Free Essay

Methodological Issues in Management Research

In:

Submitted By atiq895
Words 6205
Pages 25
Methodological Issues in Management Research

Lee Sechrest, PhD
Professor
Department of Psychology
University of Arizona
Room 312
Tuscon, AZ 85721

White paper prepared for the Department of Veterans Affairs Management Research in VA Workshop, sponsored by the HSR&D Management Decision and Research Center
November 19-20, 2001

Methodological Issues in Management Research

Managers want to make good decisions. Any decisions will, by definition, be made on the basis of some presumed information. Even if a decision were to be made by throwing dice, that process would almost certain stem from “information” indicating that no better basis for the decision could be discerned, e.g., that a randomly determined choice would be likely to be better than a decision open to bias. At least to some extent, it is axiomatic that the better the information, the better the decisions.

It is useful to distinguish between data, facts, and information. Data are simply observations, usually in the form of numbers thought to represent some systematic process underlying them, i.e., a process generating the numbers. Data do not mean anything or tell us anything until they are interpreted in some way. Merely to have an observation that on a particular day 43 patients were reported to have received a particular service is not in itself meaningful. Facts are merely data elevated in confidence to a point of suggested certainty. The observation that 43 patients received a service may be registered as a fact if it seems likely that the data are sufficiently trustworthy to justify confidence that the data are correct. Facts come in all varieties: numbers, declarative statements, equations, and so on. But facts are not necessarily information For example, if one is told that the land area of Bahrain is 231 sq.mi., that might be factual, but it might not constitute information. A useful definition of information is that it is any communication that reduces uncertainty with respect to some decision. That Bahrain is 231 sq. mi. is information only if one is in the position of having to make some decision involving that information, e.g., about whether to invest in a manufacturing enterprise in that country or whether, in a game, to choose Bahrain as the smallest of the Arab states. Even the precise estimate for land area might not be information if one already knows that Bahrain is very small and that is all the precision of knowledge that one requires.

These distinctions are important in thinking about research because not all research findings are necessarily factual, and the facts that are acquired from research efforts are not necessarily information. The reason is that research findings can be—must be—interpreted at different levels. It may be, for example, that in a particular organization more patients were processed by Unit A than by Unit B. That could lead to a conclusion that the management style employed in Unit A results in greater productivity than the management style employed in Unit B. That, in turn, could lead to the conclusion that Management Style A is superior to Management Style B. And so on. Reflection may show, however, that the only undeniable “fact” available is that more patients were recorded as having been processed by Unit A than by Unit B. The apparent better productivity of A could reflect no more than errors in recording patients processed. Or the difference, although real factually, might have happened simply by chance. The difference might be real enough but also so small as to be uninteresting. Or, even if the difference were real and sizable, there is a considerable leap in the conclusion that it should be attributable to differences in management style, and there is even a greater leap in the conclusion that one management style should be considered generally superior to the other.

Research, as suggested, may result in specific findings that can be regarded as factual and that may represent information in the sense of reducing uncertainty about decisions. Research may also be supportive of more general theory about the phenomena of interest. Thus, research on a particular management style in a particular context may be supportive of a more general theory of or about management. Even though a particular research project may be rather specific in many of its details, it may still bear on very general principles of organizational management. Conversely, a rather general theory may permit derivation of recommendations concerning specific practices. Although the precision either way, reasoning from specific findings to general theory or from general theory to specific practices, may be limited, the process is likely to result in better decisions that would be made by chance and probably better than would be made overall by intuition.

Theory and research should reflect a continuing interplay of observation and synthesis. It is possibile that theories may sometimes be better than our facts, i.e., the results of specific research. Any one research project is certain to be limited in important ways, at least in terms of its generalizability to other types of persons, to other settings, to different versions of the intervention, and so on. Moreover, any given research project is subject to limitations resulting from a wide range of errors that are inevitable. Measures are not perfect, interventions are not perfectly implemented, individuals differ in their reactions to the same set of conditions, reactions to interventions or other arrangements may vary over the course of a day, or a week, or a month. Thus, if there is a plausible, well-reasoned theory that tells us that a particular intervention ought to decrease the time taken to do some task, but the expected decrease does not happen, it may not necessarily be the case that the theory is wrong. It may well be that the data just were not up to the task of testing the theory.

In order to produce information that will be dependably useful to managers, research must be carefully planned, carried out, and interpreted. Good research does not just happen. It is the result of deliberate application of well-tested methods. The aim of this paper is to outline some of those methods and why they are important. Causal attribution

The aim of research is, generally, to reveal causal connections between variables, i.e., to demonstrate that one variable is a cause of another variable. When the demonstration is sufficient, then we are, at least potentially, in the position of being in control of the "effect" (caused) variable. We may, if we are able to change the causal variable, e.g., eliminate it, strengthen it, produce a change in the effect variable. Research is sometimes carried out for descriptive purposes, but the results of descriptive research are rarely of great interest in their own right. For example, a manager may wonder whether costs of some activity are higher on some days of the week than on others and may assemble data relevant to that question. That costs are higher, let us say, on Mondays than on other days may be a curious "fact," but it is of value only insofar as it suggests the possibility of another, follow-on study that will explain the higher cost, i.e., that will explain why the cost is higher, what causes the higher cost on Monday. If that can be determined, then the manager may be able to intervene to reduce the excess cost or may understand that the cost, although larger, is warranted because it is attributable to desirable features of the operation of the organization, e.g., more services are provided on Mondays or more serious problems are dealt with on Mondays.

Making correct causal attributions is often far from straightforward. At the heart of the matter is that a causal inference requires dealing with the counterfactual. That is, making a causal inference requires at least an implicit answer to the question "What would have happened if the causal event had not occurred?" That may be a relatively simple matter if a causal connection is direct and well understood. If more services are offered, then costs are almost certain to increase. Moreover, it is likely to seem pretty clear that if services are cut, costs will go down. If, however, it is observed that a group of new workers given three days extra training make "only" two errors per shift in the month following training, it may not be so easy to be sure how they would have performed had they not had the training. One cannot both given the training and not give the training. An inference is required.

Descriptive research may be quite useful when it helps to understand some process, particularly if that process is amenable to intervention. It may be important to know, for example, whether supervisors actually read documents that cross their desks for signatures in order to plan for streamlining of the flow of paper through an organization. Such a study might be limited to enquiry by questionnaire, but it might involve interviews or even observations.

Necessity for comparison observations

When one thinks about it, no observation is meaningful in and of itself. Observations acquire meaning by the opportunity to compare them to some expected or observed value. Stars are dim only because some other stars are bright. An object is likely to be seen as blue only if it might well have been another color. An observation (or set of observations) made in an organization is interpretable only in relation to some prior expectations. Those expectations might have been derived in any number of ways: logic, common sense, theory, or other observations.

We attribute causality to some intervention in relation to some event because we are capable of imagining that the outcome might have been otherwise, i.e., we invoke the counterfactual possibility. If an assistant tells a manager, “We are having fewer staff complaints from the long-term care unit,” and the manager replies, “That’s because of the new procedures we instituted last quarter,” the manager is implicitly accepting the proposition that had the procedures not been instituted the complaints would not have decreased. What might be the justification for that proposition?

Logic? Common sense? It may seem to the manager and the assistant simply logical if things are not going well and a new set of procedures to improve matters is introduced, improvement should occur and that any that does occur should be attributed to the intervention. Things are rarely so simple. Almost always some plausible rival explanations exist, and, if one of them is correct, it may change the decision about what to do in an important way. For example, some studies had shown that the testing of prospective employees for honesty resulted in reductions in episodes of theft or inventory loss. More definitive studies, however, seemed to indicate that the primary effect was the result simply of the demonstration to employees that management cared about theft and was resolved to do something about it. Testing employees for honesty can be expensive and hard on employee morale; by comparison communication of managers' concerns for theft may be inexpensive and accepted as a legitimate managerial function. Complaints from a unit might be reduced for similar reasons, i.e., simply because a manager expressed concern for them and a determination to reduce them.

Theory? Similar to logic but more explicitly reasoned is a theoretical justification for a conclusion. A manager might rely at least to some extent on a theory of management that would predict that a particular type of intervention would have the effect intended and result in an outcome reflected in a decrease in complaints. The legitimacy of the conclusion would depend heavily on the resemblances between the specific intervention and the type represented by the theory and between the outcome supposed by the theory and that realized in the empirical study. A characteristic of a theoretical explanation is that the mechanism for producing the change is explicit. For example, a particular intervention might operate to reduce complaints by improving conditions leading to complaints, by helping staff to be more accepting of deficient conditions, or by improving staff morale so as to decrease the likelihood of complaints.

Other observations? By contrast, a manager might have access to other observations that would support the conclusion that the change could be attributed to the intervention. The manager might note, for example, that complaints had been at a consistently high level for quite some period of time, decreasing only when the intervention was implemented. Or the manager might know that on one or more other units in which no intervention was tried, no decrease in complains was found. The manager might even be satisfied to know that after the intervention, complaints were no more frequent than reported in other similar organizations in his or her geographic area.

Comparison data.

Interpretation of any observations is, obviously, much enhanced by having available data from other sources with which to compare observations at hand. Essentially, the reason is that the comparison data make it possible to make a judgment in relation to the counterfactual notion of what the data would have looked like under other circumstances, e.g., if the intervention had not occurred. If complaints seem high and an intervention is tried, followed by a decrease in complaints, the counterfactual asks what the complain level would have been without the intervention. Answering that question is not always easy, and the answer is not only obvious. Complaints might have been going down anyway; a change in patient mix might have made fewer complaints more likely; new personnel in the unit, independent of the intervention, might have elicited fewer complaints.

Comparison data could be helpful in varying degrees, depending on just what kinds of comparisons were available. Inspection of rate-of-complaint data prior to the intervention might show that the idea that complaints were going down anyway was unlikely.
Those data would not necessarily be helpful in determining whether the change might have been due to changes in personnel or patient mix.

True (randomized) experiments.

The best comparison data would result from circumstances in which everything was identical to the conditions in the group exposed to the intervention except the occurrence of the intervention itself. The problem is that so often it is difficult, sometimes impossible, to be sure that everything was the same except for the intervention. Two units might, however apparently similar, differ in subtle ways in patient mix, or in personnel, or in some other variables that might be related to the occurrence of complaints. No matter how carefully units were selected and how similar they seemed to be, the possibility exists that the patients in them would differ in some unknown, but important way. By “important” is meant that the difference would be related in some way to an outcome of interest, say the tendency to complain.

The best way to maximize the likelihood that patients on two units will be the same even on unobserved variables is to assign them randomly to the two units. By such an assignment process, if a patient with a low tendency toward complaining were assigned to the unit to receive the intervention, probability would guarantee that a similarly low patient would be likely (but not certainly) to be assigned to the other unit. Over a series of observations, chance generally evens things out. The logic of scientific inquiry then is that if one can assume that two groups (units, in this case) were equivalent to begin with, then any later difference between them can be attributed to differences in the way they were treated, i.e., the intervention. The equivalent comparison group answers the question posed by the counterfactual, what things would have been like in the absence of the intervention.

That, in a nutshell, is the essence of the randomized clinical trial, almost universally regarded as the strongest basis for an inference about a causal relationship between two variables. Any other comparison data are considered inherently weaker as a basis for causal inference and, therefore, more likely to be misleading.

Threats to validity of the inference.

It is common practice to refer to problems with various comparison data series in terms of “threats to validity,” meaning threats to the validity of a causal inference. It will help to review some of the more likely threats and to indicate how they might crop up in specific comparisons.

Selection. If groups, say intervention and comparison groups are assembled in any way other than by random selection, there is at least a possibility, often a near certainty, that differential selection into the groups will have made them different from the beginning, thus making any final difference difficult to interpret. For example, if groups are assembled by asking for volunteers for the intervention, that group may be more enthusiastic, higher in risk-taking, more eager to please, of more of almost anything else than the left-over group of non-volunteers. Thus, if the intervention group turns out better than the comparison group, that difference cannot unequivocally be attributed to the intervention itself. Even if one takes intact groups, e.g., patients in two general medical units of a hospital, one cannot be certain that they do not differ in important ways. Medical studies, for example, have run afoul of the problem that patients selected for one treatment over another tend to be different from patients no so selected. In one well-known study, patients who received surgical treatment had better outcomes than patients not subjected to surgery, but that ultimately was shown to be because patients not selected for surgery were too sick to endure it.

Maturation. Naturally occurring processes may be mistaken for intervention effects under some circumstances. If one unit has very new leadership and another is well established, changes associated with the maturing of the leadership could be mistaken for effects of an intervention. If costs of some process are increasing over time, an intervention might appear to result in increased costs even thought it were cost neutral.

History. Sometimes in the middle of a research study, external events will occur that have an impact on the phenomenon being studied. A researcher unaware of a change in accounting procedures in an organization might mistakenly conclude that an intervention decreased costs when, in fact, the change was illusory. Events outside the system may, similarly, have effects that could be taken for the effects of an intervention. It is entirely likely, for example, that the events of Sept. 11 and subsequently may have affected all sorts of responses made within work and treatment facilities. Productivity probably dropped for a while, various complaints probably decreased, and absenteeism increased. Because of the enormous salience of Sept. 11, few investigators would be likely to miss its effects on most data being collected during that time. Similar effects might well be missed, however, if they were less obvious. History is a special threat to studies involving comparisons of data for the same group over time, but if history has more impact on one group than another, it can affect any comparison.

Regression artifact. Observed values of any variable must be considered to be in some part in error. If an observation is toward one extreme or another of a distribution, the probability is that error is in some degree involved in the location of the observation. For example, if on some occasion it is observed that complaints from a treatment unit are much higher than average, the probability is great that the number is high in part because of the odd confluence of factors not likely to persist for long, e.g., a temporary staff shortage, a malcontented patient, an equipment breakdown. Then, with no intervention at all, the number of complaints would fall at the next occasion of measurement (called regression toward the mean). Because interventions are likely to be initiated exactly when complaints are high, the natural effects of regression can easily be mistaken for an intervention effect. Incidentally, and conversely, an intervention that happens to come along when things are noted to be especially good may appear to have a bad effect because regression works in both directions.

Research Designs

As noted earlier, the randomized experiment is generally regarded as the gold standard for research, particularly for supporting causal inferences. So why are randomized experiments not used exclusively? In the first place, they simply cannot be done for many problems because the variable of interest cannot be experimentally controlled, whether for practical or for ethical reasons. We are interested in the effects of education on people’s lives, but we cannot “give” education to people who do not want it. Similarly, we are interested in the effects of sanctions on criminal behavior, but we cannot, ethically—or legally—punish some people and not punish others on an experimental basis. Second, randomization may be unacceptable to some persons or groups, i.e., they may be unwilling to be randomized to experimental conditions. If randomization is possible, it is usually preferable as a research option, but often alternatives must be sought.

A wide range of options for research is available, each with specific advantages and deficiencies dependent on the nature of the problem. These options are frequently referred to a quasi-experimental designs. All research designs are, however, simply ways of systematizing observations in such a way as to maximize information concerning the counterfactual proposition. Those research designs likely to be of greatest usefulness are the following:

Nonequivalent comparison group design.

The most widely used quasi-experimental design is, undoubtedly, the nonequivalent comparison group design, by which is meant a research scheme that calls for collecting data from an intervention group and a comparison group that do not involve random assignments. Consequently, the groups must be regarded as nonequivalent, as potentially having been selected in such a way that they are unequal to begin with. Two hospitals differing in management types may be compared, and they may actually be quite equivalent save for management, but there is no way of knowing that, so they must be regarded as nonequivalent. Obviously, the more reason with which one can argue that the groups or organizations are equivalent, the better the case for interpreting any outcome differences between them. That is why when, as is inevitable, plans must be made to compare interventions between two groups or organizations, it is important to select the two (or more) with as much care as possible. One would not opt deliberately to compare two organizations of widely disparate size or from very different communities unless those variables, size or geography, were the exact conditions of interest.

Initial differences between groups or organizations may be dealt with statistically or by developing other, supportive comparison data sets. Statistical allowances for differences is often, but not always, straightforward, depending on the nature and size of the initial differences. Statistical corrections for initial differences also depend for their persuasiveness on the quality and relevance of the data available. If initial measures are available on the variables to be measured as outcomes, allowance for differences is not likely to be terribly controversial, at least if differences are not large and other assumptions are tenable. When, however, it is necessary to use proxy variables, controversy is inevitable. For example, if one has to use a symptom-count as a proxy for severity of illness, the correction for potential initial differences in severity may be questionable.

Interrupted time series.

Often data are assembled systematically over time, usually for administrative or clinical rather than for research purposes. If, in the middle of a data series, some event occurs relevant to the processes involved in producing the series, it may be possible to estimate the effect of the event by comparing the values in the series prior to the interruption to those obtained afterwards; hence the label “interrupted time series.” Consider, for example, that on a particular date, say the first day of July, the procedures involved in providing aftercare for patients is changed and later that year a manager wonders whether the change may have affected subsequent follow-up visits to an outpatient department. The manager discovers that records of such visits are available by week for the six-month period prior to the change and later. The manager might then plan to continue assembling the data until six months of records are available subsequent to the change. It might then be possible to compare the data series before the change with the data afterwards and reach some conclusions about the effect of the change. Basically, what the manager wants to do is estimate what the data would have looked like without the change (the counterfactual) and compare that estimate with the actual data. The manager’s statistician would look for a change in level (intercept) of the two series at the point of the change and for differences in slopes of the series between the two time periods, e.g., whether a general trend in an upward direction prior to the change might reverse to a downward trend after.

The interrupted time series can be a very useful and persuasive research strategy under the right circumstances. Those circumstances begin with the availability of records if the time frame is long (managers will not want to wait for very long to assemble the necessary data) and will include the stability of the series (widely fluctuating values may frustrate attempts to detect changes) and the abruptness of the change (effects of phased-in interventions are difficult to detect). The research strategy does lend itself very readily to many administrative interventions if quick answers are not required or if a long-term perspective is needed.

Observational data with statistical corrections. A very common research strategy, but scarcely a design in the usual sense, is to assemble observational data and attempt to “rule out” rival explanations by logic and statistical means. A recent example is a study based on a large data set including extensive questionnaire responses of 78,000 nurses. The investigators were interested in factors determining the occurrence of breast cancer and discovered that nurses who had worked on night shifts for an extended period of time had an increased risk of breast cancer. That is, they compared breast cancer rates in nurses who did and did not work night shifts and related breast cancer rates to number of years working night shifts. Since many other variables could have been related both to night shift work and to breast cancer risk, they “adjusted” the data for those variables by statistical means. The data for the women who did not work night shifts, when adjusted for “confounding” variables, presumably showed what the data for the other nurses would have looked like if they had not worked night shifts.

An advantage of observational data is that they are usually fairly easy to obtain and are often already available in existing data files. On the other hand, the investigator must often make do with whatever data are available in those files. In the nurses data file, night shift work was defined only as at least three nights per month; a more refined measure would have been desirable. Even if observational data are being collected de novo, the investigator must rely on whatever values for variables happen to show up in the data. If a particular condition of interest is rare, then it simply will not show up very often, although when it does, the investigator can try to make sure that the case gets included in the data set. If a manager is interested in the effectiveness of bilingual supervisors, he or she will be limited to observing the effectiveness of those supervisors actually on the job.

Moreover, observational data are often not likely to be very persuasive when sample sizes are small. Statistical corrections for confounding groups usually require a fairly large number of cases.

Observational data are particularly subject to biases resulting from “data dredging,” looking through large quantities of data until something interesting appears to turn up.

Measurement

Good measurement is of absolutely critical importance to good research. It is unfortunate that problems in measurement so often go unrecognized and, if recognized, are treated so lightly. It is impossible to demonstrate effectiveness of any intervention without reliable measurement. Think, for example, of trying to “influence” the a variable with values created by throwing dice. That is exactly what is involved when measures of outcomes are unreliable. Measures that have only modest reliability can be expected to show at best only modest effects of interventions. Poor (unreliable) measures will almost always result in an underestimate of the effects of any intervention.

Prescriptions for good measurement are easily made but not so easily followed. Good measurement begins with good definitions of just what it is to be measured, good definitions of constructs. Multiple measures are highly desirable, especially when measures differ substantially in their structure and likely sources of bias. Measures should, ideally, be nonreactive, i.e., they should not be readily susceptible to self-serving biases and other sources of distortion. They should also be closely related to the phenomena of interest so that values on the measure map clearly onto the underlying variables of true concern. Such prescriptions are obvious; realizing them is difficult.

One measurement problem that is very often overlooked ins the assessment of the intervention or, the independent variable, as it is often called in the jargon of research methodology. Just as we cannot expect to show much effect on a variable that is poorly measured, so we cannot show much effect from a variable that is only weakly implemented. Researchers too often take the independent variable for granted, and only infrequently do they attempt to quantify it. If an accounting system is to be evaluated, then it is important to know to what extent and how well the system is implemented. If those using it are poorly trained, if they do not like it and, hence, do not use it, or if it involves technical difficulties that result in frequent down-time, a fair evaluation of the system cannot be obtained. Just as it is important in evaluating medications to know how much of a dose was actually received by patients, so it is important to know how much of an intervention was actually achieved in a managerial setting.

Methodological consultation.

The design of a good, persuasive research project nearly always requires technical expertise for the many decisions to be made. Managers who wish to become involved in research should be quick to seek methodological—and statistical—consultation. Consideration may also need to be given to the need for consultation on measurement.

Statistical analysis

Data must be subjected to statistical analyses before they are interpretable. Summary statistics such as means, standard deviations, and correlations are easily calculated; in fact, they can be done on simple spread-sheet programs. Their interpretations are not always straightforward, but they usually pose no great problems. Analyses aimed at inferring causes, however, are usually more complex and often are highly complex, requiring specialized knowledge and software. Research outside laboratories, and most of that in laboratories today, is necessarily a multidisciplinary enterprise. A managerial team must include specialists in administration, purchasing, accounting, maintenance, and so on, and a managerial research team must, similarly, include an appropriate mix of specialists.

Research will in every way be improved if statistical expertise is enlisted from the very beginning of a project. Few projects can be brought to a fully satisfactory conclusion by turning things over to a statistician to make sense of after all the other work is done.

Generalizability of research

An issue of persistent concern in the interpretation and use of information derived from research is the extent to which the results of the research may be generalized to other settings, i.e. to settings similar, but not identical, to that in which the research was done. Concerns about generalization often relate to fairly obvious characteristics of settings, e.g., the identity of the organizations or the people associated with them, but legitimate bases for concerns may lie at a deeper level. Superficially, a manufacturing facility and a health care facility seem quite dissimilar, but with respect to the fundamental organizational processes by which they operate, they may be quite similar: leadership requirements, information needs, accountability provisions, quality controls, etc. Researchers can facilitate generalization greatly by being explicit in their assumptions about the settings in which they are working and the nature of the variables underlying the processes they are studying.

To some extent every research study has to be regarded as constituting a “special case of real life.” That is, real life is complicated, messy, dynamic; research studies, of necessity, either impose order on or derive order from the situation and the resulting data. Research
Requires simplification along many dimensions, and that simplification may restrict the generalizability of the interpretations of findings. A great strength of experiments is that they are designed in such a way as to keep everything as simple as possible and as nearly identical as possible between groups. Those restrictions may mean, however, that conditions in the experiment are considerably different from those that obtain in “real life,” thus making direct applicability of the findings questionable. For example, in an experiment to test a new accounting system, everyone involved would be likely to be very carefully trained and supervised, well beyond what would achievable in any ordinary, functioning organization. Research requirements often cause investigators to focus on specific outcome measures that may not capture all the consequences of interest, The important issues for generalizability often have to do with specific research arrangements related to the intervention, the circumstances of its implementation, and the measurement of outcomes. With respect to generalizability, those issues may be more important than more obvious things such as the identity of the organization of the types of people working in it or served by it.

In biomedical research a distinction is often made between efficacy and effectiveness of interventions, the former referring generally to effects of interventions implemented under carefully controlled and sometimes artificial conditions, and the latter to effects obtainable under more-or-less real life conditions. In biomedical research it is regularly necessary to carry out studies related to the transition from the “ideal” conditions to the laboratory to the conditions of the every day world. Undoubtedly, management research may sometimes require the same kinds of transitional studies.

Observational studies offer the advantage that they usually occur in real world settings, and they may not require as many constraints as are necessary to carry out experiments. (It is the case, however, that observational studies may at least require simplifications involved in quantifying inputs and outcomes.) A major shortcoming of observational studies, however, aside from their general messiness (because nothing is really controlled) is that the legitimacy of any causal inferences may be very much in doubt.

In the first place, the direction of causality may be more apparent than real. In a study of the effects of shift work on family relationships, it appeared that shift-workers had poorer family relationships than workers with only daytime jobs. Further investigation suggested, however, the probability that a good many workers choose shift work in order to get away from unpleasant family relationships. When that possibility was taken into account, the effect disappeared. Causal inferences must be developed with great care in observational studies.

Secondly, however, observational studies may be questionable as a basis for causal inferences because of the necessity for assuming that naturally occurring, observed correlations would hold for deliberate interventions. For example, an investigator might determine that a relationship existed between amount of training and productivity of workers. That finding might lead to the supposition that giving more training to workers would increase productivity. That inference would be a big leap, however. It is not only likely, but probable, that workers who have, on their own, acquired more training differ systematically from workers without such training. It cannot at all be assumed that “giving” workers training would have the same effect as their having gotten it.

Thus, observational studies, too, have limitations on their generalizability. All the foregoing leads to the generalization that it is unusual that any important uncertainties can ever be resolved by single studies, whatever their nature, and even multiple studies should have only modest effects on our confidence that we know what we are doing.

Finally, it is important to remember that generalizations can be invoked at different levels: findings, principles, and theory. A particular research finding, let us say that implementing a particular form of feedback to workers about their performance, may generalize to other settings so that those that implement the same feedback get pretty much the same results. At a more general level, it might be that the principal that feedback improves performance is supported, so that a range of feedback arrangements could be counted on. And at a still more general level, the results of a feedback study could be regarded as generally supportive of a “theory of participative management,” strengthening it in some small way as a basis from which to derive a wide range of ideas about improving management and performance.

Similar Documents

Premium Essay

The Impact of Hrm on Organisational Performance

...weak because theoretical and methodological issues remain among most of these studies. So, this essay will mainly analyze theories concerning on HRM and firm performance relationships and methodological problems in empirical studies. As there are flaws in both theories and the process of studying the relationships, this essay concludes that the evidence in favor of HRM’s impact on organisatioanl performance is weak. It seems that it is better to pay more attention to the HRM’s theories and methodologies in order to further the understanding the correlation between HRM and firm performance. TABLE OF CONTENTS 1. Introduction 4 2. Evidence of previous studies 5 3. Theoretical evidence 8 3.1 Categorizing theories 9 3.1.1 Strategic theories 9 3.1.2 Descriptive theories 11 3.1.3 Normative theories 11 3.2 Conceptual theories 13 4. Methodological issue 15 4.1 Measurement of HRM 16 4.2 Measurement of performance 17 5. Conclusion 18 1. Introduction Human resource managers have been worrying about the inability to prove their contribution to the organisations for a long time (Drucker, 1954, and Stewart, 1996, cited in Wright, Gardner, Moynihan and Allen, 2005). As responses to the uncertainty of whether human resource management (HRM) has an impact on organisational performance, there have been a lot of studies emerging and attempting to prove HRM does add values to firms. Until now, a developing body of research has...

Words: 3635 - Pages: 15

Premium Essay

Postive Accounting Theory

...in Watts and Zimmerman’s 1986 book is taken to mean PAT, studies of accounting choices and auditing practices constitute PAT. At the same time, they also seek to explain the economics-based empirical literature in accounting and they describe, in addition to accounting choice studies, capital market-based accounting research. They point out that Ball and Brown (1968) initially popularized positive research in accounting, suggesting that PAT includes both capital market-based accounting research and research in accounting choices. This paper takes PAT to include both research programs. This usage is consistent with Watts and Zimmerman’s (1986) assertion that when they use the term “positive” to differentiate it from “prescriptive” theory. Positive Accounting Theory and Science by M. Humayun Kabir Senior Lecturer, Faculty of Business Auckland University of Technology, Auckland, New Zealand Abstract This paper examines the development of positive accounting theory (PAT) and compares it with three standard accounts of science: Popper (1959), Kuhn (1996), and Lakatos (1970). PAT has been one of the most influential accounting research programs during the last four decades. One important reason which Watts & Zimmerman (1986) have used to popularize and legitimize their approach is that their view of accounting theory is the same as that used in science. Thus, it is important to examine how far accounting has been successful in imitating natural science...

Words: 6941 - Pages: 28

Free Essay

Cross Culture Management Research Issues

...constant support and understanding during the work. SHUBHANGI SINGH A3907508034 IMBA A-16 (2008- 2012) Amity School of Business e- Mail – shubhangi_singh@live.in INDEX SERIAL NO. PARTICULARS PAGE NO. 1. Abstract 03 2. Introduction 04 3. Methodology 07 4. Literature review 08 5. Discussion 14 6 Conclusion 15 7. Suggestions 16 6. Appendices 17 7. References 18 ABSTRACT Confronted by the globalization of markets, increasing competition, deregulation and rapid technological developments, speed and flexibility become key sources of competitive advantage for organizations. This calls for radically different management approaches and a new breed of leaders. New mindsets and new sets of learning skills will be key factors of success in the knowledge intensive corporations of the future. The research field of cross-cultural management suffers from an absence of theory capable of explaining the...

Words: 5146 - Pages: 21

Premium Essay

Positive Accounting Theory

...Science by M. Humayun Kabir Senior Lecturer, Faculty of Business Auckland University of Technology, Auckland, New Zealand Abstract This paper examines the development of positive accounting theory (PAT) and compares it with three standard accounts of science: Popper (1959), Kuhn (1996), and Lakatos (1970). PAT has been one of the most influential accounting research programs during the last four decades. One important reason which Watts & Zimmerman (1986) have used to popularize and legitimize their approach is that their view of accounting theory is the same as that used in science. Thus, it is important to examine how far accounting has been successful in imitating natural science and how the development of PAT compares with the three standard accounts of science. This paper shows that accounting could not emulate the success of natural science. Further, the methodological positions of PAT conform to none of the standard accounts of science. Rather, PAT contains elements of all three. Finally, this paper identifies some methodological gaps in PAT. Keywords: Positive Accounting Theory, Philosophy of Science, Methodological Controversies Acknowledgements I would like to thank two anonymous reviewers of the journal for their helpful comments. Earlier versions of this paper benefited from comments from Lee Parker of the University of South Australia, Keith Hooper of Auckland University of Technology, Divesh Sharma of Kennesaw State University, and Santi Narayan Ghosh of the University...

Words: 9637 - Pages: 39

Premium Essay

Datamoitor

...PART I THEORY, SCHOOLS AND PRACTICE 1 Schools of Thought in Strategic Management: Fragmentation, Integration or Synthesis Tom Elfring and Henk W. Volberda Over the last thirty years, strategic management has become established as a legitimate field of research and managerial practice (Shrivastava, 1986: 363). In the evolution of strategy research, a diversity of partly competitive and partly supplementary paradigms have emerged. To provide an unequivocal definition would mean ignoring the versatility of strategic management. The choice of a definition and the application of specific strategic management techniques is greatly dependent on which paradigmatic schools of thought in strategic management one prefers. In this book, we will therefore review the various schools of thought and their contribution to the theory and practice of strategy. A school of thought is understood to be the range of thought of a specific group of researchers, which has crystallized within the field of strategic management (Brown, 1993). In other words, a school of thought can be seen as an institutionalized paradigm. Besides reflecting on the variety of schools in strategy, we will also sketch out new promising directions in strategy research and practice. Although some strategy scholars have argued that the achieved multiformity in schools of thought signifies an enrichment of the research within the field of study (cf. Mahoney, 1993), other scholars from related disciplines complain about...

Words: 11042 - Pages: 45

Premium Essay

Towards a Typology for Undertaking Doctoral Research in the Built Environment

...Towards a typology for undertaking doctoral research in the built environment Ade Alao Abstract This paper considers issues associated with identifying a typology for doctoral research in the built environment. It argues that a definition of the built environment knowledge base in terms of its constituent subject disciplines is unhelpful for doctoral research as the generation of new knowledge in an interdiscipline requires integration across the various subject disciplines. A typology for research design is identified as being capable of guiding the doctoral researcher but will require further research to tested and verify it’s theoretical and empirical basis. Keywords: Typology, Doctoral Research, Built Environment, Interdisciplinarity 1. Introduction “The built environment disciplines is a term that has come to be used by many UK universities to refer to a range of practice-oriented subjects concerned with the design, development and management of buildings, spaces and places….. …..they are a very heterogeneous collection of fields of study and practice, including architecture, town planning, land and property management, building surveying, construction technology, landscape design, housing policy and management, transport planning and urban regeneration. In some institutions disciplines such as geography and environmental management are also included. As such, they comprise something of a microcosm of the university as a whole, comprising business-oriented...

Words: 3310 - Pages: 14

Premium Essay

The Case of Value Based Communication—Epistemological and Methodological Reflections from a System Theoretical Perspective

...FORUM: QUALITATIVE SOCIAL RESEARCH SOZIALFORSCHUNG Volume 11, No. 3, Art. 17 September 2010 The Case of Value Based Communication—Epistemological and Methodological Reflections from a System Theoretical Perspective Victoria von Groddeck Key words: Abstract: The aim of this paper is to reflect the epistemological and methodological aspects of an communication empirical research study which analyzes the phenomenon of increased value communication within theory; form business organizations from a system theoretical perspective in the tradition of Niklas LUHMANN. analysis; Drawing on the theoretical term of observation it shows how a research perspective can be functional developed which opens up the scope for an empirical analysis of communication practices. This analysis; Niklas analysis focuses on the reconstruction of these practices by first understanding how these practices Luhmann; stabilize themselves and second by contrasting different practices to educe an understanding of organization different forms of observation of the relevant phenomenon and of the functions of these forms. studies; George Thus, this approach combines system theoretical epistemology, analytical research strategies, such Spencer-Brown; as form and functional analysis, and qualitative research methods, such as narrative interviews, system theory; participant observation and document analysis. value communication Table of...

Words: 9617 - Pages: 39

Premium Essay

Ethical Considerations in Marketing Research

...Ethical Considerations in Marketing Research There are many ethical considerations taken into account when performing primary research. In specific, the most important factor is the rights of the respondent. The issues concerning respondent’s rights which the marketing researcher should keep in mind while performing his job are as follows (Kimmel & Smith, 2001): 1. The information collected relating to the marketing research from a respondent should not be abused for any other purpose. In order to prevent this, the staff conducting the marketing research must be selected carefully. 2. Harassing or forcing respondents to participate in survey or answering certain questions on the questionnaire is not good professional practice. Instead, the researcher should explain why the particular question is necessary, and leaving it to the respondent to consider if he/she wants to answer them. For instance, questions relating to income of the respondents are always regarded with anxiety, and an explanation may be given before asking the question as to why it is necessary. 3. Researchers should sustain anonymity of respondents. Sustaining anonymity minimises respondent’s anxiety and decreases the fear of losing social status. If a firm breaches anonymity, it will negatively affect the firm’s credibility, reputation and public image. “The respondent is viewed as a loser, while the client pays a toll in terms of higher costs such as possible legal actions and a damaged reputation...

Words: 457 - Pages: 2

Premium Essay

Bussiness

...International Journal of Economics, Management & Accounting 19, no. 1 (2011): 1-26 © 2011 by The International Islamic University Malaysia METHODOLOGY OF ISLAMIC ECONOMICS: OVERVIEW OF PRESENT STATE AND FUTURE DIRECTION* Mohamed Aslam Haneefa and Hafas Furqanib Department of Economics, International Islamic University Malaysia, Jalan Gombak, 53100 Kuala Lumpur, Malaysia. (Email: mdaslam@iiu. edu.my) b Department of Economics, International Islamic University Malaysia, Jalan Gombak, 53100 Kuala Lumpur, Malaysia. (Email: hafasf@gmail.com) a ABSTRACT This paper argues that research and publications in the area of methodology of Islamic economics is very significant for a meaningful development of the discipline. Although the discussion on methodology of Islamic economics in contemporary Islamic economics literature is rather limited, this paper reviews the works of selected scholars who have attempted to present works on ‘methodology’ and their approach to the process of theory building in Islamic economics. The paper then presents some implications of these views based on the position that methodology investigates the criteria, rationalizations, arguments and justifications used in theory appraisal as well as evaluating the reliability of theories, this paper concludes that greater resources, both human and financial, need to be channeled to developing uÎūl al-iqtiÎād, a fundamental, but vastly, neglected area of research in contemporary Islamic economics...

Words: 10008 - Pages: 41

Premium Essay

Wastewater Use in Agriculture: Its Impacts and Methodological Issues in Valuing Impacts

...Wastewater Use in Agriculture: its Impacts and Methodological Issues in Valuing Impacts Introduction With increasing global population, the gap between the supply and demand for water is widening and is reaching such alarming levels that in some parts of the world it is posing a threat to human existence. Scientists around the globe are working on new ways of conserving water. It is an opportune time, to refocus on one of the ways to recycle water—through the reuse of urban wastewater, for irrigation and other purposes. This could release clean water for use in other sectors that need fresh water and provide water to sectors that can utilize wastewater e.g., for irrigation and other ecosystem services. In general, wastewater comprises liquid wastes generated by households, industry, commercial sources, as a result of daily usage, production, and consumption activities. Municipal treatment facilities are designed to treat raw wastewater to produce a liquid effluent of suitable quality that can be disposed to the natural surface waters with minimum impact on human health or the environment. The disposal of wastewater is a major problem faced by municipalities, particularly in the case of large metropolitan areas, with limited space for land-based treatment and disposal. On the other hand, wastewater is also a resource that can be applied for productive uses since wastewater contains nutrients that have the potential for use in agriculture, aquaculture and other activities...

Words: 802 - Pages: 4

Premium Essay

Scsc

...Organizational Behavior/Human Resources Management The OBHR is the study of people, process and outcomes within thoe rganizational behavior and human resources management. Through research, collaboration and dissemination of knowledge, students understand how to impact organizational effectiveness in a variety of different environments, industrie s and across multiple levels of analyses. Our expectation is that students within the OBHR major will craft a program of research that is built upon rigorous theory as well as strong methodological skills that are both necessary for effective scholarship. We encourage collaboration with OBHR faculty that has a proven track record of publishing within a variety of top outlets (Academy of Management Journal, Academy of Management Review, Journal of Organizational Behavior, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, Journal of Labor Research, Harvard Business Review; Human Resource Management; Industrial and Labor Relations Review; Sloan Management Review). Organizational Behavior/Human Resources Management Behavior Systems and Management Thought The objective of this course is to explore the evolution and development of management theory with particular emphasis on the design of behavioral systems in organizations. It is a core premise of the course that the design of systems to manage people in organizations is based on a set of assumptions about humans that are part of the managerial theory that guides the formation and operation...

Words: 1362 - Pages: 6

Premium Essay

Beanos Ice Cream Shoppe

...Journal of Operations Management 29 (2011) 329–342 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Journal of Operations Management journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/jom Qualitative case studies in operations management: Trends, research outcomes, and future research implications Mark Barratt, Thomas Y. Choi ∗ , Mei Li Department of Supply Chain Management, W. P. Carey School of Business, Arizona State University, Tempe, AZ 85287-4706, United States a r t i c l e i n f o a b s t r a c t Our study examines the state of qualitative case studies in operations management. Five main operations management journals are included for their impact on the field. They are in alphabetical order: Decision Sciences, International Journal of Operations and Production Management, Journal of Operations Management, Management Science, and Production and Operations Management. The qualitative case studies chosen were published between 1992 and 2007. With an increasing trend toward using more qualitative case studies, there have been meaningful and significant contributions to the field of operations management, especially in the area of theory building. However, in many of the qualitative case studies we reviewed, sufficient details in research design, data collection, and data analysis were missing. For instance, there are studies that do not offer sampling logic or a description of the analysis through which research outcomes are drawn. Further, research protocols for doing inductive...

Words: 13548 - Pages: 55

Premium Essay

Organizational Behavior

...Organizational Behavior W. Jack Duncan Book review Meera Iyer Chapters 1. Organizational behavior: defining the field 2. Historical perspective 3. Methodological foundations of organizational behavior 4. Personality development and attitudes 5. The cognitive basis of individual behavior 6. Motivation: Theory and selected research 7. Introduction to small group behavior 8. Leadership behavior and effectiveness 9. Intergroup analysis: Co-ordination and conflict 10. The organization and the individual 11. Environments, organizations and behavior 12. Power relations in organizations 13. Performance evaluation and organizational effectiveness 14. Planned change and organizational development This book is about organizational behavior. It is also a text on management. The objective of the book is to present a research based approach to management from an applied behavioral science perspective. I have summarized each chapter of the book, explaining the main points that the authors wish to communicate. © www.hrfolks.com All Rights Reserved Organizational behavior – Defining the field Organizations are collections of interacting and inter related human and non-human resources working toward a common goal or set of goals within the framework of structured relationships. Organizational behavior is concerned with all aspects of how organizations influence the behavior of individuals and how individuals...

Words: 4349 - Pages: 18

Premium Essay

Phd Hbo

...PhD Program in Organizational Behavior and Human Resources Management University of Pittsburgh's Katz Graduate School Organizational Behavior PhD Program Video The OBHR doctoral program focuses on preparing students to impact the study of people, process and outcomes within the fields of organizational behavior and human resources management. Through research, collaboration and dissemination of knowledge, students understand how to impact organizational effectiveness in a variety of different environments, industries and across multiple levels of analyses. Our expectation is that students within the OBHR major will craft a program of research that is built upon rigorous theory as well as strong methodological skills that are both necessary for effective scholarship. We encourage collaboration with OBHR faculty that has a proven track record of publishing within a variety of top outlets (Academy of Management Journal, Academy of Management Review, Journal of Organizational Behavior, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, Journal of Labor Research, Harvard Business Review; Human Resource Management; Industrial and Labor Relations Review; Sloan Management Review). Organizational Behavior/Human Resources Management PhD Program Curriculum Behavior Systems and Management Thought The objective of this course is to explore the evolution and development of management theory with particular emphasis on the design of behavioral systems in organizations. It is a core premise...

Words: 1422 - Pages: 6

Free Essay

Fdi and Asda

...MSc Management with Streams Strategy Assessment Brief Due: 4pm, 29 April 2014 Page 1 of 8 1. Background Information The purpose of this assignment is to give you an opportunity to present your understanding of the concepts in strategy, to identify and appreciate the challenges strategists face in real-life situations and to apply different frameworks and tools in order to analyse the strategic positions of a firm in an industry or market. A direct outcome of this last part is to demonstrate that you can diagnose any challenges or gaps that this firm can pursue or fill and, therefore, you can recommend alternative strategies. Instructions: 1. Pick a company you find interesting in any industry, market or country you like. Make sure that you choose a firm that meets the following criteria: a. It is not very small (10+ employees) b. It generates revenue of a few hundreds thousands pounds or equivalent (£100,000+ revenue) c. you can find enough information about it The last criterion is very important as your assignment is heavily based on the information you can collect and the level of accessibility you can get for this information 2. Read as much material as you can find about this firm. Try to understand the business, the strategies it follows and the challenges it faces in that industry 3. Then, proceed with the assignment questions 2. Assignment Questions You are a management consultant and you have been asked by the Board of Directors of your employer to prepare...

Words: 2133 - Pages: 9