactions, roles and behavior patterns become established (1990 p. 442). Once a system has self-organized, it is very resistant to change, partly because cognitive and emotional factors also continue to comprise the emergent frames (Pepler 1990 p. 442). The particular emotions and thoughts that children develop as a result of the interactions serve to stabilize the interaction. For example, one the game was established, one of the children, PI, tried to shift the focus of the game by continually saying to the group, “We have to formulate a plan.” Yet, I was the only one that would listen to her ‘plan’, everyone else continued to take the keys from whoever had had them and had them off. One limitation of my observations is that they occurred over such a short period of time, where true dynamic systems become stable over a much longer period of time. Nevertheless, I think that the concepts of coupling, positive feedback and frames illustrate how our game was organized and resistant to change. I imagine that, were we to continue with our game, it would be

Hodges 9 framed by the same beginning and ending and be comprised of the same roles. Ultimately, dynamic systems perspective is useful in describing play because play is so seemingly unstructured and intrinsic. In the chase game, no one set out to say, “ok, you will always be it, and these are the players, and this is the way that the game will go.” It was just a natural progression of satisfying interactions that became organized without our explicit consciousness.

III: ToM the Context of Play Date and Time: [the time for all these interactions is from 12:00-12:30] May 1, 2003, May 8, 2003, May 22 2003, 2. Setting: Woodward Playground during 3rd Grade recess 3. Activity Observed: During a game of keep-away, I noticed the children trying to instill a false belief about the exact location of the keys. 4. My interaction with the children: I was the chaser. The children would take my keys or papers or pens and run around while I would chase them. If I happened to retrieve my objects, the children would run around and chase me in an attempt to get them. Throughout the course of the chase game, I noticed an interesting behavior from two different people, YZ (not his real initials) and Emm, who both tried the same thing but had different results. YZ tried to make me believe that he did not have my keys, while Emm tried to make me believe that she either had the keys or knew where they were located. The behavior of both children is evidence of a well-established Theory of Mind; in particular, they are demonstrating that they understand that false beliefs can be implanted in other people’s minds. Researchers studying the theory of the mind commonly test three and four year olds, as they believe that at that age is one of the critical points wherein four year olds have developed a theory of mind and three year olds haven’t. One interesting study by Chandler, Fritz and Hala (ibid Lee and Homer 1999 p. 235) tested whether deceptive acts were learned behaviors or a result of a child’s deliberate attempt to instill a false belief into another’s mind. In the study, Chandler et. al instructed children to cover the tracks and conceal the location of a doll, so that an adult would not know its location. The children employed various methods, including withholding evidence, destroying evidence, and producing false information and/or destroying evidence. Chandler found that children as young as two engaged in deception with the intent to create false beliefs in others minds, indicating that deception is not learned behavior (Homer and Lee 1999 p. 235). But, what does Theory of Mind look like for a nine year old? On May 8, Emm attempted to instill the false belief in me that she knew the location of

Hodges 10 my keys. Her behavior was quite interesting in that it was so unconvincing. First, she wanted me to go over to a corner and talk with her, because if I talked with her she would reveal where my keys were, and in another instance she said that she had the keys and would give them to me. I pointed out to her on both occasions that someone else had the keys. In one instance, I saw that J had my keys, and I told her I wouldn’t go with her because I knew that she didn’t have the keys, but, she didn’t back down. She looked me right in the face and said that she had the keys (Incidentally, she wouldn’t look where I was pointing, to J who was walking around swinging the keys). Finally, after we had been going back and forth for a few minutes this way, she says, “Fine! Don’t believe me!” On May 28, I was very cleverly fooled by YZ. At one point, I had left the group to talk to a student, and when I returned no one was clear where the keys were or who had them last, but they thought that it was YZ. I went over to YZ and asked him if he had the keys, to which he said no. He even let me check his pockets for the keys, and I found that he didn’t have them. Throughout this he was calm, not giggling or giving himself away. I was confident that he didn’t have the keys and went hot on another trail. At the very end of recess, YZ came up to me, smiling hugely, and presented me with the keys. He had, in fact, had them the whole time. I asked him how he managed this, and he said that he had hid them in this groove in his pocket. Thus, YZ was able to instill a false belief in me through quietness and a game face, unlike Emm who was unconvincing in her assuredness. The above example brings up perhaps an additional developmental level to Theory of Mind. Whereas Emm tried to get me to believe that she had the keys when she didn’t and whereas YZ tried to get me to believe that he did not have the keys when he did, both children are demonstrating their knowledge that I have a mind that can believe things that are different from reality. Yet to convincingly instill a false belief involves the ability to lie well, to read cues and remain calm under pressure--this is to say to know what to do that would convince someone of something false. Product is one level, but process is entirely another. The above example of YZ and Emm indicates that YZ has reached this different level while Emm has not.

III. Entry Behavior Date and Time: May 1, 2003; 11:30-12:00 Setting: area of sand between the equipment and tire swing. Activities observed: 3 different strategies of entry behavior

Hodges 11 My interaction with the children: observed entry behavior According to Puttallaz and Wasserman (1990), successful entry behavior is something that is rare. Many different types of entry strategies exist, for example, passive, self-centered or competent (Tan, personal communication 4/11/03). During this observation, I saw many of these different types being used; in line with Wasserman’s research, the most successful strategies seen here involved a sequencing of behaviors. One additional interesting component of this observation is the different strategies used by people of different genders. When the interaction began, two girls were building different structures in the sand. They sat facing each other, building separate structures but engaging in ongoing conversation. L’s entry behavior consisted of watching the girls play in the sand for a few minutes, making sand castles alongside them, and finally saying something to one of the girls, who responded. According to a study done by Mallay (ibid Puttallaz and Wasserman 1990 p. 69) L used a threestep sequence that involved regard, regard and parallel activity and regard and vocalization. L’s entry behavior was successful largely because, as was the case in a different study by Corsaro, the sequencing was more critical than the initial behavior, often because children first use less direct and less successful measures in an attempt to save face (Putallaz and Wasserman 1990 p. 71). During this interaction, I saw another entry behavior strategy that was also indirect. In this interaction, a girl, C, came up to the trio, watched for a few minutes and the asked, “What are ya’ll doing?” One of the two original group members said, “Building sand castles.” C then began to build sand castles with the group, and finally interacted with them. Her entry behavior was also indirect because she asks what they are doing, rather than asking if she can play. Both girls used a sequencing of entry behaviors, after their first strategy, regard, did not result in entry into the group. The final entry behavior that I saw involved an entry strategy known as aggressive, where a child uses physical force to enter into a group (Tan, personal communication 4/11/03). A boy came and sat in the circle of girls and began to physically manipulate the sand castles the girls were building. Additionally, he reached in and picked up a small toy that the girls had put atop one of their sand structures, and started to bobble it in his hands. His entry behavior was unsuccessful, as one of the girls screamed at him, ‘get away!’; he responded by running away quickly. The gendered differences in entry behavior seem to be in agreement with the study by

Hodges 12 Forbes (cited in Puttallaz and Wasserman 1990 p. 72-74). The girls in Forbes study employed more neutral entry strategies following negative group feedback in efforts to align themselves more with the group. The boys were more likely use forceful relational entry strategies (i.e. being assertive about their relationship to the group as opposed to the activity) and were more likely to engage in face-saving behaviors and assert their positive worth after a rejection. Tentatively, I think that that the gender difference I saw--that females used more indirect entry behaviors like regard and vocalization, and that the boy used more physically aggressive behavior supports Forbes finding. The girls changed entry behavior strategies after their first or even second strategy was unsuccessful, while the boy, after negative feedback, opted not to change strategies to fit in with the group at all. He chose to get up and run away from the group entirely. Entry behavior is an important skill to master, especially earlier in life when nonverbal cues are more explicit, and the use of good entry behavior strategies is often a good predictor for social competence (Tan personal communication). Once inside the group, children have valuable opportunities to learn about equity, conflict resolution, and emotion management, to name a few. Knowing this, we can understand the second step in reciprocal determinism a little better. Our boy, through aggressive entry behavior strategies, has closed off his opportunity to engage in play with the girls, and therefore he has lost the opportunity to practice the skills listed above. In view of that, entry behavior is a valuable portal into the world of social interaction. IV. Indoor recess Date and Time: 5/18/03; 11:30-12:30 Setting: Woodward gym during indoor recess Activities Observed: creative use of hula-hoops and boys’ rough and tumble play. My role in the interaction: I mainly observed behavior. When I heard that today would be indoor recess, half of me was excited and half of me was quite apprehensive. I was curious to see the differences in play from an outdoor environment to an indoor environment, but something told me that the small, enclosed space could breed aggression. Both of my feelings were confirmed: I saw both aggressive acts and more creative forms of play, children being creative with a hula hoop, and boys playing roughly against a “zombie.” In “Play Environment”, Francis Wardle (1999) examines several aspects of the indoor environment, like spatial density and the arrangement of space that affect play; additionally, she looks at “loose parts” as components of outdoor playgrounds. As she would hypothesize, the

Hodges 13 play environment structured the type of play that these children engaged in during their indoor recess. According to Wardle (1990), an upper limit of spatial density exists. Under the limit, children close together children encourage social play and interaction, however, after the limit is reached, there is an increase in aggression and a decrease gross-motor play. A study by Smith and Connelly defined this upper limit as 25 square feet per child, whereby anything denser resulted in a significant reduction in group play (Wardle 1999 p. 247). Because spatial density was below the upper limit, it seemed to encourage play in the gym at Woodward because children were enclosed in a small space and forced to interact with each other, in sociodramatic play (getting the “zombie”), in gross motor-play, (running around with hula hoops or playing basketball) and talking in small circles. Another aspect that Wardle (1990) discusses is the arrangement of space. In general, a well-defined or partitioned space results in increases in verbal interactions, cooperation, pretend play, and assists in encouraging more adult participation. However, open spaces have been associated with increased amounts of rowdy, withdrawn and random behavior (Wardle 1990 p. 254). The setup of the gym is extremely open, with no physical structures or barriers marking off space. Children were free to use materials, such as hula-hoops, jump ropes, and basketballs throughout the space. The only fixed structures were a pile of mats that were in the corner of the room. Though the gym was not so spatially dense that it would serve to increase aggression, I believe the lack of structure in the gym did increase the aggression. Specifically, I saw one group of boys engaged in rough and tumble play who normally do not engage in this behavior (based on previous observations). About four boys engaged in the “zombie” game, whereby they would physically attack, through hanging on, punching, or pulling down to the ground, a larger child who was the zombie. In a derivation of this game, the three or four boys would sit or lay on the mat and the zombie would run and jump onto the boys. In one instance, the rough and tumble play resulted in an intervention by one of the activity helpers when the boys began using the hula-hoops as weapons to attack the zombie. At times, the game would be extended outside the small corner of the gym, when the boys would use the heating vents to hide from the zombie and would additionally chase the zombie around, like they were playing tag. I believe the lack of structure of the gym, the complete openness, contributed to the rough and tumble play of the

Hodges 14 boys. In addition to the rough and tumble play of the boys, I noticed very inventive ways of using a hula hoop, in line with Wardle’s discussion on flexible materials that when more flexible open more options to the child (1990 p. 270). Flexible materials include water and sand, which can be manipulated in an infinite amount of ways that breed creativity and ingenuity. Loose parts, like blocks of wood or ropes, are a subset of flexible materials in that they are objects to which children can ascribe their own meaning and structure on the environment, thus making the environment more responsive to children’s needs (Wardle 1990 p. 271). From my observation, I saw that children used hula-hoops in a number of different ways. A hula-hoop might seem more like an inflexible part because it cannot be disassembled; however, because children played with it in a variety of ways (many of which I would have never thought!), I consider it to be a loose part. For example, a group of girls was playing tag, when one of them decided to use the hula-hoop to group herself with another girl. Thus, the person that was “it” was chasing a hula-hoop that enclosed two girls. A few minutes later, one of the girls that was “it” used the hula hoop to physically restrain people (making them it.) The hula-hoop was also used for more physical activities, such as hula hooping and using the hulahoop as a jump rope or waving it around like a flag. The hula-hoop was also a tool for social interaction when it was used to beat the zombie in the boy’s game, to point someone out in another interaction, and to gain the attention of the activity helpers. One child insisted that all three activity helpers watch him as he hula hoop-ed. For indoor play, it appears that the concept of the progressive conformity of the environmental press is important, as it contributes to differences in play, and I think that this is important to consider when adults try to structure the indoor play environment. While including loose parts is helpful in stimulating the creative capacities for a number of children, the open structure contributes to the aggressive tendencies of another group of children. This is not surprising according to the principles of the environmental press, where the environment and the individual each bring something to the transaction. Loose parts are one essential component to both an indoor and an outdoor environment, but play environments must also have structures. It is important to provide a multitude of options that engage children in varied types of play.

Conclusion

Hodges 15 Through various observations of play, we can see the ways in which children and the environment each bring something unique to the interaction. The environment, through containing loose parts or providing fixed structures, can encourage children to engage in play that is both more social and creative, as in the case of the hula hoops, or more aggressive. The open space of the outdoor playground provided ample room for the full expression of the chase game, involving running and physical aggression. The child brings a unique composition of genetics, previous experiences, and knowledge of the social world, which he or she uses to interact with other children, through various entry behavior strategies, deceptive techniques, or through their ability to behave in different contexts. Through interacting with the playground and with other children, children can learn much about the material world, about empathy, perspective taking, and bullying. Even adults can learn so much about children and themselves through observing this classroom without walls.

Hodges 16 References

Caldarella, P. &Merrell, K.W. (1997). Common dimensions of social skills of children and adolescents: A taxonomy of positive behaviors. School Psychology Review, 26, 265-279. Homer, B. &Lee, K. (1999). Children as folk psychologists: The developing understanding of the mind. The Blackwell Reader in Developmental Psychology. Slater and Muir, ed. Oxford, England: Blackwell Publishers Pepler, D., Craig, W., &O’Connell, P. (1999). Understanding Bullying from a dynamic systems perspective. The Blackwell Reader in Developmental Psychology. Ed. Muir. Oxford, England: Blackwell Publishers. Putallaz, M. &Wasserman, A. (1990). Children’s Entry Behavior. Peer rejection in childhood. Asher and Coie ed. Slater. New York: Cambridge University Press. Siefert, K, Hoffnung, R, & Hoffnung, M (1997). Play. Lifespan Development. Boston: Houghton Mifflin Company. Tan, personal communication, March 30, 2003. Tan, personal communication, April 4, 2003. Tan, personal communication, April 11, 2003. Tan, personal communication, April 14, 2003. Tan, personal communication, April 30, 2003. Wardle, F. (1999). Play Environments. From Play and Early Childhood Development. NY: Addison-Wesley

Similar Documents

Free Essay

The Plough and the Stars - Liberation

...in 1926, because of it’s “representation of the 1916 rebels as cowards principally motivated by vanity and self-love” (Pilkington 2001 P101). Within this play, the final in a trilogy of plays, O’Casey explores many areas of interest to the people of Dublin at the time of its setting in around the Easter 1916 rising. Within the play certain themes are explored: poverty, religion, class, sex, morality, as well as the themes of nationhood and social identity. There is a recurring topic of the struggle for liberation, portrayed through the nationalistic Jack and of the labour struggle through the ‘learned’ Covey. This essay hopes to further explore the idea of liberation found within the play with emphasis on the nationalism and socialism themes. O’Casey began life in a Northside Dublin tenement, a last son of a large Protestant lower middle class Unionist family. O’Casey suffered much during his childhood years, particularly with his eyes, which stalled his learning process as he had to spend much time away from school. However he had a thirst for knowledge and taught himself to read by the age of thirteen. Like all the poor in Dublin at that time he had to leave school and find work at fourteen. He had a few unfortunate incidences in his working life as he tells us in his autobiography which appears to have influenced his later thoughts on the working man. He was fined two shillings by his employer after an incident at work, and refused to accept this unjust censure “- I have...

Words: 1374 - Pages: 6

Free Essay

Information

...Queensland, Brisbane, QLD 4075, Australia c University of Nice-Sophia Antipolis, UFR Sciences, Nice, France d INSERM U1091, CNRS UMR7277, IBV, Nice, France e MIMR-PHI Institute of Medical Research, Clayton, VIC 3800, Australia f Sir Peter MacCallum, Department of Oncology and the Pathology Department, The University of Melbourne, Parkville, VIC, Australia g Department of Pathology, The University of Melbourne, Parkville, VIC, Australia b ar t ic l e i nf o a b s t r a c t Article history: Received 27 June 2014 Received in revised form 12 August 2014 Accepted 15 August 2014 Available online 23 August 2014 The two main functions of the ovary are the production of oocytes, which allows the continuation of the species, and secretion of female sex hormones, which control many aspects of female development and physiology. Normal development of the ovaries during embryogenesis is critical for their function and the health of the individual in later life. Although the adult ovary has been investigated in great detail, we are only starting to understand the cellular and molecular biology of early ovarian development. Here we show that the adult stem cell marker Lgr5 is expressed in the cortical region of the fetal ovary and this...

Words: 10419 - Pages: 42

Premium Essay

The Adversity Quotient and Academic Performance Among College Students at St. Joseph’scollege, Quezon City

...THE ADVERSITY QUOTIENT AND ACADEMIC PERFORMANCE AMONG COLLEGE STUDENTS AT ST. JOSEPH’SCOLLEGE, QUEZON CITY An undergraduate thesis Presented to the Faculty of The Departments of Arts and Sciences St. Joseph’s College Quezon City In Partial Fulfillment of the Requirement for the Degree of Bachelor of Science in Psychology By: ZHOU HUIJUAN March, 2009 RECOMMENDATION This Thesis entitled The Adversity Quotient and Academic Performance among College Students at St. Joseph’s College, Quezon City. Submitted by Zhou, Huijuan has been examined and found satisfactory and is hereby recommended for ORAL DEFENSE. Ms. Mildred L. Lazo Thesis Adviser APPROVAL SHEET In Partial fulfillment of the requirements fro the degree of Bachelor of Science in Psychology, this thesis entitled “The Adversity Quotient and Academic Performance among College Students at St. Joseph’s College, Quezon City” was prepared and submitted to the College of Arts and Science by Zhou, Huijuan. Approved by the committee on Oral Defense on March 8, 2009 with a grade of passed. Mrs. Nelia G. Prieto Chair, Liberal Arts Ms. Mildred Lazo Panel Member Mr. Francisco Lambojon Panel Member Accepted in partial fulfillment of the degree of Bachelor of Sciences Major in Psychology. Sr. Josephini P. Ambatali, SFIC Dean Acknowledgement This work would not have been possible without the presence and contribution of many valued individuals. Through this limited paper, I wish to express my...

Words: 20407 - Pages: 82

Premium Essay

Study Habits

...THE ADVERSITY QUOTIENT AND ACADEMIC PERFORMANCE AMONG COLLEGE STUDENTS AT ST. JOSEPH’SCOLLEGE, QUEZON CITY An undergraduate thesis Presented to the Faculty of The Departments of Arts and Sciences St. Joseph’s College Quezon City In Partial Fulfillment of the Requirement for the Degree of Bachelor of Science in Psychology By: ZHOU HUIJUAN March, 2009 RECOMMENDATION This Thesis entitled The Adversity Quotient and Academic Performance among College Students at St. Joseph’s College, Quezon City. Submitted by Zhou, Huijuan has been examined and found satisfactory and is hereby recommended for ORAL DEFENSE. Ms. Mildred L. Lazo Thesis Adviser APPROVAL SHEET In Partial fulfillment of the requirements fro the degree of Bachelor of Science in Psychology, this thesis entitled “The Adversity Quotient and Academic Performance among College Students at St. Joseph’s College, Quezon City” was prepared and submitted to the College of Arts and Science by Zhou, Huijuan. Approved by the committee on Oral Defense on March 8, 2009 with a grade of passed. Mrs. Nelia G. Prieto Chair, Liberal Arts Ms. Mildred Lazo Panel Member Mr. Francisco Lambojon Panel Member Accepted in partial fulfillment of the degree of Bachelor of Sciences Major in Psychology. Sr. Josephini P. Ambatali, SFIC Dean Acknowledgement This work would not have been possible without the presence and contribution of many valued individuals. Through this limited paper, I wish to express my endless, sincerest and eternal...

Words: 20286 - Pages: 82

Free Essay

Doc, Docx, Pdf, Wps, Rtf, Odt

...profile consisted of his attitude towards attendance in classes, time allocation for studies, parents’ level of income, mother’s age and mother’s education. The research is based on student profile developed on the bases of information and data collected through survey from students of a group of private colleges. Public sector educational institutions are not the focus of this study. IntroductionMeasuring of academic performance of students is challenging since studentperformance is product of socio-economic, psychological and environmentalfactors. For the last 20 years, education in Pakistan is growing as a profitableindustry with prime objective of maximizing profit by delivering high qualityeducation that produces well-educated, skilled, mannered students according toneeds and requirements of the dynamically growing market. That’s why thescope of research is always there to find out what are the factors that affect theperformance of the students. There are two groups of students as generallyperceived i.e. those who improve and those who don’t improve. This study cancontribute to find out the factors, which are responsible for student’s inelasticbehavior towards study along with identifying those factors, which help a studentto make progress in his studies. This study focuses on investigating the factorsaffecting performance of 3 rd and 4 th year college students equal to Europeans standard K-12 and K-14. A survey was conducted to collect information andresponses of...

Words: 3190 - Pages: 13

Premium Essay

Gf App

...The Official Girlfriend Application O BASIC INFORMATION 1. Full legal name Last First Middle 2. Age 6. Measurements 3. Height 4. Weight 3. Natural hair color CONTACT INFORMATION 9. Home phone ( 11. Email ) 10. Cell Phone ( ) - GETTING TO KNOW YOU 12. Are you a virgin? Y N 13. If no, how many past sexual partners have you had? N 15. Do you smoke? Y N N 16. Do you use any 14. Have you ever had a sex change? Y illegal substances? Y 19. Do you workout? Y what is it? do you currently reside with? N N 17. Do you have kids? Y 20. Do you currently have a source of income? Y 22. Do you live on your own? Y N 25. Furthest level of edumacation (circle one): 26. Do you have a history of mental illness? Y 28. Have you ever cheated on a boyfriend? Y High School N N 1 Copyright © 2005 Brody Vercher. N TH This application must be filled out in its entirety in order to be considered for the position that you are applying. Photographs may sway my opinion one way or the other, feel free to attach any that you think may help you gain this position. OFFICIAL GI RLFRIEND Just a reminder: be completely honest with all your answers, I will be double checking applications using Google to ensure honesty. Any false information will automatically nullify any chance you might have had. Thanks, and have fun. AP PLICAT E I 5. Eye color 4. Current hair color 18. If yes, how many? N 21. If yes, 23. If no, whom 24. What kind of car...

Words: 603 - Pages: 3

Premium Essay

Birth Order Literature Review

...Literature Review It can be said that research on siblings is not a new concept or research interest,it can be traced back to many years ago to the time when Sir Francis Galton,cousin of Charles Darwin ,conducted a research on siblings (Brody, 1998).Galton conducted his study on 180 men all of whom were members of the Royal Society ,he asked them to fill in a questionnaire which had questions like indicating their birth order position and age among their siblings. The results of his study were based upon the information gathered from 99 respondents,he concluded that (1) only sons being as common as eldest sons; (2)presence of twice as many elder sons relative to the younger ones ; (3) lastly stating how there was a uniform number of birth...

Words: 2285 - Pages: 10

Premium Essay

Application Form

...may help you gain this position. OFFICIAL Y BO FRIEN D Just a reminder: be completely honest with all your answers, I will be double checking applications using Google to ensure honesty. Any false information will automatically nullify any chance you might have had. Thanks, and have fun. AP PLICATI E BASIC INFORMATION 1. Full legal name Last First Middle 2. Age 6. Measurements 3. Height 4. Weight 3. Natural hair color CONTACT INFORMATION 9. Home phone ( 11. Email ) 10. Cell Phone ( ) - GETTING TO KNOW YOU 12. Are you a virgin? Y N 13. If no, how many past sexual partners have you had? N 15. Do you smoke? Y N N 16. Do you use any 14. Have you ever had a sex change? Y illegal substances? Y 19. Do you workout? Y what is it? do you currently reside with? N N 17. Do you have kids? Y 20. Do you currently have a source of income? Y 22. Do you live on your own? Y N 25. Furthest level of edumacation (circle one): 26. Do you have a history of mental illness? Y 28. Have you ever cheated on a girlfriend? Y High School N N 1 Copyright © 2005 Brody Vercher. N O TH 5. Eye color 4. Current hair color 18. If yes, how many? N 21. If yes, 23. If no, whom 24. What kind of car do you drive? Some College Associate’s Degree Bachelor’s Degree 27. Favorite sport & team 29. Do you cook? Y N 30. Do you have any siblings? Y 32. What is your political persuasion you have? favorite movie of all time? N 31. What is your...

Words: 598 - Pages: 3

Premium Essay

Film

...Evolution: Daneil Cztrom: 1930s caused a shift, 4 trends allowed the rise of modern media research propaganda research- war efforts public opinion- mseaures public attitudes citizen surveys for insights on social behavior and differences ex: deep water drilling poll pseudo calls (online, call in) to address the question of th day social psychology studies- measures the behavior and cognition of individuals Payne Fund studies: Marketing research- surveys on consumer buying habits * connecting cocooning: happy affluent family on the eastern coast, all having their own private media bubble today, the family would have ereaders or IPADS, ambient connectivity visions of the future digital home clip 1: vacuuming the house clip 2: a day made of glass the digital home expensive, the good life average person consumes 34G’s a day information abundance “wealth of information creates a poverty of attention” – Herbet Alexander Simon we can only read about 1-2 pages and then we bounce glance theory- we only glance at channels, ads, status updates and then move on hierarchy of digital distractions – least=work, middle=facebook, twiiter, high=romantic email, top= digital pain like dropping a phone, screen crash “once I was a scuba drive, and now I am a jet ski” selective retention theory: how we read weed through the information abundance, read some, then bounce. Communication fatigue- tamagotchi trend in social networking… basically you need to all update...

Words: 3125 - Pages: 13

Premium Essay

Aa Sex

...popular genres of music for the younger generations. It is more than a genre of music, it is a complete industry filled with clothing and other merchandise. The reason this constant demeaning of women exists is because rap as a genre that rewards the objectification of women. The excuses used to justify the misogyny in rap are incomplete and lack accurate support. The most effective way for this continuous cycle ends, is if the fight and protest comes from the women themselves. Men are the problem in the objectification of women but in order for it to stop, women need to step up and take control of the situation. Women need to act sooner rather than later because in recent years the rap industry has become more and more sexual. Ra... ... middle of paper ... ...Society 113 (2000): 255-69. JSTOR. 29 November 2009 . McLune, Jennifer. "Hip-Hop's Betrayal of Black Women." Perspective on Contemporary Issues. 5th ed. Boston: Wadsworth Cengag Learning, 2009. 247-51. Print. Thrash, Rodney. "Women Say Rap Videos Demean, Not Define." St. Petersburg Times 14 June 2005. 29 November 2009 . Williams, Dana. "Beyond Rap: Musical Misogyny." Teaching Tolerance (2003): 213-15. Tolerance in the News. 12 Aug. 2003. 29 November 2009 Rap Music's Influence Upon Teenagers :: 6 Works Cited Length: 1389...

Words: 5327 - Pages: 22

Free Essay

Gift Giving Norway

...at Christmas among 50 graduate students in Norway. The students invested more the closer the coefficient of relatedness. However, partners ranked highest, which is natural for people at the start of their reproductive career. All students gave to their parents, siblings, and children, most gave to their grandparents, and only a third gave to some, but not all, of their genetic aunts/uncles. Twenty percent gave to first cousins, and none to second or third cousins. Similar patterns for gifts received were found. There were also sex differences (e.g. women had larger exchange networks than men), and birth order effects. Firstborns spent more on relatives than laterborns. However, middleborns gave more to their male friends than both firstborns and lastborns. We conclude that the results are consistent with theories of kin selection, reciprocity, sex differences and birth order effects. Keywords: gift giving, kin selection, reciprocity, sex differences, birth order. ¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯ Introduction Gift giving is a central aspect of human behavior and culture (Carrier, 1995; Cheal, 1988; Davis, 1992,...

Words: 10449 - Pages: 42

Premium Essay

Sociology in Our Times

...Licensed to: CengageBrain User Licensed to: CengageBrain User This is an electronic version of the print textbook. Due to electronic rights restrictions, some third party content may be suppressed. Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. The publisher reserves the right to remove content from this title at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it. For valuable information on pricing, previous editions, changes to current editions, and alternate formats, please visit www.cengage.com/highered to search by ISBN#, author, title, or keyword for materials in your areas of interest. Copyright 2011 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it. Licensed to: CengageBrain User Sociology in Our Times: Ninth Edition Diana Kendall Sponsoring Editor: Erin Mitchell Developmental Editor: Renee Deljon/Kristin Makarewycz Freelance Development Editor: Tricia Louvar Assistant Editor: Linda Stewart Editorial Assistant: Mallory Ortberg Media Editor: Mary Noel Marketing Manager: Andrew Keay ...

Words: 24544 - Pages: 99

Free Essay

Devorce

...This guide is intended to help you use the uncontested divorce forms provided by www.TexasLawHelp.org Do not use these forms or this guide if: The Uncontested Divorce Process in Texas  Your divorce is contested*  The wife is pregnant.  You have a bankruptcy pending. (If you have a pending bankruptcy, talk with a bankruptcy lawyer before filing for divorce.)  Neither you nor your spouse has lived in Texas in the last 6 months. Is your divorce contested? -OrIs your divorce uncontested? *Your case is contested when you and your spouse don’t agree about getting the divorce, dividing your property and debts, or what to do with your children. DO NOT USE THESE FORMS or this brochure for a contested divorce. Your case is uncontested when:  It is ‘agreed’ – You and your spouse agree about all of the issues in your case. or  It is ‘default’ – Your spouse does not file an answer with the Court after being officially served with your divorce paperwork. Table of Contents Basic Information, page 2 Know the Steps, page 3 Legal Notice Chart, page 6 Are you Ready for Court? page 7 Common Questions, page 8 REMEMBER: It is always best to have the advice of a lawyer. The court clerks, librarians, and judges want to help you, but they cannot give you legal advice, help you fill out your forms, or tell you what to do. If you proceed without a lawyer, you will be responsible for protecting yourself. Texas Divorce Brochure © Partnership for Legal Access 12/14/09 Page...

Words: 21858 - Pages: 88

Premium Essay

Obseity

...THE PM PROTECTION AGAINST DI P THE CHILD RECEIVING TH ',CUR, SUCH AS ANTIPY R li CARE PROVIDER . PARENT AS STATED IN THE NAsTION 'TANCE OF COMPLETING TH 1EACTIONS TO THEIR HEA L e immunization procedures . gulanttherapy. ale needle and syringe. ion with a pertussis-containing Raneously AM Oral Poliovim s re not available concerning th e I studies with TETRAMUNE di d arcinogenic mutagenic poles— - -MOM Obesity and Stature in Adolescenc e and Earnings in Young Adulthoo d Analysis of a British Birth Cohor t James D . Sargent, MD, David G . Blanchflower, PhD NE This product is not recomI been established. t Use (Td) is recommended. R e (DT) should be substituted in Ise) is based on a full course o f months of age in three separate on systemic symptoms within Objective: To examine the association between obe- sity and stature at various ages and earnings in young me n and women at age 23 years . Design : We estimated the effect of obesity on earnings xns Toddlers (107 doses) by constructing a series of ordinary least-squares regression equations in which the dependent variable was the natural logarithm of hourly earnings at age 23 years . We report the coefficients for obese subjects compared with those for the nonobese subjects and for height while con trolling for a number of other factors that are known t o affect pay . Setting : A birth cohort of 12 537 respondents at age 2 3 i9 to 7500 doses) in the Kaiser rilhin 60...

Words: 7087 - Pages: 29

Premium Essay

Psychology Article

...Karachi Abstract Numerous researchers investigated the relationship between job satisfaction and work motivation in diversified professions but none investigated bank managers in Pakistan. Therefore, the current study attempts to explore the relationship between work motivation and job satisfaction. Based on the literature review two hypotheses were formulated 1) There will be positive relationship between work motivation and job satisfaction, and 2) There would be a gender difference on the variable of work motivation and job satisfaction. To asses job satisfaction, “Job Satisfaction Survey (JSS; Spector, 1997)”, and for motivation, “Motivation at Work Scale (MAWS, Gagné, M., et.al, 2010)”, were used. The sample of the study consists of 80 middle managers (46 males; 34 females) from different banks of Karachi, Pakistan. A Pearson Product Moment Correlation Coefficient was calculated for the correlation between the work motivation and job satisfaction. In order to find out gender differences t test was computed. In conclusion, the findings of the study suggest that there was a positive correlation between work motivation and job satisfaction. Furthermore, there was a significant gender difference on the variable of work motivation and job satisfaction. Keywords: Motivation, job satisfaction, gender difference JEL Classification: J81, O15, Z13, J28 *An earlier version of this refereed paper was presented at the first Business Psychology Seminar held by the Department of Business Psychology...

Words: 4314 - Pages: 18